June 2011
Volume 52, Issue 7
Free
Visual Psychophysics and Physiological Optics  |   June 2011
Effects of Aging on Visual Contour Integration and Segmentation
Author Affiliations & Notes
  • Clara Casco
    From the Department of General Psychology, University of Padua, Padua, Italy; and
  • Valentina Robol
    From the Department of General Psychology, University of Padua, Padua, Italy; and
  • Michele Barollo
    From the Department of General Psychology, University of Padua, Padua, Italy; and
  • Selene Cansino
    the Laboratory of NeuroCognition, Faculty of Psychology, National Autonomous University of Mexico, Mexico City, Mexico.
  • Corresponding author: Valentina Robol, Department of General Psychology, University of Padua, via Venezia 15 35131 Padua, Italy; valentina.robol@unipd.it
Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science June 2011, Vol.52, 3955-3961. doi:https://doi.org/10.1167/iovs.10-5439
  • Views
  • PDF
  • Share
  • Tools
    • Alerts
      ×
      This feature is available to authenticated users only.
      Sign In or Create an Account ×
    • Get Citation

      Clara Casco, Valentina Robol, Michele Barollo, Selene Cansino; Effects of Aging on Visual Contour Integration and Segmentation. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 2011;52(7):3955-3961. https://doi.org/10.1167/iovs.10-5439.

      Download citation file:


      © ARVO (1962-2015); The Authors (2016-present)

      ×
  • Supplements
Abstract

Purpose.: Perception of circular disconnected contours requires the integration of relevant local orientation information across space and the suppression of irrelevant orientations. Using a detection of deviation from circularity (DFC) task, the present study examined whether the efficiency of either integrative or suppressive visual mechanisms, or both, declines with age.

Methods.: Younger and older observers' sensitivities in detecting the DFC of a contour formed by Gabors were compared in three conditions: when all elements were oriented tangentially to the contour, with and without the presence of randomly oriented background noise; and when they had alternated tangential and orthogonal orientations, without background noise.

Results.: In agreement with previous studies, the authors found that younger observers were not impaired in the mixed condition with respect to the tangential condition, suggesting the involvement of a high-level mechanism responding to the global closure information provided by tangential local orientations, even if they are interspersed with orthogonal ones. Instead, older observers were specifically impaired in the mixed condition, suggesting a reduced capability of suppressing nontangential information along the contour, and were also less efficient in suppressing irrelevant orientations in the background.

Conclusions.: These results support the suggestion that, whereas integrative mechanisms are not affected by age, suppressive mechanisms are.

Human visual functions degrade with age. Although there are age-related degenerations of the optics of the eye, 1,2 those optical changes are insufficient to explain the decline of both low-level visual abilities (acuity, 3 contrast sensitivity for spatial or chromatic patterns, 4 6 and orientation discrimination 7,8 ) and more complex visual functions (motion perception, 9 12 bilateral symmetry perception 13 and spatial integration and segregation 14 16 ). Here we investigated the complex visual functions that involve deriving a meaningful percept from fragmented visual information in the retinal image. 
In particular, these experiments investigated whether aging affects the integration of local fragments into contours and the segmentation of contours from the background. 
The ability of the visual system to reconstruct contours from a fragmented retinal image has been investigated extensively in young adults by the use of contours formed by oriented disconnected elements. Numerous studies have examined the detection of linear, 17 20 curvilinear, 21,22 or closed contours 23 26 embedded in a cluttered background of elements of different orientation from those forming the contour. 27,28 Those studies highlighted the spatial parameters that reduce contour integration and segmentation, the most powerful being orientation jitter of nearby contour segments that renders them not aligned to the contour path and relative distance between contour segments. 27 In particular, many studies have shown better detectability of contours composed of elements aligned along the global contour orientation, both in the absence 29,30 and in the presence of background noise. 18,20  
Comparison of the ability to detect a fragmented contour with and without noise is important because it highlights the combined action of the two mechanisms involved: one facilitatory and the other suppressive. The facilitatory mechanism mediates the integration of oriented contour segments, whose linking, according to the association field model, is strongest when they are aligned along their axis of preferred orientation. 21,31 33 The suppressive mechanism mediates a reduction in the response to the noisy background that may interfere with the integration process. Whereas facilitation probably relies on long-range excitatory horizontal connections between cells in the primary visual cortex (V1), 34,35 background suppression is more likely to result from short-range inhibitory connections. 36  
Despite the large number of previous studies on contour integration and segmentation, little is known about how aging affects the facilitatory and suppressive mechanisms involved in those tasks. That aging may affect facilitation and suppression is suggested indirectly by neurophysiological studies in cats and monkeys. Those studies show two age-related deficits that may affect the integrative and suppressive mechanisms: (1) decreased selectivity to orientation in senescent V1 neurons caused by reduced lateral inhibition and (2) increased spontaneous activity. 37,38 Both deficits may result from reduced γ-aminobutyric acid (GABA)–mediated inhibition. 39 Human studies provide no evidence of reduced orientation selectivity, 8,40 whereas there is an age-dependent increase in equivalent input noise or internal noise that may be related to increased spontaneous activity. 7  
If aging reduces the efficiency of neural inhibition, this could affect not only the response of individual channels but also the efficiency of lateral interactions between channels accounting for contour integration and segmentation. Two recent studies 15,41 examined the effect of aging on contour integration and segmentation within noisy backgrounds, but with contradictory results. One study 41 did not find evidence for a deficit in segmentation. Indeed, the results showed that the detrimental effect of adding the noise was the same for older and younger observers. The other study 15 showed reduced ability of older observers to detect closed circular contours embedded in noisy backgrounds, and the effect was greater for small rather than that for large inter-element distances. The results are contradictory perhaps because the different tasks used (shape discrimination 41 vs. contour detection 15 ) affect different levels of processing involved in visual integration and segmentation, spanning from contextual influences in V1 to top-down influences such as attention and task demands. 19,20  
To interpret the contour-detection results of Del Viva and Agostini, 15 two further questions have to be answered. The first regards the relative contribution of facilitatory and inhibitory lateral interactions 20 in accounting for reduced sensitivity to circular contours. The paradigm used by Del Viva and Agostini 15 does not allow this distinction; the reduced ability of older observers to detect closed circular contours embedded in a noisy background may be attributable to a reduced capability to suppress noise, particularly when integration signals are weak. Alternatively, and regardless of the presence of noise, older individuals may be less efficient in integrating elements belonging to the contour. 
The second question arises because Del Viva and Agostini 15 manipulated only inter-element distance. As a consequence, it is unclear whether there is an aging effect on contour integration that depends on the relative orientation of the elements defining the circular contour in addition to their distance. One previous study 16 examined the effect of relative local orientations on contour integration across different age groups. The results showed that the contrast threshold for detecting and discriminating the global orientation of a C-shaped contour against a blank background depended on the orientation of the local elements for younger but not for older observers. However, this result cannot be generalized to suprathreshold stimuli since it is well established that facilitation by alignment in contrast detection is a low-level, monocular phenomenon, 42 and its role in higher-level tasks such as detection of a smoothly curved suprathreshold path has often been questioned. 43,44 That is, the contrast-detection paradigm used by Roudaia et al. 16 may have pinpointed age differences in local low-level facilitatory mechanisms of contrast enhancement instead of, or in addition to, age differences in the global long-range facilitation involved in suprathreshold circular contour integration. 45  
To summarize, the question of whether aging affects the dependence of contour-integration mechanisms on the relative orientation of elements along a contour and in the background remains open. Such dependence could be accounted for by reduced orientation selectivity highlighted by primate studies. 37,38 In that case, the effect of aging on contour integration and segmentation would be general. Indeed, reduced orientation selectivity should reduce both the facilitatory interactions that mediate contour integration and the inhibitory interactions that mediate suppression of nontangential elements along the contour and in the background. Also, nonsensorial factors, if affected by age, should have a general effect on contour integration and segmentation. However, the effect of aging could be specific for either integration or segregation (i.e., selective for one type of low-level cortical lateral interactions). 
To compare the efficiency of integrative and segregative operations in younger and older adults we measured detectability of deviation from circularity (DFC) in the shape of suprathreshold circular contours, 46,47 defined by oriented Gabor elements, sinusoidal gratings (carriers) seen through a Gaussian window. We compared a condition where Gabors were aligned along the contour with a condition where Gabors had alternating tangential and orthogonal orientations to establish how aging affects the capacity to suppress irrelevant orientation information along the contour. Moreover, by comparing performances with and without background noise we intended specifically to establish whether the capacity to suppress random orientations not belonging to the contour is reduced by aging. 
Contrast sensitivity was also measured to confirm that the contrast of the carrier was above threshold for both age groups. Indeed, there is evidence of an age-related loss in sensitivity at high and middle spatial frequencies in photopic vision, 48 whereas only in scotopic vision does an age-related decline occur for spatial frequencies < 1.2 cycles/deg, consistently with age-related changes in the magnocellular pathway. 49  
Methods
This research adhered to the tenets of the Declaration of Helsinki and was approved by the bioethics committee of the Psychology Faculty of the University of Padua. Informed consent was obtained from all participants. 
Stimuli
In the spatial integration and suppression experiment the target stimuli were composed of cosine-phase Gabor patches. The SD of the two-dimensional Gaussian envelope was subtended 0.16° of visual angle and the sinusoidal grating had a wavelength λ of 0.32° of visual angle (spatial frequency = 3.13 cycles/deg). Stimuli were achromatic with a Michelson contrast of 0.87 and presented on a background with mean luminance of 38.9 cd/m2. We used high-contrast Gabors to ensure that the lower sensitivity that older observers have for carriers of this spatial frequency 48 could not be the cause of group differences in integration and segmentation. 
The circular contour was created by placing eight equally spaced Gabors (center-to-center distance = 74.4 arcmin or 3.9λ) along an imaginary circle (radius = 97.2 arcmin) centered on the screen. One of these Gabors was positioned on an imaginary circle of larger radius that varied randomly in five levels: 98.7, 103.1, 107.5, 112.0, and 116.4 arcmin. Thus, five DFC levels were obtained: 1.5, 5.9, 10.3, 14.8, and 19.2 arcmin. The displaced Gabor was chosen randomly with equal probability on every trial among four different locations (0°, 90°, 180°, and 270°). These DFC levels were selected to allow the psychometric function fit. 
Three different stimulus conditions were tested in three separate sessions (Fig. 1). In the “tangential” condition, all Gabors were tangential to the contour; in the “mixed” condition, Gabors had alternating tangential and orthogonal orientations, and the displaced Gabor was always tangential. The “noise” condition was the same as the “tangential” but with background noise consisting of randomly oriented, equally spaced Gabors. These Gabors were placed along two imaginary concentric circles centered on the screen. One had 4 Gabors and a radius of 41.3 arcmin; the other had 12 Gabors and a radius of 153.2 arcmin. 
Figure 1.
 
The circular stimuli used. (ac) Contours without DFC. (df) Contours with DFC (here only a DFC of 14.8 arcmin and only for the Gabor on the right is shown). (a) and (d) show “tangential” stimulus conditions, (b) and (e) “mixed” conditions, and (c) and (f) “noise” conditions.
Figure 1.
 
The circular stimuli used. (ac) Contours without DFC. (df) Contours with DFC (here only a DFC of 14.8 arcmin and only for the Gabor on the right is shown). (a) and (d) show “tangential” stimulus conditions, (b) and (e) “mixed” conditions, and (c) and (f) “noise” conditions.
The stimuli for the contrast sensitivity measurement consisted of full-screen vertical sinusoidal gratings. Eight spatial frequencies (0.10, 0.19, 0.42, 0.90, 1.99, 4.41, 9.91, and 19.82 cycles/deg) were tested. 
Apparatus
The stimuli for the spatial integration and suppression experiment were generated with a high-level interactive technical computing language (MATLAB 7.3.0.267 [R2006b], Mathworks; Natick, MA) and presented on a 17-in. cathode ray tube (CRT) monitor (P70f ViewSonic [Walnut, CA]; refresh rate, 100 Hz; resolution, 1024 × 768 pixels). A computer (Pentium 4; Intel, Santa Clara, CA) was used to generate and present the stimuli. Experiment control and collection of behavioral responses were undertaken using a software application suite (E-Prime, version 1.2; Psychology Software Tools, Inc., Sharpsburg, PA). Contrast sensitivity was measured using a software application tool (CRS Psycho 2.36; Cambridge Research Systems Ltd, Rochester, UK). The stimuli were generated by a graphics card (Cambridge Research Systems Ltd VSG2/3) and displayed on a 17-in. CRT monitor (Brilliance 107P; Philips [Amsterdam, The Netherlands] refresh rate, 70 Hz; resolution, 1024 × 768 pixels). 
Procedure and Design
For all measurements (spatial integration and suppression and contrast sensitivity) stimuli were viewed binocularly in a darkened room at a viewing distance of 70 cm. 
In each trial of the spatial integration and suppression experiment, a fixation cross presented for 200 ms was followed, after 300 ms, by two stimuli that were presented for 400 ms each and with an interstimulus interval of 600 ms. We used a two-interval, two-alternative forced choice (2I-2AFC) detection task in which observers had to choose, by pressing one of two alternative keys on the computer keyboard, which presentation contained a DFC. The contour with DFC was presented, with equal probability, either in the first or in the second stimulus interval. The other stimulus displayed a circular contour shape. Note that although the exposure time was relatively long, the psychophysical method used allowed comparisons, across stimulus conditions and groups, of the psychometric functions describing changes in detection over a range of DFC levels. 
Each session consisted of 80 randomly presented trials, resulting from eight repetitions of each of the DFC levels (1.5, 5.9, 10.3, 14.8, and 19.2 arcmin) and presentation order (contour with DFC either in the first or in the second stimulus interval). The experiment (within-subjects design, three sessions with counterbalanced order) lasted approximately 2 hours, including resting intervals. 
Displacement thresholds, defined as the DFC level that corresponds to the 0.75 correct detection probability (DFC thresholds), were calculated for each subject by fitting a psychometric function with the Probit analysis. 50 Dependence of DFC thresholds on stimulus type (tangential, mixed, and noise) and group was tested with two-way repeated-measures ANOVAs. Degrees of freedom (df) were corrected with the Greenhouse–Geisser procedure and corrected probability levels are reported. Post hoc pairwise comparisons were computed with Bonferroni correction. The α level was set at 0.05 for all statistical tests. 
Contrast sensitivity was measured after the third experimental session. In each trial, a full-screen vertical sinusoidal grating was presented and the subjects' task was to indicate whether they could detect it. We used the method of limits with three ascending (from lower to higher grating contrast) and three descending (from higher to lower grating contrast) series. For each subject contrast sensitivity at each spatial frequency tested was calculated by averaging across series. 
Subjects
Subjects tested on spatial integration and suppression belonged to two groups: the younger group was composed of 14 observers (mean age, 24.8 ± 3.4 years; range, 19–33 years), and the older group comprised 14 observers (mean age, 66.9 ± 6.3 years; range, 60–78 years). All participants had normal or corrected to normal vision such that binocular visual acuity was ≤ +0.10 logMAR at a distance of 70 cm (younger mean visual acuity [logMAR]: −0.11 ± 0.07; older mean visual acuity [logMAR]: +0.00 ± 0.09). Older observers did not have eye defects (such as cataract and glaucoma) or neurologic deficits (such as Alzheimer's disease or other forms of age-associated dementia). Both groups had similar socioeconomic status and educational background. 
In nine younger (mean age, 24.8 ± 3.6 years; range, 20–33 years) and eight older (mean age, 65.9 ± 7.5 years; range, 60–78 years) observers contrast sensitivity was also measured in addition to visual acuity (younger mean visual acuity [logMAR]: −0.11 ± 0.08; older mean visual acuity [logMAR]: +0.02 ± 0.10). 
For all measurements (spatial integration and suppression and contrast sensitivity) subjects wore their glasses or contact lenses. 
Results
Contrast Sensitivity
In agreement with previous findings, 48 we found that at the spatial frequency of the carrier (3.13 cycles/deg), sensitivity was lower (Fig. 2) for older than that for younger observers. However, since the contrast of the Gabors was very high, the low sensitivity to the carrier could not be the cause of group differences in integration and segmentation, even considering that a grating viewed through a Gaussian window produces a sensitivity reduction of approximately 0.5 log units. 51  
Figure 2.
 
Mean binocular contrast sensitivity functions of younger (continuous line) and older observers (dotted line).
Figure 2.
 
Mean binocular contrast sensitivity functions of younger (continuous line) and older observers (dotted line).
Spatial Integration
Figure 3 shows psychometric functions obtained in the tangential and mixed conditions by younger and older observers. The two-way ANOVA on threshold values, with group and condition (tangential versus mixed) as factors, showed a group effect [F (1,26) = 16.21, P < 0.001, ηp 2 = 0.384], indicating that DFC thresholds are higher in the older group. 
Figure 3.
 
Psychometric functions for the (a) younger and (b) older groups in the tangential (TAN) and mixed (MIX) conditions, obtained by fitting observed mean detection probabilities (TAN_obs, MIX_obs) for each DFC level.
Figure 3.
 
Psychometric functions for the (a) younger and (b) older groups in the tangential (TAN) and mixed (MIX) conditions, obtained by fitting observed mean detection probabilities (TAN_obs, MIX_obs) for each DFC level.
To detect the DFC, older observers need a larger displacement of the Gabor. Since the displaced Gabor had one of four randomly chosen positions along the circle its detection is unlikely to depend on local comparisons. Instead, detection is more likely to be mediated by the comparisons of the single displaced Gabor position with the whole contour shape. In that case, the finding that older observers have a higher DFC threshold may indicate a reduced efficiency in a global integrative process. The finding that both the factor condition [F (1,26) = 6.29, P = 0.019, ηp 2 = 0.195] and the condition × group interaction [F (1,26) = 7.71, P = 0.01, ηp 2 = 0.229] were significant supports this suggestion. Indeed, post hoc comparisons revealed that the difference between tangential and mixed conditions was not significant in the younger group (t 26 = 0.19, two-tailed, P = 0.852, d = 0.118) but it was in the older group (t 26 = −3.74, two-tailed, P = 0.001, d = 1.129). Moreover, the difference between groups was significant in the mixed (t 26 = −4.98, two-tailed, P < 0.001, d = 1.830) but not in the tangential condition (t 26 = −1.58, P = 0.127, two-tailed, d = 0.543). 
Spatial Suppression
In the noise condition, background noise was added to the “tangential” target. The noise was made of randomly oriented, equally spaced Gabors, placed along two imaginary concentric circles centered on the screen. 
Figure 4 shows psychometric functions describing detection probability as a function of DFC levels in the tangential (Figs. 1a, 1d) and noise conditions (Figs. 1c, 1f) in the two groups. The ANOVA on threshold values showed a group effect [F (1,26) = 8.95, P = 0.006, ηp 2 = 0.256], indicating a general increase of DFC thresholds with age. Moreover, the effect of condition was significant [F (1,26) = 17.90, P < 0.001, ηp 2 = 0.408], indicating that DFC thresholds are generally affected by background noise. The condition × group interaction was also significant [F (1,26) = 5.52, P = 0.027, ηp 2 = 0.175]. Post hoc comparisons showed that the effect of noise was significant in the older group (t 26 = −4.65, two-tailed, P < 0.001, d = 1.926) but not in the younger group (t 26 = −1.33, two-tailed, P = 0.195, d = 0.589). 
Figure 4.
 
Psychometric functions for the (a) younger and (b) older groups in the tangential (TAN) and noise (NOI) conditions, obtained by fitting observed mean detection probabilities (TAN_obs, NOI_obs) for each DFC level.
Figure 4.
 
Psychometric functions for the (a) younger and (b) older groups in the tangential (TAN) and noise (NOI) conditions, obtained by fitting observed mean detection probabilities (TAN_obs, NOI_obs) for each DFC level.
Furthermore, an inspection of the results of Figure 4 revealed an interesting finding. Indeed, although both groups are generally affected by noise, the largest DFC level illustrates a group dissociation: only older observers are strongly impaired. A post hoc t-test showed that the difference between the tangential and noise conditions at the largest DFC level was nonsignificant in the younger observers group (t 13 = 1.22, P = 0.246, two-tailed, d = 0.474) but it was significant in the older observers (t 13 = 4.88, P < 0.001, two-tailed, d = 2.219 group). 
Discussion
Spatial Integration
Results show that older observers are strongly impaired in detecting the DFC when the circular contour contains mixed orientations. Nevertheless, performance of the two groups does not significantly differ in the detection of the DFC when the circular contour is defined by tangential Gabors. These results are relevant to the issue of whether there are age-related losses in the integrative operation involved in the detection of DFC of curvilinear contours and whether these depend on reduced orientation discrimination. To be integrated, oriented elements lying along a curved contour have to stimulate cells with relative orientations and spatial positions that optimize their encoding of the contour. 35 That is, the association of one cell with another is strong not only along the axis given by the cell's orientation but also along a curved contour as long as the orientation of the two cells is tangential to the contour. In this case, an association field is formed that integrates the response of the two cells through excitatory connections. This is possible for orientation differences up to ±60° among elements along the contour. Conversely, if oriented elements lying along the contour stimulate cells with relative orientations and positions that do not optimize their encoding of the contour, inhibitory connections are activated. 34,35  
The “association field model” predicts detection of curved contours but does not account for the difference between open and closed contours. Closed contours are better detected than open contours. 23 Moreover, the integration of elements lying along a closed contour tolerates larger inter-element distances than the integration of elements along an open contour. 23 Finally, for closed contours integration occurs with as few as four/five tangential signal elements, even when there are, as in our stimuli, four noise elements with nontangential orientation between each pair of tangential elements along the contour. 25,45 47 To account for the relative insensitivity to perturbation of local orientation, some authors have suggested that the detection of circular contours involves the comparison of the centroid of the contrast envelope because it does not vary with Gabor orientation. 52 Detection of DFC could be based on this strategy. However, if older observers were less efficient in this strategy they should also be impaired in the tangential condition. Alternatively, the insensitivity to local orientation perturbations could be ascribed to the activation of a shape-specific mechanism that integrates the relevant orientations along the closed contour while discarding interposed nontangential orientations. This mechanism may involve extrastriate areas in the ventral stream. 53  
The specific impairment in the mixed condition indicates that aging may reduce the efficiency of the shape-specific mechanism. However, it is unlikely that this impairment is the result of older observers requiring more than four relevant orientations. Although it has been claimed that older subjects need more elements for shape integration, 15,41 the evidence produced is not indisputable. Indeed, Del Viva and Agostini 15 found a group difference in the slope of the linear regression line fitting average sensitivity data as a function of the number of elements in the target. The shallower slope they observed for older adults reflects a lower rate of sensitivity improvement as the number of elements increased. Importantly, McKendrick et al. 41 found that thresholds (i.e., the minimum number of contour elements required for shape discrimination) were very similar in the two groups and did not differ from those of four/five elements needed to activate a shape-specific mechanism. 45 Instead, an age-dependent deficit in discarding nonrelevant orientations is more likely, a suggestion confirmed by the results in the noise condition of the present study's experiment. 
Spatial Suppression
Comparison between the results obtained in the tangential and noise conditions indicates that older observers are more impaired than younger observers in the noise condition. Moreover, at the largest DFC level only older observers are significantly impaired, indicating that they do not take advantage of the largest DFC. We suggest that this is because although Gabors with large displacements from the circular contour are easily detected, they are also easily embedded in the background noise. The masking effect is increased in older subjects because of reduced suppression of the background noise. 
Reduced Background Suppression or Lower Efficiency in Detecting Local Density Irregularity?
The display containing the DFC always has one displaced Gabor, and this creates a local density irregularity in that location. As such, rather than a reduced suppression of background noise, higher DFC thresholds in the older group may indicate reduced efficiency in detecting which interval contains a local density irregularity. To check for this second possibility we left the procedure unvaried but changed the task and used a stimulus constructed by modifying each contour in the noise condition as follows: we randomized the orientation of the elements defining the contour without changing their position to obtain patterns made up of randomly oriented Gabors placed along three circles and one displaced Gabor. We asked six younger subjects who did not participate in the main experiment to perform a 2I-2AFC task, indicating in which of the two presentations there was a density irregularity. t-Tests, testing whether accuracy was significantly different from 50% (chance level) at each DFC level, showed no significant results (P-values of 0.111 [t 5 = 1.94], 1.000 [t 5 = 0.00], 0.256 [t 5 = 1.28], 0.661 [t 5 = 0.47], and 0.090 [t 5 = 2.10] for DFC levels equal to 1.5, 5.9, 10.3, 14.8, and 19.2 arcmin, respectively). 
These results rule out the possibility that higher DFC thresholds for the older group in the noise condition indicate lower efficiency in detecting which interval contains the local density irregularity. 
Suppressive Mechanism, Attention, or Working Memory?
To summarize, we have shown that aging reduces the efficiency in integrating local oriented elements into a closed curvilinear contour when this task requires the exclusion of irrelevant orientations (in the mixed and noise conditions). This suggests an age-dependent reduction in the efficiency of the suppressive mechanism, a change that reduces the capacity to discard irrelevant orientations along the contour and in the background. Neurophysiological studies in cats and monkeys provide indirect support for this suggestion. They show reduced lateral inhibition as well as increased spontaneous activity in senescent V1 neurons selective for orientation and direction of motion. 37,38 Those changes might result from reduced GABA-mediated inhibition. 39 This could specifically affect the suppressive mechanism while leaving the integrative mechanism unperturbed. Indeed, intracortical interactions underlying these two visual operations are different: whereas the majority of the postsynaptic excitatory effects result from long-range intracortical interactions, intracortical inhibitory interactions between GABAergic inhibitory cells mediating the suppression of irrelevant orientations on contour detectability are predominantly short range 36 and largely independent of orientation. 
It is also interesting to speculate whether the data can be explained by declining attentional capacity with age. Attentional factors cannot be excluded because they may affect the relatively low level perceptual operations investigated here. Indeed, it has been demonstrated that attention modulates both facilitatory and inhibitory contextual influences in contour integration and segmentation 19,54 and exclusion of distracters. 55 Declining attentional capacity with age should negatively affect all conditions tested in the present study's experiment and not only mixed and noise conditions. Thus, the reduction of attentional resources with age cannot be the only explanation. Similarly, differences in working memory cannot account for our results: indeed a general effect on all conditions tested should have emerged. 
To conclude, we suggest that in older observers reduced inhibitory intracortical lateral connections may account for the increased effect of background noise. Those same changes can account for the reduced performance of a shape-specific mechanism that integrates only a few tangential Gabors along a circular contour while suppressing nontangential ones. 
Footnotes
 Supported in part by Italian Ministry of Education, University Grants PRIN 2005 and Ex-60% (CC); a sabbatical grant from Consejo Nacional de Ciencia y Tecnología de México; and a Dirección General de Asuntos de Personal Académico grant, National Autonomous University of Mexico (SC).
Footnotes
 Disclosure: C. Casco, None; V. Robol, None; M. Barollo, None; S. Cansino, None
The authors thank Massimiliano Martinelli for assistance in stimuli generation, Rick van der Zwan for assistance with English grammar and usage, and the observers who participated in the study. 
References
Winn B Whitaker D Elliott DB Phillips NJ . Factors affecting light-adapted pupil size in normal human subjects. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1994;35:1132–1137. [PubMed]
Pierscionek BK Weale RA . The optics of the eye-lens and lenticular senescence. A review. Doc Ophthalmol. 1995;89:321–335. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Kline DW Culham JC Bartel P Lynk L . Aging effects on vernier hyperacuity: a function of oscillation rate but not target contrast. Optom Vis Sci. 2001;78:676–682. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Elliott D Whitaker D MacVeigh D . Neural contribution to spatiotemporal contrast sensitivity decline in healthy ageing eyes. Vision Res. 1990;30:541–547. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Bennett PJ Sekuler AB Ozin L . Effects of aging on calculation efficiency and equivalent noise. J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis. 1999;16:654–668. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Hardy JL Delahunt PB Okajima K Werner JS . Senescence of spatial chromatic contrast sensitivity. I. Detection under conditions controlling for optical factors. J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis. 2005;22:49–59. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Betts LR Sekuler AB Bennett PJ . The effects of aging on orientation discrimination. Vision Res. 2007;47:1769–1780. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Delahunt PB Hardy JL Werner JS . The effect of senescence on orientation discrimination and mechanism tuning. J Vis. 2008;8:5(1–9).
Betts LR Sekuler AB Bennett PJ . Spatial characteristics of center-surround antagonism in younger and older adults. J Vis. 2009;9:25(1–15).
Betts LR Taylor CP Sekuler AB Bennett PJ . Aging reduces center-surround antagonism in visual motion processing. Neuron. 2005;45:361–366. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Bennett PJ Sekuler R Sekuler AB . The effects of aging on motion detection and direction identification. Vision Res. 2007;47:799–809. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Billino J Bremmer F Gegenfurtner KR . Differential aging of motion processing mechanisms: evidence against general perceptual decline. Vision Res. 2008;48:1254–1261. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Herbert AM Overbury O Singh J Faubert J . Aging and bilateral symmetry detection. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2002;57:241–245. [CrossRef]
Andersen GJ Ni R . Aging and visual processing: declines in spatial not temporal integration. Vision Res. 2008;48:109–118. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Del Viva MM Agostini R . Visual spatial integration in the elderly. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2007;48:2940–2946. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Roudaia E Bennett PJ Sekuler AB . The effect of aging on contour integration. Vision Res. 2008;48:2767–2774. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Caputo G Casco C . A visual evoked potential correlate of global figure-ground segmentation. Vision Res. 1999;39:1597–1610. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Polat U Bonneh Y . Collinear interactions and contour integration. Spat Vis. 2000;13:393–401. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Casco C Grieco A Campana G Corvino MP Caputo G . Attention modulates psychophysical and electrophysiological response to visual texture segmentation in humans. Vision Res. 2005;45:2384–2396. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Casco C Campana G Han S Guzzon D . Psychophysical and electrophysiological evidence of independent facilitation by collinearity and similarity in texture grouping and segmentation. Vision Res. 2009;49:583–593. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Field DJ Hayes A Hess RF . Contour integration by the human visual system: evidence for a local “association field.” Vision Res. 1993;33:173–193. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Hess RF Beaudot WH Mullen KT . Dynamics of contour integration. Vision Res. 2001;41:1023–1037. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Kovács I Julesz B . A closed curve is much more than an incomplete one: effect of closure in figure-ground segmentation. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1993;90:7495–7497. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Altmann CF Bülthoff HH Kourtzi Z . Perceptual organization of local elements into global shapes in the human visual cortex. Curr Biol. 2003;13:342–349. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Achtman RL Hess RF Wang YZ . Sensitivity for global shape detection. J Vis. 2003;3:616–624. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Mathes B Fahle M . Closure facilitates contour integration. Vision Res. 2007;47:818–827. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Hess R Field D . Integration of contours: new insights. Trends Cogn Sci. 1999;3:480–486. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Hess RF Hayes A Field DJ . Contour integration and cortical processing. J Physiol (Paris). 2003;97:105–119. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Bonneh Y Sagi D . Effects of spatial configuration on contrast detection. Vision Res. 1998;38:3541–3553. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Saarinen J Levi DM . Integration of local features into a global shape. Vision Res. 2001;41:1785–1790. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Roncato S Casco C . The influence of contrast and spatial factors in the perceived shape of boundaries. Percept Psychophys. 2003;65:1252–1272. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Roncato S Casco C . A new “tilt” illusion reveals the relation between border ownership and border binding. J Vis. 2009;9:14(1–10).
Shipley TF Kellman PJ . Boundary completion in illusory contours: interpolation or extrapolation? Perception. 2003;32:985–999. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Heeger DJ . Normalization of cell responses in cat striate cortex. Vis Neurosci. 1992;9:181–197. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Field DJ Hayes A . Contour integration and the lateral connections of V1 neurons. In: Chalupa LM Werner JS eds. The Visual Neurosciences. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 2004:1069–1079.
Das A Gilbert CD . Topography of contextual modulations mediated by short-range interactions in primary visual cortex. Nature. 1999;399:655–661. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Hua T Li X He L Zhou Y Wang Y Leventhal AG . Functional degradation of visual cortical cells in old cats. Neurobiol Aging. 2006;27:155–162. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Schmolesky MT Wang Y Pu M Leventhal AG . Degradation of stimulus selectivity of visual cortical cells in senescent rhesus monkeys. Nat Neurosci. 2000;3:384–390. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Leventhal AG Wang Y Pu M Zhou Y Ma Y . GABA and its agonists improved visual cortical function in senescent monkeys. Science. 2003;300:812–815. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Govenlock SW Taylor CP Sekuler AB Bennett PJ . The effect of aging on the orientational selectivity of the human visual system. Vision Res. 2009;49:164–172. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
McKendrick AM Weymouth AE Battista J . The effect of normal aging on closed contour shape discrimination. J Vis. 2010;10:1(1–9).
Huang PC Hess RF Dakin SC . Flank facilitation and contour integration: different sites. Vision Res. 2006;46:3699–3706. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Williams CB Hess RF . Relationship between facilitation at threshold and suprathreshold contour integration. J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis. 1998;15:2046–2051. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Hess RF Dakin SC Field DJ . The role of “contrast enhancement” in the detection and appearance of visual contours. Vision Res. 1998;38:783–787. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Loffler G . Perception of contours and shapes: low and intermediate stage mechanisms. Vision Res. 2008;48:2106–2127. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Keeble DR Hess RF . Discriminating local continuity in curved figures. Vision Res. 1999;39:3287–3299. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Levi DM Klein SA . Seeing circles: what limits shape perception? Vision Res. 2000;40:2329–2339. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Owsley C Sekuler R Siemsen D . Contrast sensitivity throughout adulthood. Vision Res. 1983;23:689–699. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Schefrin BE Tregear SJ Harvey LOJr Werner JS . Senescent changes in scotopic contrast sensitivity. Vision Res. 1999;39:3728–3736. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Finney DJ . Probit Analysis. London: Cambridge University Press; 1971.
Peli E Arend LE Young GM Goldstein RB . Contrast sensitivity to patch stimuli: effects of spatial bandwidth and temporal presentation. Spat Vis. 1993;7:1–14. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Hess RF Holliday I . Primitives used in the spatial localization of nonabutting stimuli: peaks or centroids. Vision Res. 1996;36:3821–3826. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Pasupathy A Connor CE . Shape representation in area V4: position-specific tuning for boundary conformation. J Neurophysiol. 2001;86:2505–2519. [PubMed]
Kourtzi Z Huberle E . Spatiotemporal characteristics of form analysis in the human visual cortex revealed by rapid event-related fMRI adaptation. Neuroimage. 2005;28:440–452. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Cameron EL Tai JC Eckstein MP Carrasco M . Signal detection theory applied to three visual search tasks: identification, yes/no detection and localization. Spat Vis. 2004;17:295–325. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1.
 
The circular stimuli used. (ac) Contours without DFC. (df) Contours with DFC (here only a DFC of 14.8 arcmin and only for the Gabor on the right is shown). (a) and (d) show “tangential” stimulus conditions, (b) and (e) “mixed” conditions, and (c) and (f) “noise” conditions.
Figure 1.
 
The circular stimuli used. (ac) Contours without DFC. (df) Contours with DFC (here only a DFC of 14.8 arcmin and only for the Gabor on the right is shown). (a) and (d) show “tangential” stimulus conditions, (b) and (e) “mixed” conditions, and (c) and (f) “noise” conditions.
Figure 2.
 
Mean binocular contrast sensitivity functions of younger (continuous line) and older observers (dotted line).
Figure 2.
 
Mean binocular contrast sensitivity functions of younger (continuous line) and older observers (dotted line).
Figure 3.
 
Psychometric functions for the (a) younger and (b) older groups in the tangential (TAN) and mixed (MIX) conditions, obtained by fitting observed mean detection probabilities (TAN_obs, MIX_obs) for each DFC level.
Figure 3.
 
Psychometric functions for the (a) younger and (b) older groups in the tangential (TAN) and mixed (MIX) conditions, obtained by fitting observed mean detection probabilities (TAN_obs, MIX_obs) for each DFC level.
Figure 4.
 
Psychometric functions for the (a) younger and (b) older groups in the tangential (TAN) and noise (NOI) conditions, obtained by fitting observed mean detection probabilities (TAN_obs, NOI_obs) for each DFC level.
Figure 4.
 
Psychometric functions for the (a) younger and (b) older groups in the tangential (TAN) and noise (NOI) conditions, obtained by fitting observed mean detection probabilities (TAN_obs, NOI_obs) for each DFC level.
×
×

This PDF is available to Subscribers Only

Sign in or purchase a subscription to access this content. ×

You must be signed into an individual account to use this feature.

×