Our results for visual field number of abnormal test locations in glaucoma show small, but statistically significant differences among tests of different stimulus size compared with a test that keeps luminance constant and changes stimulus size. It is not likely these differences are clinically significant except possibly for the decrease in detection using the size VI stimulus with early glaucomatous loss. We found Full Threshold size V testing to be slightly more sensitive to detect glaucomatous loss than SITA Standard size III or Full Threshold size VI. Since various reports suggest slightly more abnormal test locations identified with SITA compared with Full Threshold methods,
10–12 it is unlikely that the thresholding method is the reason that the results of these stimulus sizes are so similar. We also found changing stimulus size can perform as well as changing stimulus luminance for disease detection; this was especially true with the pattern deviation analysis.
The automated perimetry methodology that uses increments in size to find a visual threshold was introduced by Frisén in 1987.
13 He used vanishing optotypes as a way to estimate peripheral acuity.
14 The stimuli varied in size as 10
−0.1. The test, high-pass resolution perimetry (ring test), continued, using a staircase procedure to estimate the visual threshold. The test was practical, efficient, and correlated highly with standard types of perimetry.
15–17 Surprisingly, unlike conventional perimetry where a fixed Goldmann stimulus size III was used, variability did not increase substantially as sensitivity decreased.
18,19 We have found similar results with two other types of perimetry where size is changed in small increments, STP, and motion perimetry.
19,20 This property makes these tests strong candidates for longitudinal recognition of visual field progression since variability does not increase as visual function deteriorates. It is encouraging that we found STP can perform as well as changing stimulus luminance for detection of abnormal test locations in glaucoma.
Our results for glaucoma using Goldmann stimulus size VI suggest that at some point, increasing stimulus size results in a decrease in the number of abnormal test locations. Another undesirable consequence of using large stimulus sizes is a reduced detection of some hemianopic defects. Since hemianopic scotomas often have very steep borders, subjects are able to see parts of the large stimulus leading to loss of defect detection and resolution.
2 However, using larger stimulus sizes has some distinct advantages. Variability is lower with larger sizes, often without a loss of signal (the difference between a healthy visual sensory apparatus and a damaged one) and the effective dynamic range is greater.
21 Reducing measurement variability does not yield a net benefit if the signal is reduced in proportion. A reasonable compromise may be to use a method like STP that finds threshold by changing stimulus size. This includes the benefits of using large sizes coupled with the fine resolution of small defects enabled by using small stimulus sizes.
Why do larger stimulus sizes have these properties? Spatial summation is the property of the visual system that relates stimulus size to luminance; threshold intensity multiplied by the stimulus area equals a constant (Ricco's law). Spatial summation gradually increases with increasing distance from the fovea in healthy subjects likely due to the increases in receptive field size and overlap (density) and neural convergence.
22–25 It has been proposed that the area of partial spatial summation, Ricco's area, increases with eccentricity to maintain a constant number of underlying retinal ganglion cells.
26 It appears that with glaucomatous damage, a similar increase in Ricco's area occurs to maintain a constant number of receptive fields.
27 Thus, we hypothesize that larger stimuli may still fall within Ricco's area of partial spatial summation and remain sensitive stimuli for disease detection. However, when the stimulus size exceeds Ricco's area, oversampling of the receptive or perceptive field array may occur and optic nerve damage may not be detected. This may at least in part be reason for the poorer detection of the size VI stimuli in our cases of early glaucomatous damage.
When comparing perimetry types, most studies use the software of the manufacturer's normative database rather than defining the reference standards for controls using a common set of healthy participants. Use of the manufacturer's normative database is problematic due to different numbers of subjects included, use of one or both eyes, and wide differences in inclusion and exclusion criteria for the database. For example, the Humphrey Matrix required a normal Humphrey 24-2 visual field for inclusion. However, the Humphrey database for SITA Standard required only that the subject have a normal eye exam and no eye disease other than refractive error; all visual field examinations obtained were included if they met standard reliability criteria. Requiring a normal visual field eliminates poor test takers and tightens the confidence limits and increases the sensitivity to detect defects at the expense of specificity.
A weakness of our study is that we are comparing size III stimuli using SITA Standard and size V using a Full Threshold algorithm. However, since we are using a common database of healthy observers, there is no bias toward size V. Also, if anything, the SITA strategy might result in an increased number of visual field defects. Bengtsson and Heijl
11 compared probability plot results between Full Threshold and SITA strategies in glaucoma patients. They found SITA showed a slightly larger number of significantly depressed points in the probability maps compared with the Full Threshold strategy. Another weakness is the small sample size of healthy observers and patients with minimal visual field damage. A larger study is necessary to determine if size V is as efficient as size III for identifying the conversion of ocular hypertension to glaucoma.
Size V and STP provide favorable stimulus methodology for detection of mild to moderate glaucoma. Size VI appears slightly less sensitive for glaucoma with mild loss. Future studies should investigate glaucoma with minimal visual field damage to determine if these findings are confirmed. If they do, size V stimuli may be preferable to size III stimuli for glaucoma detection and follow-up.