In this study, we used a model of extended eye opening with a pixel-based image analysis of the tear film, which revealed that both TBU and tear film thinning stimulate ocular surface sensory neurons. These data suggest that both global thinning or regional thinning of the tear film can stress the ocular surface, supporting a connection between ocular irritation and changes in tear film thickness or instability.
10
All extended eye opening trials in this study resulted in some level of discomfort. While both tear thinning and TBU were associated with stimulation of ocular surface sensory neurons, the cause of the stimulation was not well understood. Subjects felt more stinging and burning than cooling and pricking, suggesting that the irritation during trials could have been, in part, a chemical stimulus.
36 Many have speculated that tear thinning leads to increases in hyperosmolarity,
14,16,32 providing a chemical stimulus for surface polymodal nociceptors
11 and increasing the inflammatory response.
10,12,37 Recently, we showed, using a combination of imaging and psychophysical and cell biology methods, that tear film hyperosmolarity may transiently spike during eye opening trials, perhaps reaching levels as high as 800 to 900 mOsm/kg.
15 Thus, given tear film evaporation, thinning and turnover rates,
38–40 it appears reasonable to attribute the slow increases in discomfort during trials for subjects 1 and 2 (
Fig. 2) to thinning of the tear film with concurrent slow increases in hyperosmolarity.
However, the level of discomfort during TBU was often higher and increased much more quickly than with tear thinning (
Table 2) and significantly more pricking sensations were associated with BU trials (
Table 3). This suggests that TBU involves rapid increases in the stimulation of polymodal nociceptors, perhaps due to sharp local increases in hyperosmolarity.
15 Although the actual events within areas of TBU are unknown, this result also suggests that TBU may involve in rapid evaporation. This could lead to bound mucin layer
41 drying in the TBU area, which may slightly deform the ocular surface and result in mechanical stimulation to the nociceptors.
36 In addition, activation of cold-sensitive afferents may play a role, but their potential contribution to discomfort during TBU or tear thinning is less clear.
25,36,42,43
Regardless of the exact nature of the stimulus provided by the tear film during TBU, these results strongly support the hypothesis that both TBU and tear thinning provide stress to the ocular surface in the form of sensory stimulation of underlying corneal nerves. However, some subjects in this study withheld from blinking for extended periods that would not be realistic under daily conditions. Thus, considering an average blink rate of 12 blinks/min,
44 slow thinning of a stable tear film (as in subjects 1 and 2) would be unlikely to produce much discomfort. However, with rapid and extensive TBU, the discomfort in the interblink interval increased quickly in a short period of time (subjects 3 and 4), so that these subjects presumably must blink frequently to avoid the discomfort associated with rapid and extensive TBU.
In a recent study, we showed associations between the discomfort generated by TBU, tear film hyperosmolarity and the production of pro-inflammatory mediators by corneal epithelial cells.
15 We and others have shown that dry eye patients often repeatedly display TBU during tasks such as computer use, during which the blink rate is known to slow markedly.
45,46 Thus, in patients with extensive and rapidly developing TBU (as in subjects 3 and 4), it appears likely that inadequate blinking could lead to discomfort and repeated stimulation of surface nociceptors over the day. This may explain, in part, the origin of dry eye symptoms because repeated nociceptor stimulation is associated with increased inflammation and hyperalgesia.
11
Although keeping the eye open for extended periods occurs infrequently in daily life, we employ it here as a human-based in vivo model to study the stress that tear instability provides to the ocular surface. The need to blink at the end of the trial, defined by the MBI,
28 could be considered to provide information about both the tear film stimulus and the sensory integrity of the cornea as it responds to the pain or discomfort provided by TBU or tear thinning. In combination with spatiotemporal image analysis of the tear film, our methods allow study of the associations between tear film changes and the sensory response providing much more information than the traditional tear break-up time (TBUT) clinical test, which employs time as the single metric for tear instability. In addition, while the TBUT has been used clinically for more than 40 years,
30 interpretation of tear film fluorescence changes remains poorly understood. For example, areas of TBU are often referred to as “dry spots,”
30 but their cause and composition remain poorly understood.
To address this issue and improve the understanding of events associated with TBU and thinning, we quantified tear film fluorescence changes over time within eye opening trials. Theoretically, the total corneal fluorescence (as measured in this study) during a trial could decrease by two possible mechanisms, either by fluorescein molecules exiting the tear film or by decreases in emitted fluorescence. However, current theory suggests that there is little fluid exchange in or out of the corneal compartment of the tear film during the interblink interval,
31,32 so that most of the fluid lost as the tear film thins over time occurs by evaporation.
18,33,47 This should act to increase the concentration of fluorescein molecules,
40 which are well known to exhibit quenching once a “critical concentration” is reached.
48 Concentration quenching of fluorescein molecules occurs when the fluorescein concentration is high enough to increase collisions between adjacent molecules, cause dye dimerization, or promote energy transfer to nonfluorescent dimers, all of which reduce emitted fluorescence.
49
Recently, tear film thickness was measured directly with changes in fluorescence over time, demonstrating that fluorescein dye quenching occurred during tear film thinning.
40,50 Nichols et al.
50 showed that decreasing tear film thickness with extended eye opening was proportional to the square root of the declining fluorescence intensity. Adapting these methods to our pixel-based data, we obtained a mean fluorescein intensity decay rate of 2.98 ± 4.29 per second (data not shown), which is consistent with their average results of 4.11 ± 6.78 per second for 2% fluorescein. These data further support the hypothesis that diminishing fluorescence during eye opening trials in this study was due to fluorescein dye quenching as the tear film thinned.
In this study, we used fluorescence imaging and psychophysical methods to infer the tear film changes that produce an ocular surface sensory response to TBU and thinning. We found that both processes stimulated surface sensory neurons, and the rate of change was highly correlated with the rate of discomfort. Although the conditions in this study were not intended to mimic daily life, the results strongly suggest that tear instability can directly stimulate ocular surface neurons. If repeated over the day, this recurring stress and surface neural stimulation could explain the daily increase in dry eye symptoms of ocular discomfort.
27,51