For nonblurred sentences, the saccade disconjugacy amounted to −6.1 ± 6.0 min arc, on average. This amount of saccade disconjugacy equates to approximately 6.5% of the corresponding average saccade amplitude and thus reflects a good yoking of the binocular saccade as it would be expected for young adults.
6,7 Moreover, this saccade disconjugacy was not affected by reading the blurred text (
b = −0.03, SE = 0.01,
t = −0.23,
P = 0.82). The average disconjugacy for blurred sentences was −5.5 ± 4.6 min arc and resembled approximately 6.2% of the corresponding average saccade amplitude. The disconjugacy did not change because of the actual fixation position within the sentence (
b = 0.001, SE = 0.001,
t = 0.02,
P = 0.99) or the repetition of the measurements throughout the presentation blocks (
b = 0.02, SE = 0.04,
t = 0.65,
P = 0.52). The only variable during reading that affected the saccade disconjugacy was the amplitude of the incoming saccade (
b = 0.057, SE = 0.002,
t = 22.94,
P < 0.01), as one would expect from previous reports, larger saccades increased the observed saccade disconjugacy.
Analysis of the vergence adjustment during fixation revealed a small, albeit significant, difference between both text conditions (
b = −0.31, SE = 0.13,
t = −2.34,
P = 0.02). For nonblurred sentences the adjustment amounted to 5.5 ± 5.4 min arc and was convergent in its direction. It counterbalanced 89.7% of disconjugacy induced by the saccade. For blurred sentences the same convergent adjustment amounted to 4.7 ± 4.3 min arc, counterbalancing approximately 85.7% of the disconjugacy induced by the saccade. Further, the incoming saccade amplitude affected the vergence adjustment during fixation (
b = 0.031, SE = 0.002,
t = 13.77,
P < 0.01), reflecting that the larger the incoming saccade, the larger the vergence adjustment during fixation. In contrast, the fixation position (
b = −0.001, SE = 0.001,
t = −0.59,
P = 0.55) and the number of presentation blocks (
b = 0.039, SE = 0.037,
t = 1.06,
P = 0.29) had no effect on the vergence adjustment during fixation. For both sets of sentences, nonblurred and blurred, the saccade disconjugacy correlated with the vergence adjustment during the beginning of the fixation (
r = −0.99;
P < 0.01, and
r = −0.82;
P < 0.01), respectively) and thus reflected the stereotyped eye movement pattern of a divergence during saccades, which is followed by a convergent drift at the beginning of fixations.
9,21,51
The next analysis regarded the overall adjustment of the vergence angle for both reading conditions. For nonblurred and blurred sentences, we extracted the vergence angle at the moment during fixation when the deviation of this angle was smallest with respect to the geometrically expected vergence angle. Across subjects, the average vergence angle for nonblurred sentences was 364 ± 31 min arc. Since the geometrically expected vergence angle for a 60-cm viewing distance and an average pupil distance of 59 mm was 341 ± 20 min arc, averaged across participants, the mean minimum fixation disparity was 16 ± 21 min arc and therefore slightly smaller than the average character width (20 min arc). Most interesting, the observed vergence angle was smaller for blurred text. The vergence angle shifted in exo direction relative to the vergence angle during nonblurred sentence presentation, amounting to 357 ± 32 min arc, on average; this corresponding change of a few minutes of arc was significant (
b = −6.52, SE = 0.69,
t = −9.42,
P < 0.01).
Figure 3 shows the vergence angles for blurred sentences as a function of vergence angles for nonblurred sentences. As can be seen, both observations correlated highly (
r = −0.99;
P < 0.01), and all participants showed a smaller vergence angle with blurred text presentations.
The sequence of presentation did not change the vergence angle (b = −0.004, SE = 0.196, t = −0.02, P = 0.98). Surprisingly, neither the incoming saccade amplitude (b = 0.007, SE = 0.011, t = 0.65, P = 0.52) nor the actual fixation position (b = 0.004, SE = 0.003, t = 1.22, P = 0.22) showed an effect on the vergence angle in this first analysis. We speculated that an interaction between the text condition and the incoming saccade amplitude or the fixation position might have obscured any single effect. For this reason, we ran a new mixed-effects model analysis, but this time we included both interaction effects. The effect of text condition even increased slightly and both interactions (text condition × fixation position: b = −0.16, SE = 0.02, t = −7.59, P < 0.01; text condition × saccade amplitude: b = 0.04, SE = 0.02, t = 1.96, P = 0.05) were significant. Reducing the mixed-effects model again by analyzing only one level of the variable text condition showed that, for nonblurred text conditions, there was a slight tendency toward an increasing effect of the actual fixation position on vergence angle adjustments (b = 0.006, SE = 0.004, t = 1.86, P = 0.06), whereas, for the blurred text condition the fixation position obviously decreased the amount of adjusted vergence angle (b = −0.17, SE = 0.02, t = −6.72, P < 0.01). For incoming saccade amplitude, the effects disappeared again (nonblurred text: b = 0.003, SE = 0.014, t = 0.18, P = 0.86; blurred text: b = −0.001, SE = 0.002, t = −0.03, P = 0.97).