Because the primary difference between patients and controls extracted from the group image analysis had to do with decreased activation in the primary visual areas and increased activation in parietal and prefrontal for patients compared with controls, the voxel with the peak signal change was extracted within the left and right inferior parietal lobules, prefrontal cortex, and primary visual cortex for each subject. Bivariate 2-tailed Pearson correlations were used to examine the relationship between peak activation and performance on the 3Let and 6Let tasks for each group of subjects (younger controls, older controls, patients with AMD).
There were no significant correlations (
P < 0.05) for younger or older controls with regard to their behavioral performance on the 3Let task. In contrast, for the patients with AMD, there was a significant positive correlation between activation in the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in the 3Let task with accuracy of response (
r = 0.875,
P = 0.024) and significant negative correlations with latencies for correct (
r = −0.861,
P = 0.028) and incorrect (
r = −0.886,
P = 0.019) responses. Similar relationships were found for the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex with accuracy (
r = 0.848,
P = 0.033) and correct (
r = −0.842,
P = 0.036) and incorrect (
r = −0.845,
P = 0.034) reaction times. Data underlying the correlations between dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) activity and behavioral data in patients with AMD are presented in
Figure 5 . Although there was a trend for a similar relationship between accuracy and activation in the inferior parietal lobules in patients with AMD, it did not reach significance for the left or right inferior parietal lobule (left:
r = −0.725,
P = 0.103; right:
r = −0.793,
P = 0.060). There was, however, a significant negative correlation between response latency to correct and incorrect trials with peak activation in left and right inferior parietal lobules (left inferior parietal lobule, correct:
r = −0.839,
P = 0.037; left inferior parietal lobule, incorrect:
r = −0.908,
P = 0.012; right inferior parietal lobule, correct:
r = −0.818,
P = 0.047; right inferior parietal lobule, incorrect:
r = −0.898,
P = 0.015). In the patients with AMD, no relationship was observed between peak activation in visual cortices and performance.
As in the 3Let task, there were no significant correlations (P < 0.05) for the younger controls between peak activation and behavioral performance on the 6Let task. For patients with AMD, there were significant negative correlations between reaction times on incorrect trials with peak activation in the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (r = −0.950, P = 0.004), left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (r = −.903, P = 0.014), and right inferior parietal lobule (r = −0.878, P = 0.021) for the group of older controls. Similarly, patients with AMD showed significant positive correlations between accuracy of response and peak activation in the left and the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (left: r = 0.933, P = 0.007; right: r = 0.828, P = 0.042). Patients with AMD also demonstrated significant negative correlations between peak activation in the left and right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex with speed of response for correct and incorrect trials (left DLPFC, correct: r = −0.915, P = 0.011; left DLPFC, incorrect: r = −0.959, P = 0.002; right DLPFC, correct: r = −0.837, P = 0.038; right DLPFC, incorrect: r = −0.840, P = 0.036). There was also a significant relationship for patients with AMD relative to peak activation in the left inferior parietal lobule for correct responses (r = 0.841, P = 0.036) and a trend with incorrect reaction time (r = −0.782, P = 0.066). As with the 3Let task, there were no significant correlations between peak activation in visual cortices and behavioral performance.