June 2001
Volume 42, Issue 7
Free
Eye Movements, Strabismus, Amblyopia and Neuro-ophthalmology  |   June 2001
Spatial Localization after Different Types of Retinal Detachment Surgery
Author Affiliations
  • Clifford R. Weir
    From the Tennent Institute of Ophthalmology and the
  • Marie Cleary
    Department of Orthoptics, Gartnavel General Hospital, Glasgow, Scotland, United Kingdom; and the
    Department of Vision Sciences, Glasgow Caledonian University, Scotland, United Kingdom.
  • Stuart Parks
    From the Tennent Institute of Ophthalmology and the
  • Tom Barrie
    From the Tennent Institute of Ophthalmology and the
  • Harold M. Hammer
    From the Tennent Institute of Ophthalmology and the
  • John Murdoch
    From the Tennent Institute of Ophthalmology and the
Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science June 2001, Vol.42, 1495-1498. doi:
  • Views
  • PDF
  • Share
  • Tools
    • Alerts
      ×
      This feature is available to authenticated users only.
      Sign In or Create an Account ×
    • Get Citation

      Clifford R. Weir, Marie Cleary, Stuart Parks, Tom Barrie, Harold M. Hammer, John Murdoch; Spatial Localization after Different Types of Retinal Detachment Surgery. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 2001;42(7):1495-1498.

      Download citation file:


      © ARVO (1962-2015); The Authors (2016-present)

      ×
  • Supplements
Abstract

purpose. To compare the effect on spatial localization of two different forms of surgery for primary rhegmatogenous retinal detachment.

methods. Two groups of 30 patients (one group undergoing conventional external scleral-buckling procedures, the other undergoing vitrectomy procedures) were recruited. They pointed at targets appearing on a computer touchscreen without being able to see their hands, while viewing targets with the non–surgically treated eye. The sizes of the horizontal pointing errors were recorded on three separate occasions: before surgery, on the first postoperative day, and approximately 10 days later.

results. On the first postoperative day a significant change in localization of 2.9 ± 0.9° [SD]) was observed in the scleral-buckling group, compared with 1.3 ± 0.6° in the vitrectomy group. These changes resolved by the second postoperative assessment.

conclusions. These results, particularly in patients in the scleral-buckling group in whom greater manipulation of the extraocular muscles inevitably occurs, are consistent with an alteration in the extraretinal eye position information that is used in spatial localization. This is likely to be a consequence of modified efference copy and/or extraocular muscle proprioception.

The ability to locate the position of objects in the surrounding visual world (spatial localization) is an important aspect of normal visual function. For this to occur with precision the brain must know the direction in which a person is looking, and it depends to a large extent on visual (i.e., retinal) input for this information. However, because the eyes are able to move within the orbits, retinal information by itself is not adequate to specify this visual direction. Further nonvisual (i.e., extraretinal) information regarding the position of the eyes is required to interpret the retinal data and thus determine the direction of gaze. It is believed that this extraretinal eye position information is obtained from two distinct extraretinal sources: monitoring of the motor command sent to the extraocular muscles (efference copy or corollary discharge 1 2 ) and extraocular muscle proprioception. 3 However, the relative contribution of each has been an issue of great controversy for many years. 4 5 6  
The task of pointing at targets in surrounding space is an established method for assessing spatial localization, particularly if subjects are unable to see the pointing hand. 7 8 9 10 11 Integration of the retinal and extraretinal information from the sources outlined in the prior paragraph enables the location of the object of interest to be determined with accuracy. To reach out, or point to this object, an appropriate motor command is sent to the arm and hand, which allows the efficient performance of the task. Patients who undergo conventional scleral-buckling surgery for retinal detachment have been shown to make errors when asked to perform such tasks of spatial localization, while viewing targets with the surgically treated eye. 12 These changes are attributed to alterations in extraocular muscle proprioception, as a consequence of the perioperative manipulation of the muscles. 
However, the visual information available to these patients in the immediate postoperative period (i.e., acuity and field of vision) must also have altered. This is likely to contribute more to these errors than the change in proprioception. A more interesting finding is the postoperative localization shifts found in 4 of 10 of these patients when they were tested while viewing with the fellow non–surgically treated eye. It is reasonable to assume that under these circumstances, modified extraocular muscle proprioception from the surgical eye influences the central interpretation of gaze direction, particularly because it is known that eye position information from both eyes is used for this very purpose. 7 11 13 If this were the case, then patients who undergo retinal detachment surgery that does not directly involve manipulating the extraocular muscles (i.e., vitrectomy) would not be expected to demonstrate any localization changes after surgery when viewing with the fellow nonsurgical eye. To test this hypothesis a comparison of the effect on spatial localization of two different surgical procedures for primary rhegmatogenous retinal detachment—conventional external scleral-buckling surgery and vitrectomy—was made. 
Methods
All procedures conformed to the Declaration of Helsinki for research involving human subjects. Ethics committee approval was obtained, and informed consent was given in all cases. This was a comparative nonrandomized prospective study that was undertaken during a 6-month period from April to October 2000, within the Vitreo-retinal Service at the Tennent Institute of Ophthalmology, Gartnavel General Hospital, Glasgow, Scotland, United Kingdom. 
Protocol
The testing protocol was very similar to that described in a previous study. 11 Subjects were seated and viewed a computer touchscreen (luminance 57 candelas[ cd]/m2; IBM, Greenock, UK) from a distance of 40 cm. Their heads were stabilized with chin rests and cheek pads. Pictures of three vertical poles were presented on the screen: in the center and 15° to the left and to the right of center (Fig. 1) . A red target (a ball, 1.5 cm in diameter, luminance 45 cd/m2) appears on the top of one pole, and the subject is asked to touch the screen at the bottom of the pole on which the ball has landed, with the outstretched index finger of the dominant hand. The ball then moves randomly to the top of another pole, and the subject is asked to touch the location of this pole. 
No limit of time was placed on the pointing response. The target was presented on 10 separate occasions to each pole, and the location of each pointing response was stored online for later analysis. The disparity between the true location and the mean touched location of the 10 presentations was taken as the horizontal pointing error for that pole. A trial run was allowed in which the subjects could initially visualize the pointing hand to enable them to become familiar with the testing procedure. A cardboard sheet covered with black cloth was then used to mask the lower part of the screen, thereby preventing the pointing hand’s being seen. The subjects were allowed to practice with this in place before the formal testing session began. Each subject was tested before surgery (either the day before or the day of surgery) and again on the first postoperative day. A further test was performed on the first follow-up clinic visit, approximately 10 days later. All subjects were tested with appropriate refractive correction, while viewing monocularly with the nonsurgical eye, with the surgical eye patched during this time. The differences between the mean pointing errors recorded before surgery, on the first postoperative day and at the subsequent follow-up visit were then calculated for individual subjects for each pole. Statistical analysis was performed by computer (Prism software; GraphPad, San Diego, CA). 
Subjects
Sixty patients who underwent surgery for primary rhegmatogenous retinal detachment participated in the study and were divided into two groups of 30 as follows: group 1: those who underwent primary external scleral-buckling surgery (mean age 48 ± 17 [SD] years; range, 19–83 years); group 2: those who underwent primary vitrectomy (mean age, 55 ± 15 years; range, 23–73 years). Their clinical details are summarized in Table 1 . The choice of surgical procedure to be performed was decided by one of the vitreoretinal surgeons (HMH, TB, or JM) and was dependent on the requirements of individual patients. 
The number of subjects necessary to detect a difference between the two groups with a power of 90% at the 5% significance level was determined using a power calculation. 14 The SD required for this calculation was derived from a pilot study. None of the subjects had any ophthalmic history of note and no medical history that could have affected their ocular motility or pointing responses. Visual acuities were recorded with appropriate refractive correction using the log minimum angle of resolution (MAR) crowded test at a distance of 3 m. 15  
Surgical Procedure
All surgery was performed with patients under general anesthesia. The conventional external scleral-buckling procedures consisted of drainage of subretinal fluid and application of cryotherapy in the region of the retinal break(s), to create an adhesion between the sensory retina and the underlying retinal pigment epithelium. Silicone explants were placed overlaying the retinal break(s) and oriented either circumferentially (n = 25) or radially (n = 5). An encircling band was also used when appropriate (n = 12). Intravitreal gas was used to effect temporary internal tamponade as required. All four of the rectus muscles were slung to aid movement of the eye. The vitrectomy procedures consisted of a standard three-port pars plana approach, with internal drainage of subretinal fluid, followed by fluid–gas exchange. External cryotherapy was applied in the region of the retinal hole(s). No muscle slings or external buckles were used in the vitrectomy group. 
Results
All subjects were able to perform the test without difficulty on each occasion. Normality testing confirmed a gaussian distribution of the data allowing parametric statistical tests to be performed. The macula was detached in 16 patients and attached in 14 in the scleral-buckling group and was detached in 23 patients and attached in 7 in the vitrectomy group. Before surgery, the visual acuity of the surgical eyes ranged from 0.025 log units to hand movements in the scleral-buckling group and from 0.15 log units to perception of light in the vitrectomy group. The mean visual acuity of the fellow nonsurgical eyes was 0.02 ± 0.09 (SD) log units in the scleral-buckling group and 0.04 ± 0.11 log units in the vitrectomy group. The mean visual acuity of the nonsurgical eye did not change in the postoperative period. The mean length of time between the first and second postoperative assessments was 10.3 ± 1.9 (SD) days in the scleral-buckling group and 9.8 ± 2.1 days in the vitrectomy group. 
Analysis of variance showed that the pointing responses for individual subjects to each of the three poles were similar during each testing session (F = 0.55, P = 0.74). In view of this, the data were collapsed to obtain a single value of the mean pointing response for each patient for that particular testing session. In the scleral-buckling group there was a significant shift in spatial localization of 2.9 ± 0.9° (SD; 95% confidence interval 2.4–3.2°) on the first postoperative day (Fig. 2) . This was statistically significant (P < 0.0001, t = 17.9; one-sample t-test). In the vitrectomy group there was also a significant shift in spatial localization of 1.3 ± 0.6°; 95% confidence interval 1.1–1.5°) on the first postoperative day (P < 0.0001, t = 12.3; Fig. 2 ). A comparison of the changes in localization observed in each of these two groups at this time showed that they were significantly different from each other (P < 0.0001, t = 8.55; unpaired t-test). At the subsequent follow-up assessment 10 days later, these changes had returned toward preoperative values in both groups of patients (Fig. 2) . For example, there was a small, nonsignificant difference between the preoperative and second postoperative testing sessions of 0.5 ± 0.7° (P = 0.25, t = 1.2) in the scleral-buckling group and 0.4 ± 0.6° (P = 0.35, t = 0.95) in the vitrectomy group. There was no significant difference between the changes observed in each group (P = 0.14, t = 1.4). No correlation was found between the age or refractive errors of the patients and the size of localization shifts. 
Discussion
This study demonstrates that spatial localization in patients with primary rhegmatogenous retinal detachment alters significantly more after external scleral-buckling procedures compared with vitrectomy procedures, when viewing with the nonsurgical eye. The technique we used is a recognized method for assessing localization. 7 8 9 10 11 Although a randomized study would have been optimal, it was not feasible, because the decision about the type of surgery to be performed was determined by the clinical status of each patient. 
The results from the scleral-buckling group are consistent with those previously reported by Campos et al., 12 not only in the size of the localization shifts, but also in the time taken for the observed changes to return to their preoperative values, approximately 10 days later. However, they noted alterations in only 4 of 10 patients when testing the fellow non–surgically treated eye. In contrast, we noted changes in all subjects who underwent scleral-buckling surgery, ranging from 1.3° to 4.6°. It is possible that this difference is related to the more sensitive technique we used, in which pointing responses were recorded on a computer touchscreen, rather than the method Campos et al. 12 describe, in which the position of the target was indicated on a piece of paper. The pointing changes we found in this group are of a similar magnitude to those in previous studies in which extraocular muscle proprioception was manipulated experimentally, resulting in mean localization shifts of 2.5° 16 and 2.98°. 17 They are also in keeping with the findings of Steinbach and Smith 8 who observed changes after strabismus surgery, results that were attributed to modified afferent feedback from the extraocular muscles of the surgically treated eye. 
To the best of our knowledge alterations in spatial localization after vitrectomy procedures for retinal detachments have not been reported previously. Why should these changes occur after a procedure that does not appear to involve the extraocular muscles directly? As was discussed in the introduction the viewer relies on a combination of both retinal (visual) and extraretinal information to determine the location of targets with respect to himself or herself. Because all the patients were tested with the surgical eye patched, and because the visual acuity of the nonsurgical eye remained the same after surgery, then an alteration in retinal information is unlikely to account for the results. This indicates that a nonvisual (i.e., extraretinal) signal has influenced spatial localization in the fellow eye. As was outlined earlier, there are two possible sources of this extraretinal information: efference copy and extraocular muscle proprioception. 
Could the efferent copy of the oculomotor command change after vitrectomy? This is possible, particularly because ocular motility problems have been reported after this procedure. 18 However, it should be noted that these changes were recorded several months after surgery, and little is known about ocular motility in the immediate postoperative period. In addition, our testing was performed monocularly, when viewing with the normal fellow eye, and according to Walls, 19 the visual system monitors only the efference command sent to the dominant eye. Bridgeman 4 also supports the concept that there is only one copy of the efferent command. Because there is no reason to believe that the motility of the nonsurgical eye has changed, we cannot be sure whether efference copy influenced the extraretinal eye position signal under the circumstances of our testing procedure. 
The other possible source of the modified extraretinal information is extraocular muscle proprioception. Although no muscle slings or scleral buckles were used during the vitrectomy procedures, a degree of manipulation and rotation of the globe during surgery is inevitable. This may have produced swelling and inflammation in the periorbital tissues in proximity to the extraocular muscles, which in turn could have caused an alteration in proprioceptive feedback. It is also conceivable that periorbital, rather than extraocular, muscle receptors may be the source of this modified afferent signal. 20 However, there is little direct evidence to support the existence of such receptors. 
Although we cannot discount the role of efference copy after vitrectomy, the balance of evidence suggests that the changes in spatial localization we observed are related to an alteration in a nonvisual feedback signal derived from the surgical eye. The most likely source of such a signal is extraocular muscle sensory receptors. 
Although the prime concern in patients who undergo any form of retinal detachment surgery is successful reattachment of the retina with improved visual function, in our experience some patients report difficulty in judging the position of objects relative to themselves. Although this is likely to be related to reduced acuity in the affected eye, combined with postoperative inflammation and mydriasis, our findings suggest that particularly after scleral-buckling procedures, modified extraocular muscle proprioception could be a contributory factor immediately after surgery. What effect these and other surgical procedures involving the extraocular muscles, have on other aspects of visual function, such as oculomotor control, is not known, but perhaps warrants further assessment. 
 
Figure 1.
 
A diagram of the computer touchscreen.
Figure 1.
 
A diagram of the computer touchscreen.
Table 1.
 
Clinical Details of the Two Groups of Patients
Table 1.
 
Clinical Details of the Two Groups of Patients
Scleral Buckling Vitrectomy
Age (y) 48 ± 17 55 ± 15
Refractive error (D)
Surgical eye −3.40 ± 3.6 −3.30 ± 4.4
Fellow eye −3.30 ± 3.5 −3.10 ± 4.2
Affected eye (n)
Right 14 17
Left 16 13
Figure 2.
 
Comparison of changes in localization between the preoperative and first postoperative testing sessions (filled bars) and between the preoperative and second postoperative testing sessions (open bars) for both patient groups. Error bars, SD.
Figure 2.
 
Comparison of changes in localization between the preoperative and first postoperative testing sessions (filled bars) and between the preoperative and second postoperative testing sessions (open bars) for both patient groups. Error bars, SD.
Sperry RW. Neural basis of the spontaneous optokinetic response produced by visual neural inversion. J Comp Physiol Psychol. 1950;43:482–489. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Holst Hv. Relations between the central nervous system and the peripheral organs. Br J Anim Behav. 1954;2:89–94. [CrossRef]
Sherrington CS. Observations of the sensual role of the proprioceptive nerve supply of the extrinsic ocular muscles. Brain. 1918;41:332–343. [CrossRef]
Bridgeman B. A review of the role of efference copy in sensory and oculomotor control systems. Ann Biomed Eng. 1995;23:409–422. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Steinbach MJ. Proprioceptive knowledge of eye position. Vision Res. 1987;27:1737–1744. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Weir CR. Spatial localisation: does extraocular muscle proprioception play a role?. Graefes Arch Clin Exp Ophthalmol. 2000;238:868–873. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Mann VA, Hein A, Diamond R. Localization of targets by strabismic subjects: contrasting patterns in constant and alternating suppressors. Percept Psychophysical. 1979;25:29–34. [CrossRef]
Steinbach MJ, Smith DR. Spatial localization after strabismus surgery: evidence for inflow. Science. 1981;213:1407–1409. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Gauthier GM, Nommay D, Vercher JL. Ocular muscle proprioception and visual localization of targets in man. Brain. 1990;113:1857–1871. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Fronius M, Sireteanu R. Pointing errors in strabismics: complex patterns of distorted visuomotor coordination. Vision Res. 1994;34:689–707. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Weir CR, Cleary M, Parks S, Dutton GN. Spatial localisation in esotropia: does extraretinal eye position information change?. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2000;41:3782–3786. [PubMed]
Campos EC, Bolzani R, Schiavi C, Fanti MR, Cavallini GM. Further evidence for the role of proprioception in space perception. Doc Ophthalmol. 1989;72:155–160. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Ono H, Weber EU. Nonveridical visual direction produced by monocular viewing. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 1981;7:937–947. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Altman DG. Practical Statistics for Medical Research. 1991;455–460. Chapman and Hall London.
McGraw PV, Winn B. Glasgow Acuity Cards: a new test for the measurement of letter acuity in children. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt. 1993;13:400–404. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Han Y, Lennerstrand G. Changes of visual localization induced by eye and neck muscle vibration in normal and strabismic subjects. Graefes Arch Clin Exp Ophthalmol. 1999;237:815–823. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Velay JL, Roll R, Lennerstrand G, Roll JP. Eye proprioception and visual localization in humans: influence of ocular dominance and visual context. Vision Res. 1994;34:2169–2176. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Wright LA, Cleary M, Barrie T, Hammer HM. Motility and binocularity outcomes in vitrectomy versus scleral buckling in retinal detachment surgery. Graefes Arch Clin Exp Ophthalmol. 1999;237:1028–1032. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Walls GL. A theory of ocular dominance. Arch Ophthalmol. 1951;45:387–412. [CrossRef]
Nelson JS, Meredith MA, Stein BE. Does an extraocular proprioceptive signal reach the superior colliculus?. J Neurophysiol. 1989;62:1360–1374. [PubMed]
Figure 1.
 
A diagram of the computer touchscreen.
Figure 1.
 
A diagram of the computer touchscreen.
Figure 2.
 
Comparison of changes in localization between the preoperative and first postoperative testing sessions (filled bars) and between the preoperative and second postoperative testing sessions (open bars) for both patient groups. Error bars, SD.
Figure 2.
 
Comparison of changes in localization between the preoperative and first postoperative testing sessions (filled bars) and between the preoperative and second postoperative testing sessions (open bars) for both patient groups. Error bars, SD.
Table 1.
 
Clinical Details of the Two Groups of Patients
Table 1.
 
Clinical Details of the Two Groups of Patients
Scleral Buckling Vitrectomy
Age (y) 48 ± 17 55 ± 15
Refractive error (D)
Surgical eye −3.40 ± 3.6 −3.30 ± 4.4
Fellow eye −3.30 ± 3.5 −3.10 ± 4.2
Affected eye (n)
Right 14 17
Left 16 13
×
×

This PDF is available to Subscribers Only

Sign in or purchase a subscription to access this content. ×

You must be signed into an individual account to use this feature.

×