This study demonstrated that phoria adaptation to vertical prism
disparity is frequently impaired in patients with cerebellar
dysfunction, even when they do not have manifest ocular misalignment,
limited versional eye movement, or degradation of vertical fusional
amplitude. The mean amplitude of vertical phoria adaptation in the
patient group was significantly smaller than that in the control group.
Compared with the 95% limits of the normal range that were determined
with age-matched control subjects, 7 (54%) of 13 patients showed
abnormally poor responses for both the 10- and 30-minute adaptations.
In measuring vertical phoria adaptation, some difficulty arose from its
rather small magnitude. The normal vertical fusional response typically
breaks at 3 to 4 PD and recovers at 2 PD, and, accordingly, the prism
disparity that can be provided to elicit phoria adaptation is
limited.
12 13 14 15 16 17 19 24 However, our measurement method of
vertical phoria adaptation has advantages. First, vertical phoria
during a single measurement period was markedly stable. Because we
averaged 10 consecutive measurements, the precision of a single phoria
determination was theoretically deduced to be approximately 0.1 PD
(SD). Second, our computer-aided haploscope with a trackball system
provided little help regarding the target locus on the display; the
responses were effectively masked to the subjects. Third, vernier
acuity may contribute to improved precision of the measurement while
the subject determines whether the two targets are aligned. We thus
think that the precision of our measurements was adequate for the
purpose of this study.
The reported time required for full adaptation to vertical prisms
differs among researchers. Henson and North
10 showed that
adaptation to a 2-PD vertical prism was completed in 3 minutes, whereas
Eskridge
24 reported that it required 30 to 120 minutes.
Schor et al.
12 proposed two independent systems for
vertical phoria adaptation: a spatially global and a local system. The
former system shifts the vertical phoria uniformly across the field of
gaze and takes several tens of minutes to complete. The latter system
tunes the phoria selectively to the position, depending on the demands
of the disparity stimulus, and takes more than several hours to
complete. Because we used a uniform prism as a stimulus, the response
we observed was mainly global. Coincidentally, our previous study found
no significant difference in the amount of vertical phoria adaptation
between the 30- and 60-minute adaptations, whereas there was a
significant difference between the 10- and 30-minute adaptations
(increased with time).
20 In this study, for ease of
clinical application, we set the duration of the adaptation periods to
10 and 30 minutes, which seemed to be the minimum requirement for
adaptation saturation.
Besides the smaller mean amount of adaptation, we found that the
responses of the patient group showed some individual idiosyncrasies
that led to the wider variation in their response
(Fig. 2) . A negative
response, during which the phoria shifted to the opposite direction of
the given prism disparity, was observed in three patients (patients 1,
2, and 12) for the 10-minute adaptation only. Magnitudes of response
for the 30-minute adaptation were smaller than those for the 10-minute
adaptation in three patients (patients 3, 11, 13). Such negative
responses
8 or a decreasing amount of response over
time,
3 even though the rather long period (3 minutes) in
our testing procedure interrupted the adaptation process of the
subjects, are rarely seen in healthy subjects.
3 20 24 Normally, the rate of phoria adaptation decays exponentially with time,
which is one indication of the presence of a neural integrator with a
long time constant.
23 It is interesting to note that such
odd responses were also reported elsewhere in horizontal phoria
adaptation in patients with cerebellar dysfunction.
3 8 It
is possible that lesions in the cerebellum make the integrators for
phoria adaptation unstable or fatigue easily, as well as lowering their
gain.
The question of exact anatomic localization is always a difficult one
with clinical lesions, although we have computed tomographic (CT) and
MRI evidence. This is because most patients have multiple or diffuse
lesion(s). Our MRIs showed involvement of the brain stem and pons in
many patients with spinocerebellar degeneration. In our patients,
however, the intact Hering’s law of conjugacy and no limitation of
versional eye movements suggests preservation of the ocular motor
mechanism in the brain stem. The other eye movement disorders observed
in our patients (i.e., saccadic dysmetria, impairment of smooth
pursuit, and pathologic nystagmus), are reportedly all attributable to
lesions in different parts of the cerebellum.
25
A genetic test found that patients 1 and 2 had a CAG repeat expansion
on chromosome 19p, which refers to SCA6.
21 SCA6 is
autosomal dominant and leads to slowly progressive cerebellar ataxia
without multisystem involvement. Pathologically, severe loss of
Purkinje cells was reported, particularly in the vermis, and
disoriented axonal arrangement and many torpedoes, axonal swelling of
Purkinje cells, were found in the granular layer and white
matter.
26 Coincidentally, our MRI images of these patients
showed pure cerebellar atrophy without pons involvement
(Fig. 3) , and, in addition, a neurologic examination did not show any
pyramidal signs such as an increased deep-tendon reflex or dysuria.
Taken together, their markedly degraded responses both for the 10- and
30-minute adaptations show that the intact cerebellum is a precondition
for normal phoria adaptation.
Acute cerebellar ataxia is an inflammatory syndrome of cerebellar
dysfunction that may reflect infectious, postinfectious, or
postvaccination disorders, and it is self-limiting in most
cases.
27 Our three patients with this disease, after
remission of gait ataxia and other cerebellum-associated neurologic
deficits, showed greater responses of vertical phoria adaption than
those observed at the initial visit. Such response recovery paralleling
well-documented cerebellar symptoms suggests that vertical phoria
adaptation is a cerebellar function.
Information from animal studies regarding phoria adaptation is limited.
Judge
28 demonstrated that monkeys with floccular and
ventral paraflocculus lesions can still achieve horizontal phoria
adaptation. Nevertheless, deep cerebellar nuclei are left as possible
critical cerebellar structures influencing phoria
adaptation.
2 Recent electrophysiological studies have
reported that a variety of supranuclear regions of the brain stem, and
possibly the pons and cerebellum, may serve as inputs to the adaptive
system for vergence control.
29 30
In understanding the mechanisms underlying ocular misalignment
associated with cerebellar dysfunction, or in planning its treatment,
the limited function of vertical phoria adaptation should probably be
taken into account. For example, skew deviation, usually a comitant
vertical misalignment of the eyes, is frequently associated with
cerebellar dysfunction, although a variety of ocular motor disorders
have been reported.
8 31 32 A comitant or noncomitant
vertical deviation, termed alternating skew deviation on lateral gaze
(left hyper for the right gaze and right hyper for the left gaze), has
also been reported in patients with lesions in the cerebellar pathways
to the cervicomedullary junction.
33 Theoretically,
unilateral or bilateral lesion(s) of otolith inputs could explain the
skew and alternating skew deviations, respectively. In an experiment
with monkeys, in contrast, cerebellectomy without involving the primary
otolith pathways caused persistent alternating skew
deviation.
34 We do not have any direct evidence from
patients with skew deviation, because we excluded patients with
manifest ocular misalignment from this study. Prismatic compensation
for ocular misalignment could induce another spatially global or local
vergence adaptation. However, it is conceivable that impairment of
vertical phoria adaptation due to cerebellar lesions may allow an
underlying imbalance of the otolith inputs to manifest itself.
In conclusion, this study revealed that phoria adaptation to a vertical
binocular disparity is frequently impaired in patients with cerebellar
dysfunction. We have no evidence about whether cerebellar lesions
respond equally to horizontal and vertical fusional disparities, and
therefore the previously mentioned controversy regarding horizontal
phoria adaptation remains unresolved. Our results, however, bolster
support for the hypothesis that phoria adaptation is a
cerebellum-dependent response.
3
The authors thank James Maxwell, PhD (University of California,
Berkeley) and Isao Nagano, MD (Okayama University Medical School) for
helpful advice.