The mean distance logMAR visual acuities for the three groups were
0.39 ± 0.16 (cataract, Snellen equivalent 20/50; range,
20/28–20/110), 0.67 ± 0.32 (ARMD, ∼20/100 Snellen; range,
20/32–20/200), and 0.02 ± 0.06 (control, 20/20 Snellen; range,
20/17–20/25). The mean near logMAR visual acuities for the three
groups were 0.43 ± 0.15 (cataract, 0.67 M), 0.87 ± 0.46
(ARMD, 1.85 M), and 0.04 ± 0.05 (control, 0.28 M). There were no
significant differences among the three groups regarding age at final
year of education (P ≫ 0.05). However, there was a
significant difference among the groups in the reported number of hours
spent reading per day (ANOVA, F2,44 = 4.2, P < 0.03). The cataract and control groups read for a
similar number of hours per day (1.46 ± 1.09 and 1.42 ±
0.83, respectively), but the subjects with ARMD read much less
(0.55 ± 0.98 hours per day).
Reading speed versus print size has been shown to have an inverted
U-shaped function, in that it decreases from optimum speeds for both
small and very large words.
19 27 However, over the limited
print sizes used in this study (−0.3 to 1.3 logMAR, text size
of 0.04° to 1.66°) reading speed increases as a function of text
size until it reaches a plateau.
19 27 This plateau
represents the optimal reading speed, and the word size at which this
is reached represents the critical print size. For each condition,
reading speeds for individual subjects were plotted against log print
size
(Fig. 1) . The user-defined curve-fitting capabilities of graph-making
software (KaleidaGraph ver. 3.08; Synergy Software, Reading, PA) were
used to perform the following least-squares bilinear curve fit to the
data
\[\mathrm{If\ }print\ size\mathrm{\ {>}\ size}_{\mathrm{critical}}\mathrm{,\ }reading\ speed{=}reading\ speed_{\mathrm{optimal}}\]
\[\mathrm{Else\ }reading\ speed{=}reading\ speed_{\mathrm{optimal}}-{[}\mathrm{m}{\cdot}(size_{\mathrm{critical}}-print\ size){]}\]
where
size critical is the critical
print size above which reading speed is optimal at a level reading
speed optimal, and below which reading speed
decreases linearly with a gradient
m.
The print size at which no words could be read was assigned a reading
speed of zero and included in the curve fit. Seven of the subjects with
ARMD could read very few lines on the charts. In these cases, there
were not enough points to provide a reliable fit using the
least-squares method. Instead, critical print size and optimal reading
speed were determined by eye. Often, critical print size was the
largest print size used (1.3 logMAR), and optimal reading speed was
therefore recorded as the reading speed for that line.
Means and SDs of optimal reading speed for the three subject groups are
shown in
Table 1 . Optimal reading speed was unaffected by cataract, yet was
significantly reduced by ARMD (F
2,44 = 44.9,
P ≪ 0.001). Optimal reading speed was affected by contrast
polarity (F
1,44 = 12.0,
P <
0.002), with subjects with cataract reading slightly faster with
white-on-black text (102.7 compared with 99.1 wpm) and subjects with
ARMD showing no difference. Pupillary dilation had no significant
effect on optimal reading speed (F
1,44 = 0.03,
P = 0.87).
Means and SDs for critical print size are shown in
Table 1 . Critical
print size was affected by patient group (F
2,44 =
31.6,
P ≪ 0.001), but was unaffected by contrast polarity
(F
1,44 = 3.1,
P = 0.088) or
pupillary dilation (F
1,44 = 0.21,
P≫
0.1).
To further determine how well optimal reading speed discriminates
between the cataract and ARMD subject groups, a receiver-operating
characteristic (ROC) curve of sensitivity versus (1 − specificity) was
plotted for optimal reading speed and for distance and near VA
(Fig. 2) .
Hierarchical stepwise multiple regression was used to determine the
best predictors of optimal reading speed and critical print size using
the traditional contrast charts, from distance VA, near VA, media
clarity (clear, 0; cataract, 1), macular function (healthy, 0; ARMD,
1), and education level (final year at age 14, 0; final year later than
age 14, 1). Optimal reading speed was best predicted by macular
function (r 2 = 0.65) with the
regression equation being: optimal reading speed = 105 − 66
(macular function).
Near VA added significant additional information (combined r 2 = 0.78), with the regression
equation becoming: optimal reading speed = 118.4 − 40.7
(macular function) − 44.6 (near VA).
Critical print size was best predicted from distance VA
(r 2 = 0.76), with no other factor
providing significant additional information. Similarly, near VA
correlated highly with critical print size
(r 2 = 0.69).