Subjective fixation disparity correlated with each of the three factors investigated: dynamic asymmetry, dark vergence, and nonius bias. The combination of these three factors in a multiple regression analysis explained approximately 62% of the interindividual variability in subjective fixation disparity. In a previous study,
14 we explained approximately 50% of interindividual variance in fixation disparity using dynamic asymmetry alone. In the present study, only 17% (
n = 19) of the variance was explained. This is an obvious difference; however, in the previous study,
14 the range of fixation disparity varied from approximately 15 arc min eso to 20 arc min exo (partly due to the selection of subjects with large fixation disparities), while in the current random sample the range of observed fixation disparities was smaller (from 5 arc min exo to 5 arc min eso). Accordingly, the range of dynamic asymmetry was also much smaller in the present study, so that the overall correlations tended to be smaller. The multiple regression analysis explained a larger proportion of variance than each factor alone, since the three factors influencing subjective fixation disparity showed smaller intercorrelations. Because data of 1 of our 20 subjects had to be excluded as outlier, our findings refer to a sample of subjects with normally distributed data, but not to any observer.
Despite the significant correlation between heterophoria and subjective fixation disparity (
Fig. 3), heterophoria was not included in multiple regression analyses, because there was a significant correlation between heterophoria and dynamic asymmetry (
r = 0.51). Interestingly, Kim et al.
27 also found a strong correlation between heterophoria and dynamic asymmetry (
r = 0.9), defined as a ratio of convergent-to-divergent peak velocity (
V con/
V div). Recalculating this correlation according to their definition, our data also showed a high correlation (
r = 0.54,
P = 0.01;
Fig. 4). Interestingly, the dynamic asymmetry did not correlate significantly to dark vergence, the other possible measure for the resting vergence (
r = 0.008,
n = 18;
r = 0.03,
n = 19). The possible reason for a correlation of the asymmetry with heterophoria but not with dark vergence may lie in the role of accommodative functions that are included in heterophoria but not in dark vergence. A full account for accommodation would require measurement of the accommodative response and AC/A ratio and inclusion of these factors in a multiple regression analysis; such accommodative data were not available in the present study. In a similar study by Jaschinski,
17 such accommodative components tended to be small and nonsignificant in multiple regression analyses for fixation disparity; however, these factors may still play a certain role. These findings suggest that the correlation between dynamic asymmetry and heterophoria, found by Kim et al.,
27 and in the present study, appeared due to accommodative components that are components of heterophoria.
The present study represents a novel investigation in that (1) we extracted dynamic asymmetry from objective measures of vergence velocity (while subjective estimations were used in our previous study
14 ) and (2) we additionally included resting vergence (dark vergence) and nonius bias into combined analyses of these three factors. Regarding the theoretical framework used to explain fixation disparity, Patel et al.
11 have also mentioned the possible influences on fixation disparity of vergence adaptation, proximal cues, viewing distance, heterophoria, and dark vergence. They supposed that when observing fixation disparity “under conditions that eliminate (or keep fixed) the aforementioned parameters (i.e., in the absence of adaptation, for stimuli without proximal cues, when accommodation input and viewing distance are kept constant), that these are modulatory effects, rather than being the basic neural origin of fixation disparity. These factors may affect fixation disparity indirectly via changes in vergence dynamics” (Patel et al.
11 ). So far, however, the results of the present experiment suggest that clinically relevant subjective fixation disparity can originate from a combination of independent physiological sources that directly affect fixation disparity, not only via changes in vergence dynamics. Nevertheless, the relative contributions of these modulating factors in determining vergence dynamics and fixation disparity should be investigated in more detail.
The present study has the following limitations. Only a single step stimulus amplitude of 60 arc min at a single 60-cm viewing distance was included; the use of a wider range of disparity vergence step stimuli and distances could provide a data set for a more critical testing of the model. Other alternative vergence control type models may be tested within an interindividual approach to explain the physiological origin of fixation disparity. The conclusions refer to a sample of subjects with normal distribution of vergence parameters. Additional physiological explanations may be needed for subjects with deviating vergence states or clinical conditions.
Supported by DAAD and Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft JA747/4.