November 2011
Volume 52, Issue 12
Free
Visual Neuroscience  |   November 2011
Visual Cortical Function in Very Low Birth Weight Infants without Retinal or Cerebral Pathology
Author Affiliations & Notes
  • Chuan Hou
    From the The Smith-Kettlewell Eye Research Institute, San Francisco, California;
  • Anthony M. Norcia
    From the The Smith-Kettlewell Eye Research Institute, San Francisco, California;
    the Department of Psychology,
  • Ashima Madan
    the Department of Pediatrics, School of Medicine and
    Healthy Start Pediatrics, Mountain View, California.
  • Solina Tith
    the Department of Biological Sciences, Stanford University, Stanford, California; and
  • Rashi Agarwal
    the Department of Pediatrics, School of Medicine and
  • William V. Good
    From the The Smith-Kettlewell Eye Research Institute, San Francisco, California;
  • Corresponding author: William V. Good, Smith-Kettlewell Eye Research Institute, 2318 Fillmore Street, San Francisco, CA; good@ski.org
Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science November 2011, Vol.52, 9091-9098. doi:https://doi.org/10.1167/iovs.11-7458
  • Views
  • PDF
  • Share
  • Tools
    • Alerts
      ×
      This feature is available to authenticated users only.
      Sign In or Create an Account ×
    • Get Citation

      Chuan Hou, Anthony M. Norcia, Ashima Madan, Solina Tith, Rashi Agarwal, William V. Good; Visual Cortical Function in Very Low Birth Weight Infants without Retinal or Cerebral Pathology. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 2011;52(12):9091-9098. https://doi.org/10.1167/iovs.11-7458.

      Download citation file:


      © ARVO (1962-2015); The Authors (2016-present)

      ×
  • Supplements
Abstract

Purpose.: Preterm infants are at high risk of visual and neural developmental deficits. However, the development of visual cortical function in preterm infants with no retinal or neurologic morbidity has not been well defined. To determine whether premature birth itself alters visual cortical function, swept parameter visual evoked potential (sVEP) responses of healthy preterm infants were compared with those of term infants.

Methods.: Fifty-two term infants and 58 very low birth weight (VLBW) infants without significant retinopathy of prematurity or neurologic morbidities were enrolled. Recruited VLBW infants were between 26 and 33 weeks of gestational age, with birth weights of less than 1500 g. Spatial frequency, contrast, and vernier offset sweep VEP tuning functions were measured at 5 to 7 months' corrected age. Acuity and contrast thresholds were derived by extrapolating the tuning functions to 0 amplitude. These thresholds and suprathreshold response amplitudes were compared between groups.

Results.: Preterm infants showed increased thresholds (indicating decreased sensitivity to visual stimuli) and reductions in amplitudes for all three measures. These changes in cortical responsiveness were larger in the <30 weeks ' gestational age subgroup than in the ≥30 weeks' gestational age subgroup.

Conclusions.: Preterm infants with VLBW had measurable and significant changes in cortical responsiveness that were correlated with gestational age. These results suggest that premature birth in the absence of identifiable retinal or neurologic abnormalities has a significant effect on visual cortical sensitivity at 5 to 7 months' of corrected age and that gestational age is an important factor in visual development.

Prematurity, defined as a gestational age (GA) <37 weeks at birth, and low birth weight, defined by a birth weight <2500 g at birth, are the leading causes of neonatal mortality and morbidity in the United States. 1 This group of infants is at higher risk for visual, cognitive, and motor impairment, as well as for behavioral deficits and disorders of attention compared with infants born full term. 2 10 Very low birth weight infants (VLBW, defined as weighing ≤1500 g at birth) are at particularly high risk because of an increased prevalence of retinopathy of prematurity (ROP), 3,5,7 9,11 15 intraventricular hemorrhage (IVH), and periventricular leukomalacia (PVL), 16 19 among other complications. 4,5,16 20 Several studies have demonstrated the co-occurrence of visual impairment in preterm infants with other neurologic deficits. 4,5,10,16 21 These preterm infants were mostly of extremely low weight (defined as <1000 g) at birth, 5,10,20,22,23 with short gestational age. 23,24 Analyses of prognostic factors for mid- and long-term outcome of extremely low birth weight infants have indicated that the most significant variable correlated with long-term neurologic outcome is gestational age. 23 For example, among short-gestational-age infants, over one third develop neurocognitive (learning disability), motor or behavioral deficits and disorders of attention, 2,6 and gestational age at birth of ≤28 1 or ≤32 25,26 has been associated with a higher risk of long-term visual abnormalities. 25  
The visual pathway from the retina to the primary visual and association cortex is particularly vulnerable during the prenatal and neonatal period, because it undergoes significant development during this time. 21,27 30 Visual impairments in infants and children born prematurely have been reported for decades. 3 5,7 10 However, one question that has not been adequately resolved is whether visual deficits in premature infants result solely from the effects of morbidity (e.g., ROP, IVH, or PVL), or whether premature exposure to the visual world in itself may influence visual functioning. Behavioral studies have shown either no trend, 13,31 a trend toward rapid development, 32 or reduced acuity with preterm birth, 33 35 whereas those in which electrophysiological techniques were used have shown no trend, 36 a trend toward a more rapid maturation, 37 40 or delayed latency in preterm infants. 25 Research on this question is inconclusive because these studies have not excluded infants with ROP or cerebral lesions and have typically included a wide range of gestational ages from 26 to 36 weeks. A single study in 8- to 12-year-old children born preterm but without major brain and neuromotor impairment showed no significant differences between the groups on the ophthalmic, visual cognitive, neurologic, neuromotor, or MRI measures. 41 However, no previous study, to our knowledge, that has examined visual functioning in VLBW infants has excluded detectable cerebral abnormalities (e.g., IVH or PVL) and ROP. Only by testing such a group can the effects of premature visual exposure alone be investigated during this period of rapid visual development. 
Most studies have relied on a single measure (i.e., visual acuity), to assess functioning of the entire visual system, 13,32,34,35 or as an indicator of premature infants' neurologic status. 42 44 If the effects of prematurity on visual pathway development are selective, as has been suggested, 39,45 then a measure of a single visual function may fail to identify some deficits or may not identify the maximum differences. A more useful strategy would be to evaluate a number of different visual functions. 
In the present study, we focused on specifically visual cortical functions by using the swept parameter visual evoked potential (sVEP). This method can be used to estimate sensory thresholds (e.g., the limits of neuronal performance), as well as responsiveness at suprathreshold levels. 37,46,47 Thresholds in the sVEP method are estimated by extrapolating amplitude versus stimulus–intensity functions to 0 amplitude. 48 We measured sVEP response functions for grating spatial frequency as one estimate of visual acuity (two-point resolution, also referred grating threshold as grating acuity), for spatial contrast to estimate visual sensitivity at a low spatial frequency (contrast thresholds or contrast sensitivity), and as a function of vernier offset size, for a second estimate of visual acuity based on relative position sensitivity (vernier acuity). These three thresholds develop at different rates, with low spatial frequency contrast sensitivity maturing first, grating acuity second, and vernier acuity last. 47,49 Grating acuity, a test of spatial resolution that measures the finest grating producing a visual response, reaches half of adult values by 8 months of age. 50,51 Contrast sensitivity, a measure of the ability to detect slight changes in luminance across space, is approximately half that of adult sensitivity by 3 months of age. 49,52 Vernier acuity measures the minimum offset that can be detected between two line segments and is relatively poor during the first year of life when measured with the sVEP. Behavioral and sVEP methods have shown that vernier acuity reaches approximately half of adult values by 5 years of age. 51,53 Vernier acuity requires recognition of spatial relationships and is believed to require a greater degree of cortical processing. Therefore, it may be a more meaningful indicator of higher-order visual cognitive function than other visual functions. 47  
The goal of this study was to determine whether premature birth itself at different gestational ages in the absence of retinal or cerebral pathology contributes significantly to alterations in visual development. 
Methods
Participants
A total of 58 VLBW preterm infants (preterms; mean birth weight ± SD, 1230 ± 205 g) with gestational age (GA) between 26 and 33 weeks and with corrected ages of between 5 and 7 months and 52 age-matched term infants (terms) were enrolled in the study. Among the 58 preterms, 27 were born at <30 weeks of GA, and 31 were born at ≥30 weeks of GA. sVEP assessment at 5 to 7 months of age allows the comparison of terms and preterms in a period when development is either constant or slow. 48,49 Table 1 shows the characteristics of the enrolled infants. GA was assessed by the best obstetrical estimate using the last menstrual period and ultrasound examination. Corrected age at examination was calculated as chronologic age minus the difference of full-term assumed GA (40 weeks) and GA at birth. As seen in Table 1, terms were corrected-age–matched (P = 0.070) to preterms at the examination. However, their chronologic age at examination was younger than that of the preterms (P < 0.001). The corrected age was the same as the chronologic age in the terms because we assumed 40-weeks' GA for the calculation. 
Table 1.
 
Characteristics of Enrolled Infants
Table 1.
 
Characteristics of Enrolled Infants
Demographics Terms (n = 52) Preterms (n = 58) t-Test P
Sex M = 26 M = 20
F = 26 F = 38
Gestational age, wk <30 wk (n = 27), ≥30 wk (n = 31) 40 30.2 ± 3.4
Corrected age at examination, wk 28 ± 4* 27 ± 4 0.070
Chronologic age at examination, wk 28 ± 4 36 ± 5 <0.001
VLBW preterms were recruited from the Department of Pediatrics, at Stanford University, from 2002 to 2008. The infants were consecutively enrolled if they were born at less than 34 weeks of GA, were singletons, and weighed less than 1500 g. We excluded any that had IVH, PVL, and ROP. We also excluded infants with major congenital malformations, genetic chromosomal abnormality, metabolic disorders, or congenital infection at the time of the assessment. Infants of 26 weeks' GA were the most premature infants that met our inclusion criterion that we could include in this cohort. All 58 preterms underwent serial head ultrasound (US) scans between 2 days of age and discharge, and 33 underwent magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) scans near discharge. IVH or PVL was determined by US/MRI. ROP was determined by an ophthalmologist on the basis of a fundus examination. The terms were recruited by letter from the local geographic area, on the basis of information from the birth records maintained by the Department of Vital Statistics of California. They were singletons of at least 38 weeks' gestation, weighing at least 2500 g, born to a parent who was at least 18 years of age. 
The research protocol was approved by the Institutional Review Board of Stanford University and the California Pacific Medical Center and conformed to the tenets of the Declaration of Helsinki. Written informed consent was obtained from the parents of the infants after the sVEP recording procedure was explained. 
Stimuli
Stimuli were presented on a high-bandwidth monochrome monitor (MR2000HB-MED; Richardson Electronics, LaFox, IL) at a screen resolution of 1600 × 1200 pixels and a 60-Hz vertical refresh rate, as described in detail previously. 46 The stimulus field was 18° × 25°. Viewing distance was 100 cm for all subjects. The mean luminance of the display was 102 cd/m2. Three stimulus conditions were presented: sweeps of spatial frequency at high contrast, sweeps of contrast at low spatial frequency, and sweeps of vernier offsets placed in a high-contrast pattern (details in Fig. 1). 
Figure 1.
 
sVEP stimuli. Spatial frequency sVEP (left): an 80% contrast, 3.76-Hz, phase-reversing cosine grating (shown as a square-wave grating) was swept from 2 to 16 cyc/deg in 10 linear steps. Contrast sVEP (middle): a 3.76-Hz, phase-reversing, 2-cyc/deg, cosine grating was swept from 0.5% to 20% contrast in 10-logarithm steps. Vernier sVEP (right): a 2-cyc/deg, 80% contrast, square-wave grating contained vernier displacements that were periodically introduced and removed at 3.76 Hz. The size of the vernier displacements was swept from 8 to 0.5 arcmin in 10-logarithm steps. The sweep duration for all three measures was 10 seconds.
Figure 1.
 
sVEP stimuli. Spatial frequency sVEP (left): an 80% contrast, 3.76-Hz, phase-reversing cosine grating (shown as a square-wave grating) was swept from 2 to 16 cyc/deg in 10 linear steps. Contrast sVEP (middle): a 3.76-Hz, phase-reversing, 2-cyc/deg, cosine grating was swept from 0.5% to 20% contrast in 10-logarithm steps. Vernier sVEP (right): a 2-cyc/deg, 80% contrast, square-wave grating contained vernier displacements that were periodically introduced and removed at 3.76 Hz. The size of the vernier displacements was swept from 8 to 0.5 arcmin in 10-logarithm steps. The sweep duration for all three measures was 10 seconds.
sVEP Recording and Procedure
The electroencephalogram (EEG) was amplified (Model 12 amplifier; Grass, Warwick, RI) (filter settings, 1–100 Hz at −6 dB) at a gain of 20,000. Active electrodes were placed over the infant's scalp at the occipital pole, at locations O1, Oz, and O2, of the international 10-20 system. 54 A reference electrode was placed at Cz and a ground electrode at Pz. Electrode impedance was equal to or less than 10 kΩ. 
During sVEP recording, infants were seated in their parent's lap in front of the monitor. The experimenter attracted the infant's attention to the stimulus with small toys (∼1–2 cm in size) dangled over the center of the display. Recordings were interrupted when the infant was judged not to be attending. If the experimenter interrupted the display with a mouse input, both display and data acquisition program loops were reset to a previous point in the display that was 1 second before the mouse click. 
sVEP Analysis
The sVEP technique has been described in detail previously. 46,50,55 Briefly, the VEP response amplitude was measured as a spatial frequency, contrast, or vernier displacement was varied continuously over a range covering both below- and above-threshold values. The stimulus is presented at a given temporal frequency (3.76 Hz in this study) that drives visual cortical neurons at that frequency and at exact integer multiples of that frequency, as long as the stimulus is in the visible range. The visual response synchronized to the display is sampled with appropriately positioned leads, and the VEP amplitude versus stimulus intensity function is measured as the stimulus-driven response drops into the background EEG noise. In this study, swept parameter presentations were repeated several times (four to eight) to increase the signal to noise ratio through averaging out the uncorrelated background EEG activity. 
To measure the response functions, sVEP recordings for each 10-second trial were divided into 10 sequential epochs that corresponded to the swept stimulus values. For each epoch, a recursive least square (RLS) adaptive filter 56 was used to generate a series of complex-valued spectral coefficients representing the amplitude and phase of response components tuned to various multiples (harmonics, e.g., the second and fourth harmonics [2F1 and 4F1] for the grating and contrast sVEP, and the first, second and fourth harmonics [1F1, 2F1, and 4F1] for the vernier sVEP) of the stimulus frequency (e.g., 3.76 Hz in the present study). These spectral coefficients for each epoch were averaged across trials for each subject, recording the derivation, harmonic, and stimulus conditions. Statistical significance for each epoch was quantified using P values derived from the circular T (T 2 circ) statistic, 57 which tests whether a given response amplitude is significantly different from 0, taking into account both response amplitude and phase consistency across trials. Then, the group VEP amplitudes were also averaged coherently across observers. 
Statistical Analysis
To estimate thresholds from the term and preterm response functions, we constructed vector mean swept parameter response functions for each stimulus for each group of infants. To estimate the standard errors of thresholds and slopes of the group average response functions, we used a jackknife procedure. 58 First, for each group of subjects, we determined the range of epochs for regression using the algorithm cited above, and calculated the threshold, T, and slope, S. Then, using the same regression range, we recalculated a set of n (the number of subjects) estimates of threshold and slope, T i and S i , each obtained by regressing the group response function obtained by removing the ith subject from the sample. The SE of the threshold, Tse, was T s e = ( n 1 ) [ n ( T i T μ ) ] / n , where Tm was the mean of the T i . The SE of the slope was obtained by the same formula, substituting S i for T i . Significant differences between the group thresholds defined by jackknife procedure were identified by two-tailed, heteroscedastic t-tests. 
Group differences in suprathreshold response amplitude were computed on the basis of individual participant response amplitudes (scalar means) and tested with two-tailed, heteroscedastic t-tests. The scalar means focused the analysis on amplitude differences, rather than a combination of amplitude and phase differences that comprise differences between vector means. 
Results
sVEP Response Functions in Term and Preterm Infants
We analyzed sVEP response functions at the second (2F1) and the fourth (4F1) harmonics for spatial frequency and contrast sweep measures and the first (1F1), the second (2F1), and the fourth (4F1) harmonics for vernier sweep measure, because these harmonic components showed the strongest responses as described in previous publications. 46,50,59 Vector-averaged response functions for the spatial frequency, contrast, and vernier sVEP measures are shown in Figure 2. Each of these functions shows a monotonic increase in amplitude as the stimulus values go from the invisible to the visible range. 
Figure 2.
 
Mean response functions for each of the three visual measures. Vector averaged sVEP response functions in terms and preterms for spatial frequency (a, b), contrast (c, d), and vernier sweep (eg) stimuli at Oz derivation. Error bars plot the SEM based on the T 2 circ statistic. 57 Arrows: the mean thresholds derived from the group response functions. The t-test P values for the group threshold differences between preterms and terms are *<0.05, **<0.01, and ***<0.001. Horizontal bars: the significance of amplitude differences between preterm and term infants for P < 0.05 at corresponding sweep values of the stimuli. The response amplitudes were significantly lower, and group thresholds were significantly worse in preterm than those of term infants for all three measures.
Figure 2.
 
Mean response functions for each of the three visual measures. Vector averaged sVEP response functions in terms and preterms for spatial frequency (a, b), contrast (c, d), and vernier sweep (eg) stimuli at Oz derivation. Error bars plot the SEM based on the T 2 circ statistic. 57 Arrows: the mean thresholds derived from the group response functions. The t-test P values for the group threshold differences between preterms and terms are *<0.05, **<0.01, and ***<0.001. Horizontal bars: the significance of amplitude differences between preterm and term infants for P < 0.05 at corresponding sweep values of the stimuli. The response amplitudes were significantly lower, and group thresholds were significantly worse in preterm than those of term infants for all three measures.
Two approaches were taken to quantify the differences between groups: one to test amplitude differences at suprathreshold values of the stimuli and the other to estimate differences in sensory threshold for the three stimuli. We compared amplitude differences between the groups using the data from each epoch of the stimuli. The horizontal black bars in Figure 2 indicate the significance of amplitude differences between the preterms and terms for P < 0.05 at corresponding sweep values of the stimuli. Most individual participant responses were largest in the stimulus furthest from threshold and therefore we will refer these amplitudes as peak amplitudes for simplicity. The peak amplitudes were lower in the preterms for both the second and fourth harmonics of the spatial frequency sweep, for the second harmonic of the contrast sweep, and for the first harmonics of the vernier offset sweeps. The scalar means of peak amplitudes, along with standard errors and P values, are shown in Table 2, and these correspond to the epoch data at the most visible sweep value of the stimuli in Figure 2
Table 2.
 
Scalar Means of Peak Amplitudes for the Three Measures Shown in Figure 2
Table 2.
 
Scalar Means of Peak Amplitudes for the Three Measures Shown in Figure 2
Group Response 1F1 2F1 4F1
Spatial frequency, cyc/deg
    Preterms (n = 52) 6.29 ± 0.66 2.17 ± 0.25
    Terms (n = 46) 8.73 ± 0.83 3.47 ± 0.42
    Difference (P) <0.05 <0.01
Contrast, %
    Preterms (n = 58) 4.49 ± 0.42 1.47 ± 0.14
    Terms (n = 50) 5.93 ± 0.66 1.86 ± 0.25
    Difference (P) 0.065 0.157
Vernier, arcmin
    Preterms (n = 47) 7.89 ± 0.71 2.83 ± 0.28 1.63 ± 0.18
    Terms (n = 51) 10.47 ± 0.94 3.30 ± 0.30 2.15 ± 0.24
    Difference (P) <0.05 0.252 0.084
We used regressions of the response functions to 0 amplitude to estimate sensory thresholds for the group functions. Group thresholds along with standard errors and P values are shown in Table 3 and Figure 2. The group thresholds (grating acuity, vernier acuity, and contrast sensitivity) were significantly higher in the preterm than in the terms for all three measures. The threshold elevations reflect the fact that the response functions were either laterally shifted toward the range that the stimuli became more visible in preterms compared with the terms. For the grating and vernier measures, the response functions in the preterms were shifted toward the lower spatial frequency range for the grating and the larger vernier offset range for the vernier. These leftward shifts of the response function led to a decrease in the estimated grating acuity and vernier acuity in the preterms. For the contrast measure, the response function was shifted toward (rightward) the higher contrast range and that led to a decrease in the estimated contrast sensitivity in the preterms. 
Table 3.
 
Group Thresholds for the Three Measures Shown in Figure 2
Table 3.
 
Group Thresholds for the Three Measures Shown in Figure 2
Group Response 1F1 2F1 4F1
Spatial frequency, cyc/deg
    Preterms (n = 52) 12.66 ± 0.49 13.20 ± 0.53
    Terms (n = 46) 15.24 ± 0.43 13.97 ± 0.35
    Difference (P) <0.001 0.224
Contrast, %
    Preterms (n = 58) 1.19 ± 0.09 1.44 ± 0.07
    Terms (n = 52) 0.27 ± 0.19 1.24 ± 0.04
    Difference (P) <0.0001 <0.01
Vernier, arcmin
    Preterms (n = 51) 0.60 ± 0.05 1.25 ± 0.07 1.15 ± 0.03
    Terms (n = 47) 0.44 ± 0.07 0.67 ± 0.22 0.83 ± 0.03
    Difference (P) <0.05 <0.05 <0.0001
Effect of GA on sVEP Response Functions in Preterm Infants
We also compared sVEP response functions in the preterm infants <30 weeks and ≥30 weeks of GA for the three measures, using the approaches just described, except that we compared amplitude differences between these two subgroups using the epoch data at the most visible sweep value of the stimuli (peak amplitude). A GA of 30 weeks was chosen as the threshold for dividing the group to produce equal sample sizes. Vector averaged response functions for each stimulus type and harmonic along with group thresholds are shown in Figure 3. The peak amplitudes were lower in the <30-week GA subgroup than that in the ≥30-week GA subgroup for all measures except for the vernier offset first and second harmonics. In this case, however, midrange values were depressed in the <30-week GA subgroup (Figs. 3e, 3f). In some cases the amplitude differences were present only at the most suprathreshold part of the range (Figs. 3b, 3c), but in others, amplitude reduction was present over the full range (Fig. 3g). The scalar means of peak amplitudes along with standard errors and P values are shown in Table 4 and these correspond to the epoch data at the most visible sweep value of the stimuli in Figure 3. Group thresholds were worse in the <30-week GA subgroup than that in the ≥30-week GA subgroup for all visual measures at one or more of the measured response harmonics. Group thresholds along with standard errors and P values are shown in Table 5 and Figure 3
Figure 3.
 
GA differences in the sVEP mean response functions for each of the three visual measures. Vector-averaged sVEP response functions in the ≥30-week GA subgroup and <30-week GA subgroup for spatial frequency (a, b), contrast (c, d), and vernier (eg) stimuli at Oz derivation. Error bars represent the SEM. Arrows: the mean thresholds derived from the group response function for each group. The t-test P values for the group threshold differences are *<0.05, **<0.01, and ***<0.001. Horizontal bars: the significance of peak amplitude differences between the < and ≥30-week GA subgroups for P < 0.05. The mean peak amplitudes were significantly lower and group thresholds were significantly worse in the <30-week GA subgroup than those in the ≥30-week GA subgroup for all visual measures.
Figure 3.
 
GA differences in the sVEP mean response functions for each of the three visual measures. Vector-averaged sVEP response functions in the ≥30-week GA subgroup and <30-week GA subgroup for spatial frequency (a, b), contrast (c, d), and vernier (eg) stimuli at Oz derivation. Error bars represent the SEM. Arrows: the mean thresholds derived from the group response function for each group. The t-test P values for the group threshold differences are *<0.05, **<0.01, and ***<0.001. Horizontal bars: the significance of peak amplitude differences between the < and ≥30-week GA subgroups for P < 0.05. The mean peak amplitudes were significantly lower and group thresholds were significantly worse in the <30-week GA subgroup than those in the ≥30-week GA subgroup for all visual measures.
Table 4.
 
Scalar Means of Peak Amplitudes for the Three Measures Shown in Figure 3
Table 4.
 
Scalar Means of Peak Amplitudes for the Three Measures Shown in Figure 3
Group Response 1F1 2F1 4F1
Spatial frequency, cyc/deg
    <30 wk (n = 24) 4.90 ± 0.78 1.45 ± 0.20
    ≥30 wk (n = 28) 7.47 ± 0.98 2.78 ± 0.40
    Difference (P) <0.05 <0.01
Contrast, %
    <30 wk (n = 27) 3.77 ± 0.49 1.15 ± 0.13
    ≥30 wk (n = 31) 5.44 ± 0.65 1.83 ± 0.23
    Difference (P) <0.05 <0.05
Vernier, arcmin
    <30 wk (n = 22) 6.43 ± 0.75 2.36 ± 0.26 1.09 ± 0.15
    ≥30 wk (n = 29) 9.00 ± 1.07 3.19 ± 0.46 2.14 ± 0.28
    Difference (P) 0.055 0.12 <0.01
Table 5.
 
Group Thresholds for the Three Measures Shown in Figure 3
Table 5.
 
Group Thresholds for the Three Measures Shown in Figure 3
Group Response 1F1 2F1 4F1
Spatial frequency, cyc/deg
    <30 wk (n = 24) 11.34 ± 0.70 12.35 ± 1.17
    ≥30 wk (n = 28) 11.67 ± 0.58 12.13 ± 0.61
    Difference (P) <0.05 0.86
Contrast, %
    <30 wk (n = 27) 1.14 ± 0.06 2.83 ± 0.06
    ≥30 wk (n = 31) 0.94 ± 0.06 1.02 ± 0.08
    Difference (P) <0.05 <0.001
Vernier, arcmin
    <30 wk (n = 22) 1.11 ± 0.07 1.07 ± 0.12 1.28 ± 0.06
    ≥30 wk (n = 29) 0.57 ± 0.06 0.71 ± 0.07 0.80 ± 0.03
    Difference (P) <0.0001 <0.05 <0.0001
Discussion
Most studies have relied on a single measure (i.e., visual acuity), to assess functioning of the entire visual system, 13,32,34,35 or as an indicator of premature infants' neurologic status. 42 44 Most important, these studies did not exclude retinal pathology (e.g., ROP) or cerebral pathology (e.g., IVH an/or PVL) during the prenatal and neonatal period. Thus, it is not clear whether premature birth itself (premature exposure to the visual world) affects visual development or whether previously reported differences between the term and preterm infants were due to subtle neurologic abnormalities. In the present study, three different assays of spatial vision were obtained by using the sVEP in a cohort of VLBW preterms in the absence of identifiable ROP, IVH, or PVL at 5 to 7 months' corrected age. Sensory thresholds for all three measures were elevated in the preterms compared with the terms and differed between <30-week GA subgroup and the ≥30-week GA subgroup infants in the preterm group. The VLBW preterms also had significant decreases in cortical responsiveness to suprathreshold stimuli that were more severe within the preterm group when the infants were stratified according to GA. The extrapolation method for estimating thresholds is robust against changes in response amplitude, 60 and thus the threshold elevations we report are not likely to be a simple consequence of reduced amplitude, especially in cases in which the response function is shifted laterally without a change in slope, as is the case with several of the response functions. 
The effects of prematurity and GA were present for each of the three sweep types, suggesting that the effect is robust and is not specific to a single visual task. Developmentally, visual maturity is reached at quite different ages for low spatial frequency contrast sensitivity, grating, and vernier acuity, suggesting that our three tasks were tapping at least partially separate cortical mechanisms. 47,61 At present, we do not know whether the effects observed in this study in infancy persist or are predictive of visual and other developmental outcomes, either in infancy or in later childhood. A recent behavioral study 62 found that performance on the Griffith Mental Developmental Scales was normal at both 3 and 12 months in infants with normal head ultrasound in the neonatal period, but that abnormal head ultrasound results were at least partially predictive of behavioral abnormalities. The Griffith scales measure different aspects of visual function that are being measured by the sweep VEP, and this difference in what is assessed may explain the apparent discrepancy between our results and the results on the Griffith scales. 
GA at delivery is a critical factor for visual and neuronal development, especially for long-term outcomes. 23 The <30-week GA subgroup showed more severe reductions in cortical responsiveness than did the ≥30-week GA subgroup, especially for the fourth harmonic response of vernier sVEP (see Fig. 3g). The substantially decreased vernier threshold and strong reduction in amplitude at fourth harmonic response of sVEP clearly shows the presence of a neural deficit in the infants born at <30 weeks of GA. This result is consistent with previous behavioral findings that GA at birth of ≤28 1 or ≤32 25,26 weeks is associated with a higher risk of long-term visual abnormalities. The effects of GA were larger on the vernier sVEP than on the spatial frequency and contrast threshold measures. Effects seen in our study with preterm infants and results in amblyopia 63,64 and cortical visual impairment 61 suggest that vernier offset responses are more sensitive to disruption by abnormal visual experience or perinatal experience than are grating-based measure. The first-harmonic response to vernier offsets requires the encoding of the spatial relationships between stimulus elements, not just detection of the presence or absence of spatial pattern, and is believed to require a greater degree of cortical processing. 61,63,64  
Reductions in the amplitude of the sVEP at suprathreshold levels could be caused by several factors. A decrease in neuronal mass, as occurs in profound brain damage, has been shown to adversely affect the response amplitude. 65 However, this group of VLBW infants had normal head ultrasounds. It is possible that some of the infants had subtle neurologic injuries that were undetected by the imaging modalities that were used. If so, sVEP measures would provide a sensitive tool for detecting neurologic changes. It is plausible, given what is known about the effects of premature birth, especially birth a <28 or 30 weeks of GA or extremely low birth weight (<1000 g), 1,25,26 to consider that this occurs even in preterm infants without ROP and detectable neurologic morbidity (e.g., IVH or PVL). Another theoretical explanation includes the possibility that the normal balance of neuronal excitation and inhibition is altered in preterm infants with neurologic injury. Indeed, it has been reported that the GABA pathway is vulnerable to perinatal brain injury, with a loss of GABAergic neuron expression found in premature infants. 66 A shift of this balance toward inhibition would have the effect of reducing signal amplitude. It is also possible that decreases in the temporal precision (synchronization) of synaptic activity could occur and result in reduced response amplitudes. 67,68  
Recent studies of MRI and diffusion tensor imaging suggest that white matter changes are responsible for alterations in vision in premature infants. 69,70 Such changes can be subtle and perhaps not even visible on conventional MR scanning. Deficits in processing local and global motion can also occur in the apparent absence of cerebral pathology. 71 It is possible that the infants in our study had such MRI changes and that these changes account in part for our findings. 
Findings in this study indicate significant threshold and suprathreshold neurophysiological changes in infants with VLBW. Changes were detected several months (5–7 months) after the birth and were present in all three visual sensitivity measures, suggesting a more generalized effect of premature birth, especially birth at <30 weeks of GA on visual development. These three measures (grating acuity, contrast sensitivity, and vernier acuity) are most likely subserved by different cortical mechanisms. 47,61 Whether these changes portend subclinical or clinically important alterations in visuocortical functioning is an open question requiring longer follow-up and additional investigation. 
The authors thank Patricia Hartsell, Sharon Cassinelli, and Judith Y. Hall for their assistance in recruiting and co-coordinating preterm participants' visits and Margaret Q. McGovern for the assistance in recruiting and conducting term infants' experiments. 
Footnotes
 ST was an undergraduate student at Stanford University, and RA was a volunteer research assistant at Stanford University School of Medicine at the time of the study.
Footnotes
 Supported in part by Grant 1 R01-EY015228 (WVG, AM, AMN) from the National Eye Institute and Grant 5 M01 RR000070 from the National Center for Research Resources (Stanford University), National Institutes of Health, Department of Health and Human Services, Bethesda, MD; and by funds from the Children's Eye Foundation of the American Association for Pediatric Ophthalmology and Strabismus (WVG).
Footnotes
 Disclosure: C. Hou, None; A.M. Norcia, None; A. Madan, None; S. Tith, None; R. Agarwal, None; W.V. Good, None
References
Spencer R . Long-term visual outcomes in extremely low-birth-weight children (an American Ophthalmological Society thesis). Trans Am Ophthalmol Soc. 2006;104:493–516. [PubMed]
Bhutta AT Cleves MA Casey PH Cradock MM Anand KJ . Cognitive and behavioral outcomes of school-aged children who were born preterm: a meta-analysis. JAMA. 2002;288:728–737. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Fledelius HC Kjer B . Surveillance for retinopathy of prematurity in a Danish country: epidemiological experience over 20 years. Acta Ophthalmol Scand. 2004;82:38–41. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Hack M Wilson-Costello D Friedman H Taylor GH Schluchter M Fanaroff AA . Neurodevelopment and predictors of outcomes of children with birth weights of less than 1000 g: 1992–1995. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2000;154:725–731. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Lemons JA Bauer CR Oh W . Very low birth weight outcomes of the National Institute of Child health and human development neonatal research network, January 1995 through December 1996. NICHD Neonatal Research Network. Pediatrics. 2001;107:E1. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Marlow N Wolke D Bracewell MA Samara M . Neurologic and developmental disability at six years of age after extremely preterm birth. N Engl J Med. 2005;352:9–19. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
O'Connor AR Stephenson T Johnson A . Long-term ophthalmic outcome of low birth weight children with and without retinopathy of prematurity. Pediatrics. 2002;109:12–18. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Palmer EA Hardy RJ Dobson V . 15-year outcomes following threshold retinopathy of prematurity: final results from the multicenter trial of cryotherapy for retinopathy of prematurity. Arch Ophthalmol. 2005;123:311–318. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Rudanko SL Fellman V Laatikainen L . Visual impairment in children born prematurely from 1972 through 1989. Ophthalmology. 2003;110:1639–1645. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Wilson-Costello D Friedman H Minich N Fanaroff AA Hack M . Improved survival rates with increased neurodevelopmental disability for extremely low birth weight infants in the 1990s. Pediatrics. 2005;115:997–1003. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Early Treatment For Retinopathy Of Prematurity Cooperative G. Revised indications for the treatment of retinopathy of prematurity: results of the early treatment for retinopathy of prematurity randomized trial. Arch Ophthalmol. 2003;121:1684–1694. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Steinkuller PG Du L Gilbert C Foster A Collins ML Coats DK . Childhood blindness. J AAPOS. 1999;3:26–32. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Dobson V Mayer DL Lee CP . Visual acuity screening of preterm infants. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1980;19:1498–1505. [PubMed]
Fledelius HC . Pre-term delivery and subsequent ocular development: a 7–10 year follow-up of children screened 1982–84 for ROP. 1) visual function, slit-lamp findings, and fundus appearance. Acta Ophthalmol Scand. 1996;74:288–293. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Msall ME Phelps DL DiGaudio KM . Severity of neonatal retinopathy of prematurity is predictive of neurodevelopmental functional outcome at age 5.5 years. Behalf of the Cryotherapy for Retinopathy of Prematurity Cooperative Group. Pediatrics. 2000;106:998–1005. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Cioni G Fazzi B Coluccini M Bartalena L Boldrini A van Hof-van Duin J . Cerebral visual impairment in preterm infants with periventricular leukomalacia. Pediatr Neurol. 1997;17:331–338. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
O'Keefe M Kafil-Hussain N Flitcroft I Lanigan B . Ocular significance of intraventricular haemorrhage in premature infants. Br J Ophthalmol. 2001;85:357–359. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Pike MG Holmstrom G de Vries LS . Patterns of visual impairment associated with lesions of the preterm infant brain. Dev Med Child Neurol. 1994;36:849–862. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Volpe JJ . Cerebral white matter injury of the premature infant-more common than you think. Pediatrics. 2003;112:176–180. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Pennefather PM Tin W . Ocular abnormalities associated with cerebral palsy after preterm birth. Eye. 2000;14:78–81. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Provis JM van Driel D Billson FA Russell P . Development of the human retina: patterns of cell distribution and redistribution in the ganglion cell layer. J Comp Neurol. 1985;233:429–451. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Greene MF . Outcomes of very low birth weight in young adults. N Engl J Med. 2002;346:146–148. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Valcamonico A Accorsi P Sanzeni C . Mid- and long-term outcome of extremely low birth weight (ELBW) infants: an analysis of prognostic factors. J Matern Fetal Neonatal Med. 2007;20:465–471. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Tommiska V Heinonen K Ikonen S . A national short-term follow-up study of extremely low birth weight infants born in Finland in 1996–1997. Pediatrics. 2001;107:E2. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Feng JJ Wang TX Yang CH Wang WP Xu X . Flash visual evoked potentials at 2-year-old infants with different birth weights. World J Pediatr. 2010;6:163–168. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Saldir M Sarici SU Mutlu FM Mocan C Altinsoy HI Ozcan O . An analysis of neonatal risk factors associated with the development of ophthalmologic problems at infancy and early childhood: a study of premature infants born at or before 32 weeks of gestation. J Pediatr Ophthalmol Strabismus. 2010;47:331–337. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Birch EE O'Connor AR . Preterm birth and visual development. Semin Neonatol. 2001;6:487–497. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Hevner RF . Development of connections in the human visual system during fetal mid-gestation: a DiI-tracing study. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol. 2000;59:385–392. [PubMed]
Lein ES Finney EM McQuillen PS Shatz CJ . Subplate neuron ablation alters neurotrophin expression and ocular dominance column formation. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1999;96:13491–13495. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Rakic P Riley KP . Overproduction and elimination of retinal axons in the fetal rhesus monkey. Science. 1983;219:1441–1444. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Ricci D Cesarini L Gallini F . Cortical visual function in preterm infants in the first year. J Pediatr. 2010;156:550–555. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
van Hof-van Duin J Mohn G . The development of visual acuity in normal fullterm and preterm infants. Vision Res. 1986;26:909–916. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Fledelius H . Prematurity and the eye: ophthalmic 10-year follow-up of children of low and normal birth weight. Acta Ophthalmol Suppl. 1976;128:3–245. [PubMed]
Getz L Dobson V Luna B . Grating acuity development in 2-week-old to 3-year-old children born prior to term. Clin Vision Sci. 1992;251–256.
Sebris SL Dobson V Hartmann EE . Assessment and prediction of visual acuity in 3- to 4-year-old children born prior to term. Hum Neurobiol. 1984;3:87–92. [PubMed]
Kos-Pietro S Towle VL Cakmur R Spire JP . Maturation of human visual evoked potentials: 27 weeks conceptional age to 2 years. Neuropediatrics. 1997;28:318–323. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Norcia AM Tyler CW Piecuch R Clyman R Grobstein J . Visual acuity development in normal and abnormal preterm human infants. J Pediatr Ophthalmol Strabismus. 1987;24:70–74. [PubMed]
Roy MS Barsoum-Homsy M Orquin J Benoit J . Maturation of binocular pattern visual evoked potentials in normal full-term and preterm infants from 1 to 6 months of age. Pediatr Res. 1995;37:140–144. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Sokol S Jones K . Implicit time of pattern evoked potentials in infants: an index of maturation of spatial vision. Vision Res. 1979;19:747–755. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Tsuneishi S Casaer P . Effects of preterm extrauterine visual experience on the development of the human visual system: a flash VEP study. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2000;42:663–668. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
O'Reilly M Vollmer B Vargha-Khadem F . Ophthalmological, cognitive, electrophysiological and MRI assessment of visual processing in preterm children without major neuromotor impairment. Dev Sci. 2010;13:692–705. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Cioni G Bertuccelli B Boldrini A . Correlation between visual function, neurodevelopmental outcome, and magnetic resonance imaging findings in infants with periventricular leucomalacia. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed. 2000;82:F134–F140. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Guzzetta A Cioni G Cowan F Mercuri E . Visual disorders in children with brain lesions, 1: Maturation of visual function in infants with neonatal brain lesions: correlation with neuroimaging. Eur J Paediatr Neurol. 2001;5:107–114. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
SanGiovanni JP Allred EN Mayer DL Stewart JE Herrera MG Leviton A . Reduced visual resolution acuity and cerebral white matter damage in very-low- birthweight infants. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2000;42:809–815. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Taylor HG Hack M Klein N Schatschneider C . Achievement in children with birth weights less than 750 grams with normal cognitive abilities: evidence for specific learning disabilities. J Pediatr Psychol. 1995;20:703–719. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Mirabella G Kjaer PK Norcia AM Good WV Madan A . Visual development in very low birth weight infants. Pediatr Res. 2006;60:435–439. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Skoczenski AM Norcia AM . Development of VEP Vernier acuity and grating acuity in human infants. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1999;40:2411–2417. [PubMed]
Campbell FW Maffei L . Electrophysiological evidence for the existence of orientation and size detectors in the human visual system. J Physiol. 1970;207:635–652. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Norcia AM Tyler CW Hamer RD . Development of contrast sensitivity in the human infant. Vision Res. 1990;30:1475–1486. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Norcia AM Tyler CW . Spatial frequency sweep VEP: visual acuity during the first year of life. Vision Res. 1985;25:1399–1408. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Skoczenski AM Norcia AM . Late maturation of visual hyperacuity. Psychol Sci. 2002;13:537–541. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Norcia AM Tyler CW Hamer RD . High visual contrast sensitivity in the young human infant. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1988;29:44–49. [PubMed]
Zanker J Mohn G Weber U Zeitler-Driess K Fahle M . The development of vernier acuity in human infants. Vision Res. 1992;32:1557–1564. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Odom JV Bach M Barber C . Visual evoked potentials standard (2004). Doc Ophthalmol. 2004;108:115–123. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Norcia AM Tyler CW Hamer RD Wesemann W . Measurement of spatial contrast sensitivity with the swept contrast VEP. Vision Res. 1989;29:627–637. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Tang Y Norcia AM . An adaptive filter for steady-state evoked responses. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1995;96:268–277. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Victor JD Mast J . A new statistic for steady-state evoked potentials. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1991;78:378–388. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Sprent P . Applied Non-parametric Statistical Methods. London: Chapman & Hall; 1989.
Norcia AM Tyler CW Allen D . Electrophysiological assessment of contrast sensitivity in human infants. Am J Optom Physiol Opt. 1986;63:12–15. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Norcia AM Clarke M Tyler CW . Digital filtering and robust regression techniques for estimating sensory thresholds from the evoked potential. IEEE Eng Med Biol Mag. 1985;4:26–32. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Skoczenski AM Good WV . Vernier acuity is selectively affected in infants and children with cortical visual impairment. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2004;46:526–532. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Ricci D Romeo DM Gallini F . Early visual assessment in preterm infants with and without brain lesions: correlation with visual and neurodevelopmental outcome at 12 months. Early Hum Dev. 2011;87:177–182. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
McKee SP Levi DM Movshon JA . The pattern of visual deficits in amblyopia. J Vis. 2003;3:380–405. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Hou C Good WV Norcia AM . Validation study of VEP vernier acuity in normal-vision and amblyopic adults. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2007;48:4070–4078. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Good WV . Development of a quantitative method to measure vision in children with chronic cortical visual impairment. Trans Am Ophthalmol Soc. 2001;99:253–269. [PubMed]
Robinson S Li Q Dechant A Cohen ML . Neonatal loss of gamma-aminobutyric acid pathway expression after human perinatal brain injury. J Neurosurg. 2006;104:396–408. [PubMed]
Lestienne R . Spike timing, synchronization and information processing on the sensory side of the central nervous system. Prog Neurobiol. 2001;65:545–591. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Rodriguez-Molina VM Aertsen A Heck DH . Spike timing and reliability in cortical pyramidal neurons: effects of EPSC kinetics, input synchronization and background noise on spike timing. PLoS One. 2007;2:e319. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Ramenghi LA Ricci D Mercuri E . Visual performance and brain structures in the developing brain of pre-term infants. Early Hum Dev. 2010;86(suppl 1):73–75. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Bassi L Ricci D Volzone A . Probabilistic diffusion tractography of the optic radiations and visual function in preterm infants at term equivalent age. Brain. 2008;131:573–582. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
MacKay TL Jakobson LS Ellemberg D Lewis TL Maurer D Casiro O . Deficits in the processing of local and global motion in very low birthweight children. Neuropsychologia. 2005;43:1738–1748. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1.
 
sVEP stimuli. Spatial frequency sVEP (left): an 80% contrast, 3.76-Hz, phase-reversing cosine grating (shown as a square-wave grating) was swept from 2 to 16 cyc/deg in 10 linear steps. Contrast sVEP (middle): a 3.76-Hz, phase-reversing, 2-cyc/deg, cosine grating was swept from 0.5% to 20% contrast in 10-logarithm steps. Vernier sVEP (right): a 2-cyc/deg, 80% contrast, square-wave grating contained vernier displacements that were periodically introduced and removed at 3.76 Hz. The size of the vernier displacements was swept from 8 to 0.5 arcmin in 10-logarithm steps. The sweep duration for all three measures was 10 seconds.
Figure 1.
 
sVEP stimuli. Spatial frequency sVEP (left): an 80% contrast, 3.76-Hz, phase-reversing cosine grating (shown as a square-wave grating) was swept from 2 to 16 cyc/deg in 10 linear steps. Contrast sVEP (middle): a 3.76-Hz, phase-reversing, 2-cyc/deg, cosine grating was swept from 0.5% to 20% contrast in 10-logarithm steps. Vernier sVEP (right): a 2-cyc/deg, 80% contrast, square-wave grating contained vernier displacements that were periodically introduced and removed at 3.76 Hz. The size of the vernier displacements was swept from 8 to 0.5 arcmin in 10-logarithm steps. The sweep duration for all three measures was 10 seconds.
Figure 2.
 
Mean response functions for each of the three visual measures. Vector averaged sVEP response functions in terms and preterms for spatial frequency (a, b), contrast (c, d), and vernier sweep (eg) stimuli at Oz derivation. Error bars plot the SEM based on the T 2 circ statistic. 57 Arrows: the mean thresholds derived from the group response functions. The t-test P values for the group threshold differences between preterms and terms are *<0.05, **<0.01, and ***<0.001. Horizontal bars: the significance of amplitude differences between preterm and term infants for P < 0.05 at corresponding sweep values of the stimuli. The response amplitudes were significantly lower, and group thresholds were significantly worse in preterm than those of term infants for all three measures.
Figure 2.
 
Mean response functions for each of the three visual measures. Vector averaged sVEP response functions in terms and preterms for spatial frequency (a, b), contrast (c, d), and vernier sweep (eg) stimuli at Oz derivation. Error bars plot the SEM based on the T 2 circ statistic. 57 Arrows: the mean thresholds derived from the group response functions. The t-test P values for the group threshold differences between preterms and terms are *<0.05, **<0.01, and ***<0.001. Horizontal bars: the significance of amplitude differences between preterm and term infants for P < 0.05 at corresponding sweep values of the stimuli. The response amplitudes were significantly lower, and group thresholds were significantly worse in preterm than those of term infants for all three measures.
Figure 3.
 
GA differences in the sVEP mean response functions for each of the three visual measures. Vector-averaged sVEP response functions in the ≥30-week GA subgroup and <30-week GA subgroup for spatial frequency (a, b), contrast (c, d), and vernier (eg) stimuli at Oz derivation. Error bars represent the SEM. Arrows: the mean thresholds derived from the group response function for each group. The t-test P values for the group threshold differences are *<0.05, **<0.01, and ***<0.001. Horizontal bars: the significance of peak amplitude differences between the < and ≥30-week GA subgroups for P < 0.05. The mean peak amplitudes were significantly lower and group thresholds were significantly worse in the <30-week GA subgroup than those in the ≥30-week GA subgroup for all visual measures.
Figure 3.
 
GA differences in the sVEP mean response functions for each of the three visual measures. Vector-averaged sVEP response functions in the ≥30-week GA subgroup and <30-week GA subgroup for spatial frequency (a, b), contrast (c, d), and vernier (eg) stimuli at Oz derivation. Error bars represent the SEM. Arrows: the mean thresholds derived from the group response function for each group. The t-test P values for the group threshold differences are *<0.05, **<0.01, and ***<0.001. Horizontal bars: the significance of peak amplitude differences between the < and ≥30-week GA subgroups for P < 0.05. The mean peak amplitudes were significantly lower and group thresholds were significantly worse in the <30-week GA subgroup than those in the ≥30-week GA subgroup for all visual measures.
Table 1.
 
Characteristics of Enrolled Infants
Table 1.
 
Characteristics of Enrolled Infants
Demographics Terms (n = 52) Preterms (n = 58) t-Test P
Sex M = 26 M = 20
F = 26 F = 38
Gestational age, wk <30 wk (n = 27), ≥30 wk (n = 31) 40 30.2 ± 3.4
Corrected age at examination, wk 28 ± 4* 27 ± 4 0.070
Chronologic age at examination, wk 28 ± 4 36 ± 5 <0.001
Table 2.
 
Scalar Means of Peak Amplitudes for the Three Measures Shown in Figure 2
Table 2.
 
Scalar Means of Peak Amplitudes for the Three Measures Shown in Figure 2
Group Response 1F1 2F1 4F1
Spatial frequency, cyc/deg
    Preterms (n = 52) 6.29 ± 0.66 2.17 ± 0.25
    Terms (n = 46) 8.73 ± 0.83 3.47 ± 0.42
    Difference (P) <0.05 <0.01
Contrast, %
    Preterms (n = 58) 4.49 ± 0.42 1.47 ± 0.14
    Terms (n = 50) 5.93 ± 0.66 1.86 ± 0.25
    Difference (P) 0.065 0.157
Vernier, arcmin
    Preterms (n = 47) 7.89 ± 0.71 2.83 ± 0.28 1.63 ± 0.18
    Terms (n = 51) 10.47 ± 0.94 3.30 ± 0.30 2.15 ± 0.24
    Difference (P) <0.05 0.252 0.084
Table 3.
 
Group Thresholds for the Three Measures Shown in Figure 2
Table 3.
 
Group Thresholds for the Three Measures Shown in Figure 2
Group Response 1F1 2F1 4F1
Spatial frequency, cyc/deg
    Preterms (n = 52) 12.66 ± 0.49 13.20 ± 0.53
    Terms (n = 46) 15.24 ± 0.43 13.97 ± 0.35
    Difference (P) <0.001 0.224
Contrast, %
    Preterms (n = 58) 1.19 ± 0.09 1.44 ± 0.07
    Terms (n = 52) 0.27 ± 0.19 1.24 ± 0.04
    Difference (P) <0.0001 <0.01
Vernier, arcmin
    Preterms (n = 51) 0.60 ± 0.05 1.25 ± 0.07 1.15 ± 0.03
    Terms (n = 47) 0.44 ± 0.07 0.67 ± 0.22 0.83 ± 0.03
    Difference (P) <0.05 <0.05 <0.0001
Table 4.
 
Scalar Means of Peak Amplitudes for the Three Measures Shown in Figure 3
Table 4.
 
Scalar Means of Peak Amplitudes for the Three Measures Shown in Figure 3
Group Response 1F1 2F1 4F1
Spatial frequency, cyc/deg
    <30 wk (n = 24) 4.90 ± 0.78 1.45 ± 0.20
    ≥30 wk (n = 28) 7.47 ± 0.98 2.78 ± 0.40
    Difference (P) <0.05 <0.01
Contrast, %
    <30 wk (n = 27) 3.77 ± 0.49 1.15 ± 0.13
    ≥30 wk (n = 31) 5.44 ± 0.65 1.83 ± 0.23
    Difference (P) <0.05 <0.05
Vernier, arcmin
    <30 wk (n = 22) 6.43 ± 0.75 2.36 ± 0.26 1.09 ± 0.15
    ≥30 wk (n = 29) 9.00 ± 1.07 3.19 ± 0.46 2.14 ± 0.28
    Difference (P) 0.055 0.12 <0.01
Table 5.
 
Group Thresholds for the Three Measures Shown in Figure 3
Table 5.
 
Group Thresholds for the Three Measures Shown in Figure 3
Group Response 1F1 2F1 4F1
Spatial frequency, cyc/deg
    <30 wk (n = 24) 11.34 ± 0.70 12.35 ± 1.17
    ≥30 wk (n = 28) 11.67 ± 0.58 12.13 ± 0.61
    Difference (P) <0.05 0.86
Contrast, %
    <30 wk (n = 27) 1.14 ± 0.06 2.83 ± 0.06
    ≥30 wk (n = 31) 0.94 ± 0.06 1.02 ± 0.08
    Difference (P) <0.05 <0.001
Vernier, arcmin
    <30 wk (n = 22) 1.11 ± 0.07 1.07 ± 0.12 1.28 ± 0.06
    ≥30 wk (n = 29) 0.57 ± 0.06 0.71 ± 0.07 0.80 ± 0.03
    Difference (P) <0.0001 <0.05 <0.0001
×
×

This PDF is available to Subscribers Only

Sign in or purchase a subscription to access this content. ×

You must be signed into an individual account to use this feature.

×