April 2014
Volume 55, Issue 4
Free
Eye Movements, Strabismus, Amblyopia and Neuro-ophthalmology  |   April 2014
Grating Visual Acuity in Infantile Nystagmus in the Absence of Image Motion
Author Affiliations & Notes
  • Matt J. Dunn
    School of Optometry and Vision Sciences, Cardiff University, Cardiff, United Kingdom
  • Tom H. Margrain
    School of Optometry and Vision Sciences, Cardiff University, Cardiff, United Kingdom
  • J. Margaret Woodhouse
    School of Optometry and Vision Sciences, Cardiff University, Cardiff, United Kingdom
  • Fergal A. Ennis
    School of Optometry and Vision Sciences, Cardiff University, Cardiff, United Kingdom
  • Christopher M. Harris
    Centre for Robotics and Neural Systems, Plymouth University, Plymouth, United Kingdom
  • Jonathan T. Erichsen
    School of Optometry and Vision Sciences, Cardiff University, Cardiff, United Kingdom
  • Correspondence: Jonathan T. Erichsen, School of Optometry and Vision Sciences, Cardiff University, Maindy Road, Cardiff CF24 4HQ, Wales, UK; ErichsenJT@cardiff.ac.uk
Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science April 2014, Vol.55, 2682-2686. doi:https://doi.org/10.1167/iovs.13-13455
  • Views
  • PDF
  • Share
  • Tools
    • Alerts
      ×
      This feature is available to authenticated users only.
      Sign In or Create an Account ×
    • Get Citation

      Matt J. Dunn, Tom H. Margrain, J. Margaret Woodhouse, Fergal A. Ennis, Christopher M. Harris, Jonathan T. Erichsen; Grating Visual Acuity in Infantile Nystagmus in the Absence of Image Motion. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 2014;55(4):2682-2686. https://doi.org/10.1167/iovs.13-13455.

      Download citation file:


      © ARVO (1962-2015); The Authors (2016-present)

      ×
  • Supplements
Abstract

Purpose.: Infantile nystagmus (IN) consists of largely horizontal oscillations of the eyes that usually begin shortly after birth. The condition is almost always associated with lower-than-normal visual acuity (VA). This is assumed to be at least partially due to motion blur induced by the eye movements. Here, we investigated the effect of image motion on VA.

Methods.: Grating stimuli were presented, illuminated by either multiple tachistoscopic flashes (0.76 ms) to circumvent retinal image motion, or under constant illumination, to subjects with horizontal idiopathic IN and controls. A staircase procedure was used to estimate VA (by judging direction of tilt) under each condition. Orientation-specific effects were investigated by testing gratings oriented about both the horizontal and vertical axes.

Results.: Nystagmats had poorer VA than controls under both constant and tachistoscopic illumination. Neither group showed a significant difference in VA between illumination conditions. Nystagmats performed worse for vertically oriented gratings, even under tachistoscopic conditions (P < 0.01), but there was no significant effect of orientation in controls.

Conclusions.: The fact that VA was not significantly affected by either illumination condition strongly suggests that the eye movements themselves do not significantly degrade VA in adults with IN. Treatments and therapies that seek to modify and/or reduce eye movements may therefore be fundamentally limited in any improvement that can be achieved with respect to VA.

Introduction
Infantile nystagmus (IN) describes a regular, repetitive movement of the eyes. It usually develops within the first 6 months of life, causing ocular oscillations that are constant and persist throughout life. While many individuals with IN have a comorbid pathology of the visual pathway, approximately 30% appear not to and have been labeled as “idiopathic.” 1 Despite the absence of any other detectable pathology, idiopathic cases of IN are typically associated with a moderate reduction in visual acuity (VA), which has been assumed to be caused by the eye movements themselves. For example, the Nystagmus Acuity Function (NAF) and eXpanded NAF (NAFX) are outcome measures that quantify eye movement characteristics in order to predict VA. 2,3 Yet, it is not actually known to what extent image motion affects VA in individuals with IN. 
Infantile nystagmus waveforms typically exhibit so-called foveations—periods during which the eyes move more slowly. It has been presumed that these periods exist to facilitate better VA by reducing motion blur induced by the eye movements. Nonetheless, the eyes are never truly stable for more than a few milliseconds. In normal subjects, an increase in image velocity (above 2.5 deg/s) causes a concordant reduction in VA and perceived contrast intensity, regardless of the direction of movement. 47 One previous study has examined the effects of comparable (nystagmoid) image motion on the vision of normal subjects, and found a decline in VA at velocities above 3 deg/s. 8 While many nystagmus waveforms contain foveation periods with velocities below this threshold, some do not, even in subjects with idiopathic IN. Previous studies 2,3,9,10 have demonstrated a strong intersubject correlation between waveform dynamics and VA. In addition, in experiments in which normally sighted subjects are presented with image motion similar to that produced by nystagmus waveforms, VA improves as simulated foveation period duration increases. 8,1113 This wealth of evidence has led to the assumption that poor waveform dynamics (i.e., brief or high-velocity foveations) reduce VA. Many clinical therapies have been predicated on this assumption. 2,14,15 Nonetheless, in principle, it remains possible that the reverse is true: that poor VA may result in the development of a waveform with less accurate, briefer foveations. 16  
Jin and colleagues 17 have demonstrated that a small flash of light is equally likely to be perceived at all times regardless of when it is presented during the nystagmus waveform. Furthermore, images stabilized on the retina, afterimages of bright flashes, and migraine auras are occasionally perceived as continuously moving in individuals with IN. 18,19 This evidence suggests that visual perception is continuous throughout the slow phases of nystagmus as well as during foveations. Chung et al. 20 have found that normal subjects presented with an image moving in a nystagmoid fashion have improved VA when the image is shown during the simulated foveations but hidden for the remainder of the slow phases. One might therefore expect VA to be similarly degraded by motion blur during the entire slow phase in individuals with IN. 
Here, we sought to measure VA in adults with IN in the absence of image motion, by using briefly flashed gratings in an otherwise dark environment. Abadi and King-Smith 21 have adopted a similar approach. They have determined the luminance required to detect the presence of a single line under continuous and tachistoscopic (0.2 ms) conditions; data were derived from four individuals with IN and three control subjects. Visual stimuli were presented to both groups with a brief flash of light to eliminate image motion, so that the impact of image motion on visual sensitivity could be estimated. They have found that sensitivity to a 16° long line oriented in the same axis as the nystagmus is higher than to a line oriented in the orthogonal axis, which is attributed to meridional amblyopia. However, the relationship between the tachistoscopic and continuous presentations is not discussed, and the sensitivity measure used (i.e., relative sensitivity) cannot be interpreted clinically. Therefore, we used gratings to directly measure the impact of image motion on VA. 
Methods
Seventeen subjects with horizontal idiopathic IN volunteered for the study. First, the diagnosis of idiopathic IN as reported by the subject or by their ophthalmologist was investigated by an optometrist using high-speed eye movement recording, ophthalmoscopy, optical coherence tomography, and a detailed family history. Subjects with nystagmus showing any signs of coexisting ocular pathology other than strabismus were excluded. Following these examinations, four were excluded on the basis of eye movement recordings (one with gaze-evoked nystagmus but no nystagmus in the primary position; three with fusion maldevelopment nystagmus syndrome), two were excluded on the basis of history (achromatopsia and acquired nystagmus), one was excluded owing to iris transillumination (suggesting albinism), and one was excluded owing to having active pathology (Fuchs' endothelial dystrophy). Nine subjects with IN remained to participate in the study (three females, aged 21–69 years; mean, 43 years). Nine normally sighted individuals with no history of ocular disease were recruited (four females, aged 21–48 years; mean, 28 years). The investigation was carried out in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki; informed consent was obtained from the subjects after explanation of the nature and possible consequences of the study. Ethical approval was granted by the Cardiff School of Optometry and Vision Sciences Human Research Ethics Committee. 
First, clinical monocular VA of each eye was measured by using a self-illuminated logMAR chart at 3 m under clinical conditions. The eye with the best VA was then used as the test eye. Subjects with equal VA had their dominant eye tested, as determined by investigation of suppression using a distance Mallett unit. In the case of equidominance, the right eye was tested by default. For the test eye, habitual distance spectacle correction was worn, or refracted correction was provided if refractive error exceeded ±0.50 diopters (mean sphere) from the habitual correction. The nontest eye was patched. 
Subjects were seated 2 m in front of a 12° aperture in the center of a white cardboard mask, through which square-wave gratings were presented (Fig. 1). Large gratings were used in order to ensure that the participant's gaze would be directed toward similar visual stimuli at all times, regardless of eye position during the nystagmus cycle. In addition, gratings provide a robust measure of VA, relying solely on resolution rather than recognition as in the case of optotypes. Twenty square-wave gratings were produced by a high-quality professional printer (RA-4 process, Durst Epsilon photographic printer; Durst Image Technology UK Ltd., Surrey, UK) with fundamental spatial frequencies ranging from −0.46 to 1.48 logMAR on heat-treated, nonglossy photographic card large enough to fill the 12° aperture. 
Figure 1
 
Photograph of the aperture frame illuminated by the flash unit, with a grating mounted inside (tilted 5° up to the left), as viewed by subjects. The bull's-eye targets serving as horizontal and vertical axis references can be seen around the grating edge.
Figure 1
 
Photograph of the aperture frame illuminated by the flash unit, with a grating mounted inside (tilted 5° up to the left), as viewed by subjects. The bull's-eye targets serving as horizontal and vertical axis references can be seen around the grating edge.
Four small bull's-eye targets were arranged around the aperture at 90° intervals, providing reference axes (horizontal and vertical) to aid in judgment of tilt. The bull's-eye targets were illuminated by spots of light from a projector, situated behind the subject. 
Gratings were illuminated either constantly, by a lamp providing 1.62 log cd/m2, or tachistoscopically by an unlimited number of flashes each lasting 0.76 (±0.01) ms, from a Metz Mecablitz 76 MZ-5 flash unit (Metz, Zirndorf, Germany) with an output of 4.64 cd·s/m2. Flash brightness was empirically adjusted in a pilot experiment to provide VA approximately equal to that obtained under constant illumination for one normally sighted individual. Assuming an eye rotating at 14 deg/s (the average ocular velocity in IN 22 ), a flash of this duration would cause only 0.01° of image smear (allowing a maximum possible VA of −0.19 logMAR). The flash was strobed, with the delay between flashes varying randomly between 2 to 6 Hz in order to prevent flash-timing prediction. 
For each presentation, gratings were automatically tilted on a motorized platform either 5° up/down from horizontal or left/right from vertical. Figure 2 shows the tilting mechanism with the aperture removed. Subjects were allowed as much time (or as many flashes) as desired before reporting the perceived tilt direction of each presentation, using a response box. No feedback was given for correct or incorrect responses. The finest grating available that provided a VA equivalent to or worse than the subject's clinical VA (i.e., slightly coarser) was used for the first presentation. Visual acuity was estimated by using a two-alternative forced choice transformed up–down psychophysical staircase procedure of eight reversals with a three-up/one-down criterion. The direction of tilt for any given presentation was decided by combined Gellerman-Fellows sequences. 23 Grating reorientation and flash delivery was automated and computer controlled. The computer identified which grating was to be used next, and the gratings were then physically replaced. Visual acuity was estimated as the mean of the final six staircase reversals. 24  
Figure 2
 
Computer-controlled platform used for automating grating orientation, shown here with the aperture removed.
Figure 2
 
Computer-controlled platform used for automating grating orientation, shown here with the aperture removed.
As mentioned above, this procedure was performed under two different lighting conditions, with gratings oriented about two axes: 
  •  
    Constant horizontal: Gratings oriented ±5° about the horizontal axis, under constant illumination;
  •  
    Tachistoscopic horizontal: Gratings oriented ±5° about the horizontal axis, illuminated tachistoscopically;
  •  
    Constant vertical: Gratings oriented ±5° about the vertical axis, under constant illumination; and
  •  
    Tachistoscopic vertical: Gratings oriented ±5° about the vertical axis, illuminated tachistoscopically.
Test presentation order was randomized. 
Results
The Table shows the data obtained from all 18 subjects, including clinical VA and, for each of the four conditions, grating acuity (logMAR). 
Table
 
VA (logMAR) Recorded for All Subjects
Table
 
VA (logMAR) Recorded for All Subjects
Subject Sex Age, y Clinical VA Constant Horizontal Constant Vertical Tachistoscopic Horizontal Tachistoscopic Vertical
Idiopathic IN GT2 M 59 0.78 0.80 0.86 0.70 0.91
DB M 53 0.64 0.65 0.65 0.80 0.65
JT M 24 0.42 0.21 0.33 0.33 0.41
SW F 69 0.16 0.21 0.37 0.17 0.34
JC2 F 54 0.54 0.42 0.53 0.34 0.54
GS M 28 0.54 0.39 0.51 0.28 0.42
NB M 44 0.26 −0.06 0.31 −0.11 0.33
DP M 38 0.60 0.16 0.54 −0.02 0.68
VW F 21 0.34 0.36 0.46 0.23 0.52
Mean ± standard error 0.48 ± 0.07 0.35 ± 0.09 0.51 ± 0.06 0.30 ± 0.10 0.53 ± 0.06
Controls LP F 23 0.10 0.03 0.01 0.17 0.08
JS2 M 24 −0.16 −0.04 −0.03 0.01 −0.07
FE M 47 −0.08 −0.14 −0.11 −0.08 −0.03
PG M 20 −0.20 −0.11 −0.14 −0.17 −0.08
TM M 48 −0.22 −0.11 −0.03 −0.11 −0.04
AS F 23 −0.14 −0.03 −0.06 −0.01 −0.10
MU F 21 −0.08 0.03 −0.07 −0.03 0.02
BF F 26 −0.10 −0.08 0.01 0.02 0.07
JT2 M 23 −0.14 −0.09 −0.09 −0.20 0.00
Mean ± standard error −0.11 ± 0.03 −0.06 ± 0.02 −0.06 ± 0.02 −0.05 ± 0.04 −0.02 ± 0.02
The data from the Table are summarized in Figure 3
Figure 3
 
Graphical representation of the VAs recorded for all subjects. Error bars indicate standard error.
Figure 3
 
Graphical representation of the VAs recorded for all subjects. Error bars indicate standard error.
Figure 3 shows that, under all illumination conditions and orientations, subjects with idiopathic IN performed significantly worse than controls (all P < 0.005). Subjects with idiopathic IN performed worse for vertically oriented gratings, whereas controls did not show an orientation effect (see below). Most importantly, illumination type did not affect VA for either group. Note that no effect of illumination was expected or observed in the control group, since the brightness of the flash was adjusted in a pilot experiment to give approximately the same VA. 
Tachistoscopic Versus Constant Illumination
The effect of tachistoscopic presentation on VA was analyzed by using paired samples t-tests. Tachistoscopic presentation caused no significant difference in VA in controls for either orientation (horizontal: P = 0.6224; vertical: P = 0.0807). Similarly, in nystagmats, there was no significant difference between lighting conditions for either orientation (horizontal: P = 0.2311; vertical: P = 0.2431). 
Effect of Orientation
Paired samples t-tests indicate a significant orientation effect in nystagmats under both constant (P = 0.0076) and tachistoscopic (P = 0.0188) conditions. For both lighting conditions, near-horizontal grating acuity was better than that for near-vertical gratings. However, the VA for control subjects was not significantly different regardless of orientation under both conditions (P = 0.8672 for constant light and P = 0.4426 for tachistoscopic presentation). 
Discussion
Under all lighting conditions and stimulus orientations, VA was worse for subjects with idiopathic IN than controls. Crucially, the fact that VA did not improve under tachistoscopic illumination suggests that image motion may not be the limiting factor to VA in IN. We found no significant difference in VA between constant and tachistoscopic illumination, even for vertically oriented gratings. Since all the nystagmats in this study had primarily horizontal nystagmus, if motion blur were a limiting factor to visual perception, one would have expected vertically oriented gratings to be clearer under tachistoscopic illumination, resulting in a change in measured VA. Although no effect of illumination was expected in controls (since the flash brightness was set to approximately achieve equality), the absence of a significant improvement in VA in the subjects with idiopathic IN was unexpected. 
Under both lighting conditions, subjects with idiopathic IN had significantly poorer VA for vertical gratings than horizontal, whereas controls showed no effect of orientation. This finding is strongly suggestive of meridional amblyopia in IN and has previously been reported under constant illumination. 25 Abadi and King-Smith 21 have found a similar effect under tachistoscopic illumination using a measure of visual sensitivity, although ours is the first study to measure VA under this condition. 
Previous studies 2,3,12 have reported a correlation between foveation quality (e.g., duration, accuracy) and VA, and concluded that eye movement characteristics can be used to predict VA. While this has been shown with simulated waveforms in controls and between individuals with IN, the correlation does not appear to be evident in response to waveform changes within the same subject. 26,27 The results of minimizing image motion in the present study strongly suggest that there is an upper limit on the VA possible in adults with idiopathic IN, and that this limit is independent of eye movement characteristics. 
Treatments such as biofeedback have been shown to cause increased foveation duration, but were abandoned owing to the lack of an improvement in VA. 28,29 In light of our unexpected finding indicating that VA cannot be expected to improve, it may now be worth revisiting this and other therapies, as there may be other visual benefits that are not captured by VA measurement. For example, we hypothesize that prolonging foveation duration might result in faster visual recognition speed (i.e., reduced visual recognition time), since the retinal locus of highest photoreceptor density would be directed toward the object of interest for a greater proportion of time. 
Despite the incessant eye movements, adults with IN usually do not experience oscillopsia, 18 but regardless of this stable percept, retinal anatomy dictates that vision cannot be optimal when the fovea is not directed at the locus of attention. It is hardly surprising therefore that VA, a static measure of visual function in which viewing time is unlimited, cannot adequately represent the visual experience of those with nystagmus. 
Algorithmic measures of waveform characteristics (such as Nystagmus Optimal Fixation Function and NAFX) are designed to quantify visual performance, but these are currently predicated on the presumed relationship between VA and foveation characteristics. Alternative assessments might measure other aspects of visual performance, such as processing speed (e.g., time-restricted optotype recognition tasks 30 or visual response speed measurements 31 ) or target acquisition timing. 32 Ideally, these measures would correlate with foveation characteristics and subjective visual experience better than VA. 
Image motion blur can have a deleterious effect on vision in normal subjects, which has understandably led to an assumption that the blur induced by the oscillations in IN is, at least partly, responsible for their reduced VA. However, previous studies 27,33,34 have found little if any significant change in subjects' VA as a result of modifications to their eye movements, whether produced by varying gaze angle, stress, or task demand. Moreover, although treatments for nystagmus are often designed to reduce the velocity of the eye movements, they rarely elicit improvements in VA, whether using optotypes for recognition acuity 15,35,36 or its prerequisite, resolution acuity, as measured by gratings in the present study. 
The results of the present study indicate that removing the image motion blur altogether in subjects with IN also does not change VA, suggesting that their VA may already be fundamentally limited, owing to an underlying pathology and/or stimulus deprivation amblyopia as a result of motion blur during the critical period for visual development. One view on the pathogenesis of IN is that it is a developmental adaptation to enhance contrast in the presence of a pre-existing visual acuity deficit. 3739 If this is the case, then the parameters of the adult waveform (foveation duration, average eye velocity, etc.) may well reflect the maximum VA that was available in infancy. This would explain the strong correlation between, for example, foveation duration and VA across subjects. 10 In other words, poor-quality nystagmus waveforms may not lead to poor VA; rather, the properties of nystagmus waveforms in adults may reflect the underlying VA, as suggested by a recent study on the development of IN. 40 For these reasons, interventional studies are likely to require better outcome measures than VA alone if they are to demonstrate an objective change in visual performance. 
Acknowledgments
We thank Lawrence Wilkinson for the loan of an EyeLink 1000 (SR Research, Ottawa, Canada) to assess the eye movements of the subjects with IN in this study and the Nystagmus Network (UK) for their help with recruiting subjects for the study. 
Disclosure: M.J. Dunn, None; T.H. Margrain, None; J.M. Woodhouse, None; F.A. Ennis, None; C.M. Harris, None; J.T. Erichsen, None 
References
Lorenz B Gampe E. Analysis of 180 patients with sensory defect nystagmus (SDN) and congenital idiopathic nystagmus (CIN) [in German]. Klin Monbl Augenheilkd . 2001; 218: 3–12. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Sheth NV Dell'Osso LF Leigh RJ Vandoren CL Peckham HP Van Doren CL. The effects of afferent stimulation on congenital nystagmus foveation periods. Vision Res . 1995; 35: 2371–2382. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Dell'Osso LF Jacobs JB. An expanded nystagmus acuity function: intra- and intersubject prediction of best-corrected visual acuity. Doc Ophthalmol . 2002; 104: 249–276. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Ludvigh E Miller JW. Study of visual acuity during the ocular pursuit of moving test objects, I: introduction. J Opt Soc Am . 1958; 48: 799–802. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Miller JW. Study of visual acuity during the ocular pursuit of moving test objects, II: effects of direction of movement, relative movement, and illumination. J Opt Soc Am . 1958; 48: 803–808. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Westheimer G McKee SP. Visual acuity in the presence of retinal-image motion. J Opt Soc Am . 1975; 65: 847–850. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Demer JL Amjadi F. Dynamic visual acuity of normal subjects during vertical optotype and head motion. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci . 1993; 34: 1894–1906. [PubMed]
Chung STL Bedell HE. Effect of retinal image motion on visual-acuity and contour interaction in congenital nystagmus. Vision Res . 1995; 35: 3071–3082. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Abadi RV Worfolk R. Retinal slip velocities in congenital nystagmus. Vision Res . 1989; 29: 195–205. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Abadi RV Pascal E. Visual resolution limits in human albinism. Vision Res . 1991; 31: 1445–1447. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Chung STL Bedell HE. Congenital nystagmus image motion: influence on visual acuity at different luminances. Optom Vis Sci . 1997; 74: 266–272. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Currie DC Bedell HE Song S. Visual-acuity for optotypes with image motions simulating congenital nystagmus. Clin Vis Sci . 1993; 8: 73–84.
Chung STL Bedell HE. Velocity criteria for “foveation periods” determined from image motions simulating congenital nystagmus. Optom Vis Sci . 1996; 73: 92–103. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Dell'Osso LF Hertle RW Leigh RJ Jacobs JB King S Yaniglos S. Effects of topical brinzolamide on infantile nystagmus syndrome waveforms: eyedrops for nystagmus. J Neuroophthalmol . 2011; 31: 228–233. [PubMed]
Hertle RW Dell'Osso LF FitzGibbon EJ Thompson D Yang D Mellow SD. Horizontal rectus tenotomy in patients with congenital nystagmus: results in 10 adults. Ophthalmology . 2003; 110: 2097–2105. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Harris CM. Infantile (congenital) nystagmus. Optom Today . 2013; 48–53.
Jin YH Goldstein HP Reinecke RD. Absence of visual sampling in infantile nystagmus. Korean J Ophthalmol . 1989; 3: 28–32. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Leigh RJ Dell'Osso LF Yaniglos SS Thurston SE. Oscillopsia, retinal image stabilization and congenital nystagmus. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci . 1988; 29: 279–282. [PubMed]
Dell'Osso LF. The mechanism of oscillopsia and its suppression. Ann N Y Acad Sci . 2011; 1233: 298–306. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Chung ST LaFrance MW Bedell HE. Influence of motion smear on visual acuity in simulated infantile nystagmus. Optom Vis Sci . 2011; 88: 200–207. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Abadi RV King-Smith PE. Congenital nystagmus modifies orientational detection. Vision Res . 1979; 19: 1409–1411. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Bedell HE. Perception of a clear and stable visual world with congenital nystagmus. Optom Vis Sci . 2000; 77: 573–581. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Fellows BJ. Chance stimulus sequences for discrimination tasks. Psychol Bull . 1967; 67: 87–92. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Levitt H. Transformed up-down methods in psychoacoustics. J Acoust Soc Am . 1971; 49: 467–477. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Meiusi RS Lavoie JD Summers CG. The effect of grating orientation on resolution acuity in patients with nystagmus. J Pediatr Ophthalmol Strabismus . 1993; 30: 259–261. [PubMed]
Erichsen JT Wiggins D Woodhouse JM Margrain TH Harris CM. Effect of eye orientation on visual acuity in infantile nystagmus (INS). In: 17th European Conference on Eye Movements . August 11–16, 2013; Lund, Sweden. Poster 514.
Jones PH Harris CM Woodhouse JM Margrain TH Ennis F Erichsen JT. Stress and visual function in infantile nystagmus syndrome. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci . 2013; 54: 7943–7951. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Mezawa M Ishikawa S Ukai K. Changes in wave-form of congenital nystagmus associated with biofeedback treatment. Br J Ophthalmol . 1990; 74: 472–476. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Ciuffreda KJ Goldrich SG Neary C. Use of eye movement auditory biofeedback in the control of nystagmus. Am J Optom Physiol Opt . 1982; 59: 396–409. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Yang DS Hertle RW Hill VM Stevens DJ. Gaze-dependent and time-restricted visual acuity measures in patients with infantile nystagmus syndrome (INS). Am J Ophthalmol . 2005; 139: 716–718. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Hertle RW Maybodi M Reed GF Guerami AH Yang D Fitzgibbon EJ. Latency of dynamic and gaze-dependent optotype recognition in patients with infantile nystagmus syndrome versus control subjects. Ann N Y Acad Sci . 2002; 956: 601–603. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Wang ZI Dell'Osso LF. Being “slow to see” is a dynamic visual function consequence of infantile nystagmus syndrome: model predictions and patient data identify stimulus timing as its cause. Vision Res . 2007; 47: 1550–1560. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Wiggins D Woodhouse JM Margrain TH Harris CM Erichsen JT. Infantile nystagmus adapts to visual demand. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci . 2007; 48: 2089–2094. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Cham KM Anderson AJ Abel LA. Task-induced stress and motivation decrease foveation-period durations in infantile nystagmus syndrome. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci . 2008; 49: 2977–2984. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Kumar A Shetty S Vijayalakshmi P Hertle RW. Improvement in visual acuity following surgery for correction of head posture in infantile nystagmus syndrome. J Pediatr Ophthalmol Strabismus . 2011; 48: 341–346. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
McLean RJ Proudlock F Thomas S Degg C Gottlob I. Congenital nystagmus: randomized, controlled, double-masked trial of memantine/gabapentin. Ann Neurol . 2007; 61: 130–138. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Harris CM Berry D. A developmental model of infantile nystagmus. Semin Ophthalmol . 2006; 21: 63–69. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Harris CM Waddington J. Optimal control theory of normal and pathological slow eye movements. J Control Eng Technol . 2013; 3: 181–188.
Harris CM. Oculomotor developmental pathology: an evo-devo perspective. In: Liversedge S Gilchrist ID Everling S eds. Oxford Handbook of Eye Movements . Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2011: 663–686.
Felius J Muhanna ZA. Visual deprivation and foveation characteristics both underlie visual acuity deficits in idiopathic infantile nystagmus. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci . 2013; 54: 3520–3525. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1
 
Photograph of the aperture frame illuminated by the flash unit, with a grating mounted inside (tilted 5° up to the left), as viewed by subjects. The bull's-eye targets serving as horizontal and vertical axis references can be seen around the grating edge.
Figure 1
 
Photograph of the aperture frame illuminated by the flash unit, with a grating mounted inside (tilted 5° up to the left), as viewed by subjects. The bull's-eye targets serving as horizontal and vertical axis references can be seen around the grating edge.
Figure 2
 
Computer-controlled platform used for automating grating orientation, shown here with the aperture removed.
Figure 2
 
Computer-controlled platform used for automating grating orientation, shown here with the aperture removed.
Figure 3
 
Graphical representation of the VAs recorded for all subjects. Error bars indicate standard error.
Figure 3
 
Graphical representation of the VAs recorded for all subjects. Error bars indicate standard error.
Table
 
VA (logMAR) Recorded for All Subjects
Table
 
VA (logMAR) Recorded for All Subjects
Subject Sex Age, y Clinical VA Constant Horizontal Constant Vertical Tachistoscopic Horizontal Tachistoscopic Vertical
Idiopathic IN GT2 M 59 0.78 0.80 0.86 0.70 0.91
DB M 53 0.64 0.65 0.65 0.80 0.65
JT M 24 0.42 0.21 0.33 0.33 0.41
SW F 69 0.16 0.21 0.37 0.17 0.34
JC2 F 54 0.54 0.42 0.53 0.34 0.54
GS M 28 0.54 0.39 0.51 0.28 0.42
NB M 44 0.26 −0.06 0.31 −0.11 0.33
DP M 38 0.60 0.16 0.54 −0.02 0.68
VW F 21 0.34 0.36 0.46 0.23 0.52
Mean ± standard error 0.48 ± 0.07 0.35 ± 0.09 0.51 ± 0.06 0.30 ± 0.10 0.53 ± 0.06
Controls LP F 23 0.10 0.03 0.01 0.17 0.08
JS2 M 24 −0.16 −0.04 −0.03 0.01 −0.07
FE M 47 −0.08 −0.14 −0.11 −0.08 −0.03
PG M 20 −0.20 −0.11 −0.14 −0.17 −0.08
TM M 48 −0.22 −0.11 −0.03 −0.11 −0.04
AS F 23 −0.14 −0.03 −0.06 −0.01 −0.10
MU F 21 −0.08 0.03 −0.07 −0.03 0.02
BF F 26 −0.10 −0.08 0.01 0.02 0.07
JT2 M 23 −0.14 −0.09 −0.09 −0.20 0.00
Mean ± standard error −0.11 ± 0.03 −0.06 ± 0.02 −0.06 ± 0.02 −0.05 ± 0.04 −0.02 ± 0.02
×
×

This PDF is available to Subscribers Only

Sign in or purchase a subscription to access this content. ×

You must be signed into an individual account to use this feature.

×