There is no doubt that the overall shape of the “retina neurome”—more than 60 types of neurons, arrayed into at least a dozen functional modules—is correct, but the list of cell types shown in
Figure 1 is, like the human genome, a first draft. Refinements will occur and imprecise places will be rendered exact. Where is the job of structural description essentially done, and where is it more of a work in progress? In one domain, the job of structural description is of course far from done. The emerging connectome project aspires to describe not only all the cell types but all of their connections.
75 –78 Even for the connectome, however, a medium-scale description—classification at the level of whole cells—is still critical, and this is the scale that finds the greatest usefulness for physiological and genetic analyses. What pressing questions remain?
As cell populations, the photoreceptors, horizontal cells, and bipolar cells are now well defined.
19 There are a great many unresolved questions about their functions. A few examples: Are there mixed rod and cone inputs to bipolar cells in some species, and if so, why? What is the interneuronal circuitry of the red and green cones in primates? What is the synaptic mechanism of horizontal cell feedback? For bipolar cells, the great issue is not to define the cell populations structurally, but to understand their physiology. What are the molecular signatures of the bipolar cells, and what information does each type convey from the outer retina to the inner? But the identities of the players are known.
Amacrine cells are more complex and the description of the amacrine cell population less complete. For rabbit amacrine cells (
Figs. 1,
8) the photofilling technique is quantitative, in the sense that the recovery rate for targeted cells was 94%. When compared with known amacrine cell populations, the sample turned out to be accurate: Starburst cells are known to comprise 3% of the total amacrine population and were 5% of the photofilled sample; AII cells 11% and 13%; IAC 2% and 4%; DAPI-3 cells 3% and 3%. These fiducial cells span the range of morphologies and stratification of rabbit amacrine cells, yielding some confidence that the method accurately samples the existing population of small and medium field amacrine cells.
But that study had two notable weaknesses. First, because amacrine cells are distributed among at least 29 types, the number of examples of any one type was very small. When dividing types based on small numbers of examples, the lines that separate the types may occasionally have been incorrectly drawn. And even when the major features are clearly correct—as usually turned out to be the case when the cells could be visualized more clearly in the Golgi material—the details will surely be refined as methods for marking particular cell types become available. An example is the “fountain” cell, so named because of the distinctive recurving pattern of its dendrites.
37,38 This cell type was recognized and its main features correctly described from a sample of only five photofilled examples. Fortunately, the basic description was confirmed in a later study
74 in which a large number of the cells were injected, providing higher quality micrographs and a more precise description of the course of the dendrites. As molecular markers become available for more amacrine cells, this level of detail should be achieved for most cell types and for species other than the rabbit.
Second, even a large sample will encounter few of the retina's wide-field amacrine cells. The cells spread so widely that the retinas's surface can be effectively blanketed using small absolute numbers of cells; any sampling technique will encounter them rarely. They are seen by a few specialized methods,
45,49 and in the transgenic mouse GFP-M,
62,80 whose Thy-1 promoter favors expression of GFP in wide-field amacrine cells (as well as the ganglion cells for which this mouse strain is better known.) They are present in most or all levels of the inner plexiform layer and thus affect many kinds of retinal computation. As already noted, electrophysiological studies have borne this out, revealing a variety of response modulations from outside the classic receptive field. It is clear that the retina's response to any object will be affected by the visual context on which that object appears—but there is at present no way systematically to ensure that these infrequent amacrine cells have been adequately surveyed.
Retinal ganglion cells present even greater difficulties, not the least of which is that there are major variations from species to species. This contrasts with the other retinal cell classes: Some retinas have cone-rich regions centrally, but a cone is generally recognizable as a cone no matter where it occurs. Horizontal cells in monkeys have a narrower spatial extent than horizontal cells in rabbits or cats, but they are plainly the same functional element. As far as is known, the same goes for variations in the shapes of bipolar cells. Remarkably, the four classic, easy-to-stain amacrine cells (AII, starburst, A17, and TH) could hardly be more different in morphology, and each has a distinctive connectivity; yet these are conserved in the retinas of mice, rats, rabbits, cats, monkeys, and humans.
At the level of the ganglion cells, this pattern of conservation breaks down, and significant species differences appear. The central principle remains the same: Retinal ganglion cells in any mammalian species come in more than a dozen anatomic and physiological types, and thus send more than a dozen different representations of the visual input to the brain. But the shapes, and apparently the functions, of retinal ganglion cells vary widely. Decades of argument have failed to resolve even the apparently simple question of homology (or not) between the small (β/midget) and large (α/parasol) ganglion cells of cats and monkeys. The monkey, previously touted as a paragon of ganglion cell simplicity, possesses at least a dozen easily distinguishable ganglion cell types and probably more.
52 In the mouse, much studied for its genetic advantages, the number appears close to 20; there is agreement on many of the cell types, but an agreed-on taxonomy remains elusive.
81 –84
Why is this a critical problem? The answer, of course, is that the outputs of the retinal ganglion cells are the building blocks of visual perception (
Fig. 8). If each point in the visual scene is reported to the higher brain centers by 20 different representations, the brain must use that information in some way—yet textbook concepts of central visual function give no acknowledgment to their existence. A canonical statement of the problem is given by the “smooth cell,” a type of retinal ganglion cell in the macaque monkey meticulously studied by Crook and her colleagues.
85,86 This cell has a physiology indistinguishable (to standard testing) from the classic α/Y/parasol cell, being particularly sensitive to stimuli that flash or move. And yet it is clearly a different cell: (1) The smooth cell is instantly distinguishable from parasol cells in dendritic morphology, (2) it has twice the dendritic field diameter of a parasol cell, and (3) it tiles the retina with a uniform mosaic independent of the mosaic of parasol cells. Thus, the smooth cells send to the brain a coding of the visual input similar to that of the parasol cells, but each smooth cell reports on a region of visual space approximately four times as big as that sampled by a parasol cell. The smooth cells project to the lateral geniculate body, the way station to the cortex. Why does the cortex need to view the world through two different-sized samplings of the same features? Is there some other difference in the encoding transmitted by the smooth cell—something not revealed by testing with standard grating stimuli? And how do these separate representations combine to create visual perception?
To understand what kind of information is carried by each of the diverse ganglion cell types is a very hard problem,
87 a challenge for even the most thoughtful contemporary students of sensory coding.
88 –91 Anatomic structure cannot answer these questions. But it can, together with molecular tools, make a contribution, which is to limit the universe of possibilities: it can specify, in a concrete and unchangeable way,
how many channels electrophysiologists should seek, and a definitive structural classification of retinal ganglion cells is therefore a pressing goal.
Supported over the years by the National Institutes of Health Grant EY017169, the Howard Hughes Medical Institute, and Research to Prevent Blindness.
I am grateful to my collaborators and co-workers, too many to name here but recognized as co-authors on the publications of our laboratory. It is these friendships that have made the work possible and the life of science enjoyable.
I owe the greatest debt to my teacher, mentor, and friend, Adelbert Ames, III, who was for more than 30 years Professor at Harvard Medical School and the Massachusetts General Hospital. Reading Del's papers inspired me to enter retina research. As a fellow in his laboratory, I learned about the cell biology of the nervous system, but also about personal conduct and the values of science. Del made important discoveries: He was the first to show, long ago, that mammalian central nervous tissue could in fact survive and function outside its ordinary protective coverings, opening the way to the multitude of reduced preparations that are the mainstay of modern neuroscience. He did important work on the formation of the cerebrospinal fluid and on the mechanism of tissue damage in stroke, work that had direct consequences for the treatment of patients. Along the way, he developed Ames Medium, widely used today for in vitro studies of mammalian CNS tissue. (Characteristically, he never expected compensation for this invention.) He is a modest person who avoids the spotlight, but he has served as a beacon of clear thinking and scientific values to all who come into contact with him at Harvard and in the wider scientific community. I owe him a great debt, and dedicate this article to him.