Abstract
Purpose.:
Chicks emmetropize accurately to experimentally induced myopic and hyperopic defocus. The authors investigated the emmetropization response when a specific proportion of the retina was exposed to myopic defocus while the remainder was exposed to (competing) hyperopic defocus.
Methods.:
Normal chicks (14–15 days old) were fitted monocularly with a “lens-cone” device that exposed a specific proportion of the available visual field to a high-contrast grating under 10 diopters (D) of myopic defocus (with accommodation relaxed) in a series of patches. The remainder of the visual field (adjacent patches) viewed a grating under 10 D of hyperopic defocus. Groups of chicks wore a lens-cone device designed to provide a “spatial ratio” (relative proportion of visual field area) of 100:0, 50:50, 40:60, 33:67, 25:75, or 0:100 myopic versus hyperopic defocus. On-axis ocular refraction and axial ocular component dimensions were assessed after 3 and 6 days of cone wear.
Results.:
Interocular differences in refraction (mean ± SD) at day 6 were as follows: +10.4 ± 2.5 D, +7.6 ± 3.6 D, +5.9 ± 3.7 D, +1.6 ± 2.6 D, −2.4 ± 2.7 D, and −8.9 ± 2.6 D for spatial ratios of 100:0, 50:50, 40:60. 33:67, 25:75, and 0:100 respectively. The corresponding interocular vitreous chamber depths were as follows: −515 ± 135 μm, −447 ± 137 μm, −253 ± 220 μm, −105 ± 252 μm, 230 ± 218 μm, and 592 ± 161 μm. The refraction and biometry results for the 33:67 and 25:75 groups were significantly different from those of the single defocus control groups.
Conclusions.:
In chicks, the on-axis emmetropization response was weighted according to the spatial ratio. Thus, as the proportion of retinal area receiving myopic defocus increased relative to that receiving hyperopic defocus, the degree of myopic eye growth was reduced.
To achieve good vision, the component parts of a growing eye must be carefully coordinated such that the eye's axial length is appropriately tuned to its focal length. During an animal's postnatal maturation, the eyes tend to become relatively free from the low levels of axial ametropia that are common at birth, a phenomenon known as emmetropization. (This process explains why ametropia is relatively rare in mature wild and domesticated animals, such as pigeon,
1 chick,
2 tree shrew,
3 rhesus monkey,
4 fish,
5 marmoset,
6 and guinea pig
7 ). Studies in animal models, in which innate refractive errors can be simulated by rearing an animal with a lens in front of one or both eyes, have demonstrated that emmetropization is partially a visually guided process. Emmetropization has also been observed in longitudinal studies in humans, with the eyes of infants gradually becoming less hyperopic during the first few years of life.
8
In recent years, there has been a dramatic increase in the prevalence of myopia in schoolchildren and young adults in a number of East Asian countries.
9 –13 Thus, in these populations, the process of emmetropization either fails for some reason or is overridden as children become older. Because emmetropization is in part a visually guided process, it is plausible that a better understanding of how visual images are “processed” to appropriately modulate the rate of vitreous chamber elongation will produce insights into the pathogenesis of human myopia.
Historically, most experiments designed to study the effects of imposing myopic defocus (with positive lenses) or hyperopic defocus (with negative lenses) in animal models have concentrated on the relationship between central (on-axis) vision and emmetropization, with little regard to peripheral vision. Although this work has advanced our knowledge, the issue of the complexity of the visual environment has rarely been addressed. More recently, however, interest has grown in considering how our rich visual environment, which presents objects at different positions in the visual field and with multiple levels of defocus, is integrated by (and across) the retina to guide eye growth during emmetropization. One important characteristic of emmetropization is that the detection of an area of defocus, and the consequent structural changes in the eye's dimensions, can be confined to a localized region. Specifically, it has been shown that when a large region of the retina is selectively deprived of form vision or is exposed to lens-induced defocus, differential growth of the posterior eye is restricted to the region concerned in chicks
14 –16 and tree shrews
17 (Siegwart JT, et al.
IOVS 1993;34:ARVO Abstract 1208). Thus, instead of global compensatory growth, it appears that particular regions of the retina are able to decode visual cues and initiate compensatory regional eye growth at those sites. More recently, experiments in monkeys have shown that the macula does not dominate emmetropization, as previously assumed.
18 Instead, the peripheral retina has been found to play a significant role in emmetropization when the macula is ablated.
19 Attempts in chicks to influence central refractive status by imposing peripheral defocus have had mixed success
20,21 (Morgan I, et al.
IOVS 2006;47: E-Abstract 3328). Although the discrepant findings from these chick studies may in part be attributable to differences in chick strain, treatment age, or experimental design, they may also be indicative of a more complex relationship between axial refractive error and peripheral defocus than has previously been assumed.
A pioneering study by Wildsoet and Schmidt
22 used a lens-cone system with single, semitransparent target plane to try to unravel how visual emmetropization cues are decoded. In a later study, Diether and Wildsoet
23 used a cone system with two target planes to more precisely control the magnitudes of hyperopic and myopic defocus experienced by chicks (and to investigate the role of target contrast). They found that emmetropization was dominated by myopic defocus in this competing defocus setting (in which Maltese cross-targets produced a 50:50 spatial ratio of competing defocus). Furthermore, the regions of competing defocus appeared to influence emmetropization in an all-or-nothing manner under these conditions.
However, because visual scenes are typically composed of multiple objects at varying distances, they expose the retina to multiple levels of defocus (see Fig. 7 in Ref.
24). Thus, a “map” of retinal defocus should include several parameters: sign of the defocus, magnitude of the defocus, spatial area subtended by the region of defocus, and eccentricity of the defocus. Among these parameters, the sign and magnitude of defocus have been extensively studied in the literature, whereas the spatial area of defocus (more correctly, the solid angle) and its eccentricity have received less attention. In a previous study,
24 we showed that the eye can integrate simultaneously presented competing defocus signals (introduced by concentric-design bifocal lenses, such that the defocused images lay on top of one another). As mentioned, the additional ability of the eye to respond to local regions of defocus
14,15 demonstrates that the retina can also decode defocus in a spatially restricted manner. Together these findings prompted us to explore an alternative mode of competing defocus in which defocused images were presented to the eye as discrete, regionally distinct patches in such a way that adjacent retinal regions received defocus stimuli of opposing signs. Here we evaluated various spatial ratios of laterally separated competing defocus stimuli in this mode to test how the eye integrates this information and determines its emmetropization response and, crucially, its resultant on-axis refractive error.
Refractive and biometric data were collected before the application of the lens-cone system and at day 3 and day 6 of the experiment. Retinoscopy was performed immediately after A-scan biometry so as to allow measurement along the same axis and thus reduce alignment errors. Details of the retinoscopy and ultrasound measurements have been reported previously.
24 Animals were excluded from the experiment if the lens-cone system became detached at any time, the device was decentered so that the pupil was not aligned with the optic zone of the lens, a chick was unable to open the eye behind the device on two occasions, or the weight of a chick did not show evidence of normal growth.
The dual plane lens-cone device was applied unilaterally. The fellow eye acted as a control. Animals were divided into seven groups. Six spatial area ratios of competing defocus (myopic/hyperopic) were tested: 100:0, 50:50, 40:60, 33:67, 25:75, 0:100. An additional control group was designated to study the effect of cone wear alone because this would produce peripheral form deprivation. Chicks in this group wore a “blank cone” with no visual targets and with no lens. Animals were checked daily to rectify dislocation or decentration of the cone. Lenses were cleaned every 1 to 2 days. The lighting level ranged from 800 to 1100 lux, as measured at the level of food containers. There were 15 chicks per group initially. This was reduced to a minimum of eight over the course of the experiment due to the exclusion of chicks because of cone dislocation or displacement.
All data are presented as the mean ± SD. One-way ANOVA was used to examine the effect of area ratio of defocus; Bonferroni post hoc tests were used to determine the significance of differences between treatment groups.
Graded emmetropization responses similar to those described were observed in the middle of the treatment period. However, emmetropization was incomplete for both the single defocus groups at day 3 (interocular RE = +6.0 D and −5.5 D for the 100:0 and 0:100 groups, respectively). In spatial ratio groups 25:75 and 33:67, chicks showed refractive and biometric changes at day 3 similar to those seen at day 6 (change in RE = +1.4 D and −2.6 D, respectively; change in VCD = 0.204 mm and −0.035 mm, respectively). For the most part, these changes were already significantly different from those observed in the single defocus control groups. Thus, in contrast to the single defocus groups, emmetropization in the 25:75 and 33:67 spatial ratio groups appeared to be nearly stable between day 3 and day 6, suggesting that these eyes had already reached an equilibrium state at this level of competing defocus.
It is still a mystery how the retina can discern the signs and magnitude of defocus. Further work is needed to unravel the neurobiology and biochemistry of this feat. Determining the smallest patch of defocus that the retina can recognize may shed light on the decoding mechanism. If some specific retinal neuron subtypes are critically involved in decoding defocus or any integrative processing, their receptive fields, densities, and distributions in the retina may be important in determining regional differences in sensitivity to myopic or hyperopic defocus. Furthermore, if the density of “emmetropization-responsive” cells is higher in a particular region of retina, this region may contribute more weight per unit area to determining the refractive status of the wider local area. Another possible approach to investigating this multidimensional problem would be to determine the area of the retina, centered on the macula, required to override the peripheral retina. Determining the relative balance of these regions may be important in describing the differential regional sensitivity to defocus across the retina.
In conclusion, this study provides evidence that adjacent patches of myopic and hyperopic defocus are integrated quantitatively either at the level of the retina, choroid, or sclera, to guide emmetropisatory eye growth. Our findings highlight the importance of the spatial properties of defocus in visual scenes in guiding ocular growth. Further work is needed to determine other factors that contribute to the spatial distribution of defocus and to determine how these relate to variables such as posture, size of reading materials, distribution of background objects, partial spectacle correction, and so on, that may conspire to affect the development of myopia in children.