Free
Clinical and Epidemiologic Research  |   August 2003
How Sensitive to Clinical Change are ETDRS logMAR Visual Acuity Measurements?
Author Affiliations
  • Daniel A. Rosser
    From the Institute of Ophthalmology, University College London, London, United Kingdom;
    Moorfields Eye Hospital, London, United Kingdom; the
  • Simon N. Cousens
    London School of Hygiene and Tropical Medicine, London, United Kingdom; and
  • Ian E. Murdoch
    From the Institute of Ophthalmology, University College London, London, United Kingdom;
    Moorfields Eye Hospital, London, United Kingdom; the
  • Fred W. Fitzke
    From the Institute of Ophthalmology, University College London, London, United Kingdom;
  • David A. H. Laidlaw
    St. Thomas’ Hospital, London, United Kingdom.
Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science August 2003, Vol.44, 3278-3281. doi:https://doi.org/10.1167/iovs.02-1100
  • Views
  • PDF
  • Share
  • Tools
    • Alerts
      ×
      This feature is available to authenticated users only.
      Sign In or Create an Account ×
    • Get Citation

      Daniel A. Rosser, Simon N. Cousens, Ian E. Murdoch, Fred W. Fitzke, David A. H. Laidlaw; How Sensitive to Clinical Change are ETDRS logMAR Visual Acuity Measurements?. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 2003;44(8):3278-3281. https://doi.org/10.1167/iovs.02-1100.

      Download citation file:


      © ARVO (1962-2015); The Authors (2016-present)

      ×
  • Supplements
Abstract

purpose. To determine the sensitivity to change and specificity achieved when published test–retest variability (TRV) data are used to determine whether measured changes in ETDRS logarithm of the minimum angle of resolution (logMAR) visual acuity reflect true clinical change or are attributable to measurement error alone.

methods. Various degrees of change in visual acuity were simulated in a group of normal subjects by adjusting test difficulty through manipulation of viewing distance. Sensitivity to simulated change and specificity were determined with change criteria derived from published Bland-Altman 95% ranges for TRV.

results. The relationship between viewing distance and measured acuity was as predicted theoretically. Simulated acuity change of 0.2 logMAR (two lines of letters) or greater can be reliably distinguished from no change (both sensitivity and specificity >95%) with the ETDRS chart, but a change of 0.1 logMAR or less cannot.

conclusions. The use of 95% ranges for TRV to establish the smallest measured visual acuity change that can be reliably detected ensures a high specificity but does not take account of sensitivity. Use of change criteria derived from published 95% ranges results in a sensitivity of approximately 50% (assuming identical levels of TRV). Sensitivity may be improved by using a change criterion that is smaller than the minimum change sought, providing the change criterion is still at least as large as the 95% range for TRV, so that specificity is maintained. Reducing TRV allows smaller changes in acuity to be reliably detected.

Visual acuity data are subject to a degree of measurement error. When an individual subject is tested and subsequently retested, the resultant visual acuities tend to differ, even in the absence of any true clinical change. This effect is referred to herein as test-retest variability (TRV). The clinician monitoring a patient over time therefore must disregard apparent changes resulting from TRV, but must recognize any change that reflects a genuine alteration in clinical status. Failure to do the former results in unwanted false-positive results (i.e., loss of specificity), whereas failure to do the latter compromises the test’s sensitivity to change. 
The clinician may draw on experience to make this judgment or may refer to published data on the repeatability and TRV of visual acuity tests to establish a change criterion, beyond which measured change is considered to be genuine and below which it is not. The method of Bland and Altman has been widely used to establish such a change criterion. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 The Bland-Altman approach 9 involves establishing the upper limit of measured change that can be expected when a clinically stable individual undergoes two visual acuity measurements. The 95% range for TRV is generally used to identify the change criterion against which measured differences are judged. The 95% range for TRV is defined as ±1.96 SD of the differences between paired measurements on a group of subjects. Measured changes that lie outside this range are considered to be “real” changes. This approach ensures that, in the absence of true clinical change (and assuming the TRV to be normally distributed), only 5% of measured differences will exceed this level—that is, the approach seeks to fix the specificity of the procedure at 95%. Various 95% ranges have been published, from ±0.07 to ±0.20 log of the minimum angle of resolution (logMAR; see Table 1 ). 
The Bland-Altman method provides the change criterion that is necessary to fix specificity at a level of 95%. However, another important characteristic of a diagnostic test, sensitivity, is not considered. To our knowledge, there has been no investigation of acuity test sensitivity or how sensitivity to change and specificity are influenced by the change criterion used. This may be in part because measuring sensitivity to change of a visual acuity test is difficult, in that there is no independent gold-standard method of establishing whether true clinical change has occurred and, if so, precisely how much. To overcome this problem, we used one of the advantages of contemporary logMAR visual acuity charts: The difficulty of the chart can be adjusted with absolute precision by altering the viewing distance. For example, the 0.7 logMAR letters on a chart viewed at 4 m are precisely equivalent to the 1.0 logMAR letters viewed from 8 m. Hence, although we cannot alter an individual’s acuity by an exact amount, we can alter, with great precision, the difficulty of the task that the test presents. 
The purpose of our study was to determine the level of sensitivity of the ETDRS logMAR chart to simulated change and the specificity achieved when using change criteria derived from published TRV data. Various degrees of acuity change were simulated by altering the difficulty of the test through manipulating the viewing distance. Experimental conditions were designed to avoid any factors that were thought to have the potential to increase TRV, such as uncorrected refractive error, ocular disease, variable lighting conditions, and multiple examiners. 
Materials and Methods
Subjects
Fifty subjects were recruited from the staff of Moorfields Eye Hospital and the students of City University, London. The tenets of the Declaration of Helsinki were adhered to in full. Inclusion criteria were: age less than 50 years; absence of any ocular abnormality, including media opacity; ability to understand and comply with the testing protocol; and Snellen acuity of 6/9 or better. 
One eye of each subject was assessed. When both eyes met the inclusion criteria, the right eye was used as the study eye. All subjects wore full distance refractive correction, as established by formal subjective refraction immediately before the study measurements were taken. 
Equipment
The ETDRS logMAR chart (Lighthouse International, New York, NY) is based on the design suggested by Bailey and Lovie 10 but incorporates the recommendations of the U.S. National Academy of Sciences–National Research Council (NAS-NRC). 11 The chart has been described in detail by Ferris et al. 12  
Design.
The letter stimuli are printed on a translucent panel and lit from behind. The chart has five letters per row ranging in size from +1.0 to −0.30 logMAR at 4 m. There are three versions of the chart, differing only in the letter combinations used. 
Testing Paradigm.
Subjects were required to identify each letter on the chart until they identified a full row of letters incorrectly, at which point the test was terminated, and the acuity calculated. 
Scoring.
Interpolated acuity measurements were derived using the method described by Ferris et al. 12 In this method, visual acuity is scored by the letter rather than by the line, as is common in clinical practice. The resultant reduction in the size of the scale increment has been shown to reduce the level of TRV. 3 6  
Testing Procedure
Each subject underwent acuity measurements at five different distances: 4.0, 4.5, 5.0, 6.3, and 8.0 m. Taking the 4-m distance as the reference, each increase in distance increased the difficulty of the test by a precise amount (Table 2) . The increments in relative difficulty were chosen to cover the range of TRV data reported in the literature. An additional measurement was conducted at the baseline distance of 4 m, to allow the test’s specificity to be calculated. The six measurements were performed in random order, using a randomly selected ETDRS chart, to prevent learning effects from influencing the results. As the six ETDRS measurements were conducted using three charts, the only proviso to this was that the same ETDRS chart not be used for two consecutive measurements. 
Analysis
Categorizing Subjects.
For a given degree of simulated change (or for no simulated change when both measurements were conducted at the same distance), subjects were categorized as having changed in visual acuity if the measured acuity change exceeded the relevant change criterion, otherwise they were categorized as having not changed in acuity. The change criteria investigated were those that could be identified from the published literature (95% ranges for TRV, Table 1 ). 
Sensitivity to Change.
Sensitivity was defined as the percentage of individuals having undergone a simulated change whom the test correctly identified as having a change. 
Specificity.
Specificity was defined as the percentage of individuals who, on the basis of the two test results at 4 m, were not identified as having a change. 
Results
Of the subjects recruited, 15 (30%) were emmetropes, 29 (58%) myopes, and 6 (12%) hypermetropes. The refractive error (spherical equivalent) ranged from −8.00 to +2.00 with a mean of −1.51 D. Eight subjects (16%) had astigmatism of 0.75 D (maximum 1.75 D) or more. 
The distribution of measured acuity change for each degree of simulated change (0, 0.05, 0.10, 0.20, and 0.30) was assessed for normality with the Shapiro-Wilk W-test. There was no evidence (P > 0.05) of departure from a normal distribution for any of the degrees of simulated change. 
To assess the validity of the technique of simulating acuity change through manipulation of the viewing distance, acuities measured at 4.5, 5.0, 6.3, and 8.0 m were subtracted from that measured at the reference distance of 4 m. Table 3 shows the mean differences in measured acuity at each distance, alongside the differences theoretically expected (Table 3) . The 95% confidence interval for the means in the table should be distinguished from the 95% ranges referred to elsewhere in this article and described in the introduction. In contrast to the range of values obtained, the 95% confidence interval is an index of the precision of our estimate of the population mean. The confidence interval allows us to assess whether the measured differences in acuity are compatible with those theoretically expected. The observation that, in each case, the 95% confidence interval includes the expected value (Table 3) indicates that the relationship between measured acuity and viewing distance is compatible with that predicted from theory. 
From the paired measurements taken at 4 m, the 95% range for TRV was calculated to be ±0.11 logMAR according to the Bland-Altman method. These data were added to the list of previously published 95% ranges for TRV in Table 1
Table 4 shows the sensitivities and specificities achieved using the different change criteria. No change criterion achieved sensitivity and specificity in excess of 90% for a simulated acuity change of 0.1 logMAR or less. For larger simulated changes (0.2 or 0.3 logMAR), high levels of sensitivity and specificity were achievable with our TRV (95% range ±0.11 logMAR). 
Discussion
We measured the specificity and sensitivity of the ETDRS chart to various degrees of simulated acuity change. When combined with the Bland-Altman approach to determine a setting-specific change criterion of 0.11 logMAR, the ETDRS performed well at detecting changes of 0.2 logMAR (estimated sensitivity of 100%, 95% confidence interval [CI] 93%–100%; estimated specificity 96%, 95% CI 86%–99.5%). However, for an acuity change of 0.10 logMAR, test sensitivity was poor (38%, 95% CI 25%–53%). Furthermore, this study used normal subjects wearing full refractive correction, and all the measurements were taken by a single examiner in a short time under identical conditions, using an interpolated scoring method. These are all factors that may have helped to limit the degree of TRV in this study, and we may therefore expect the test to perform less well in day-to-day use. Subsequent to the main analysis, the data were rescored using the line-assignment method favored in routine clinical practice (defined as the logMAR value of the smallest letter size at which at least three of five letters were correctly named). Acuities scored this way were subject to greater TRV (95% range ±0.17 logMAR) than those scored with the interpolated method (95% range ±0.11 logMAR). Recalculating sensitivity and specificity for the line-assignment data, using 0.17 logMAR as the change criterion resulted in poorer test performance (estimated sensitivity for a change of 0.2 logMAR of 84%, 95% CI 71%–93%; estimated specificity 94%, 95% CI 83%–99%) than for the interpolated method. It should also be noted that the use of change criteria from published studies rather than an internally derived criterion had, in some instances, a substantial impact on the estimated sensitivity and specificity of the test procedure. 
Our conclusions appear contrary to the those of published studies that have measured 95% ranges for TRV for the ETDRS chart of ±0.10 logMAR or less. 3 4 5 6 These studies have suggested that the ETDRS test can reliably detect changes of 0.10 logMAR or greater. We believe that their interpretation is unduly optimistic. Take the example of a setting in which the 95% TRV range is ±0.10 logMAR, and a change criterion of 0.10 logMAR is therefore set. For individuals with a real change of 0.10 logMAR, the distribution of measured changes, assuming normality, is shown in Figure 1 . It is clear from the data that the change criterion of 0.10 logMAR can be expected to identify 50% of these individuals as having experienced a change. More generally, the sensitivity of the procedure to detect changes of a magnitude similar to the change criterion is approximately 50%. 
In interpreting the results of this study, it should be borne in mind that, according to Bayes’ theorem, the predictive value of a diagnostic test depends the characteristics (sensitivity and specificity) of the test and on the incidence of change in the population to which the test is applied. 13 If the incidence of true change is very low, then even a test with high sensitivity and specificity may result in a high proportion of false-positive test results. For example, if a test with 95% sensitivity and 95% specificity is applied to a population in which the proportion of tested individuals with change is 5%, the positive predictive value (PPV) is 0.50. In other words, only half of the positive results are genuine changes. If the proportion of tested individuals with changes increases to 50%, the same test has a PPV of 0.95—that is, 95% of positive results are true positives. 
Summary
The degree of visual acuity change that may be reliably detected using the ETDRS chart may be not be as small as previously thought. This is because the usual method of establishing a change criterion (using Bland-Altman 95% ranges for TTV 4 6 7 14 ) measures the size of change required to fix specificity at 95%, but does not consider sensitivity. Our results suggest that using the 95% TRV range as the change criterion results in a low level of sensitivity. Sensitivity may be improved by using a change criterion that is smaller than the minimum change sought, providing the change criterion is still at least as large as the 95% range for TRV, so that specificity is maintained. Reducing TRV allows smaller changes in acuity to be reliably detected. This can be achieved clinically by reducing the size of the test’s scale increment. 1 4 6  
Table 1.
 
Published 95% Ranges for TRV
Table 1.
 
Published 95% Ranges for TRV
Author TRV*
Elliot and Sheridan 5 , Vanden-Bosch and Wall 3 ± 0.07
Arditi and Cagenello 4 ± 0.09
Bailey et al. 6 ± 0.10
Present study ± 0.11
Lovie-Kitchin 15 ± 0.16
Rosser et al. 16 ± 0.18
Reeves et al. 7 ± 0.19
Table 2.
 
Relative Increase in Task Difficulty with Increasing Viewing Distance
Table 2.
 
Relative Increase in Task Difficulty with Increasing Viewing Distance
Viewing Distance (m) Relative Difficulty (logMAR)
4.0 Reference
4.5 +0.05
5.0 +0.10
6.3 +0.20
8.0 +0.30
Table 3.
 
Viewing Distance and Measured Acuity
Table 3.
 
Viewing Distance and Measured Acuity
Viewing Distance (m) Expected Difference (logMAR) Measured Difference (logMAR) 95% CI (mean logMAR)
4.5 −0.05 −0.059 −0.079 to −0.039
5.0 −0.10 −0.087 −0.108 to −0.066
6.3 −0.20 −0.196 −0.209 to −0.183
8.0 −0.30 −0.305 −0.322 to −0.288
Table 4.
 
Specificity and Sensitivity for Each Degree of Simulated Change Using Change Criteria Derived from Published 95% Confidence Ranges for TRV
Table 4.
 
Specificity and Sensitivity for Each Degree of Simulated Change Using Change Criteria Derived from Published 95% Confidence Ranges for TRV
Change Criterion Sensitivity (0.05 change) Sensitivity (0.10 change) Sensitivity (0.20 change) Sensitivity (0.30 change) Specificity (no change)
0.07 3 5 58 (43–72) 60 (45–74) 100 (93–100)* 100 (93–100)* 84 (71–93)
0.09 4 44 (30–59) 50 (36–65) 100 (93–100)* 100 (93–100)* 90 (78–97)
0.10 6 18 (9–31) 38 (25–53) 100 (93–100)* 100 (93–100)* 90 (78–97)
0.11, † 18 (9–31) 38 (25–53) 100 (93–100)* 100 (93–100)* 96 (86–100)
0.16 15 4 (0–14) 12 (5–24) 66 (53–79) 100 (93–100)* 100 (93–100)*
0.18 16 2 (0–11) 4 (0–14) 54 (40–68) 98 (89–100) 100 (93–100)*
0.19 7 2 (0–11) 4 (0–14) 54 (40–68) 98 (89–100) 100 (93–100)*
Figure 1.
 
Expected distribution of measured changes in individuals who have a true change of 0.1 logMAR, with a 95% TRV range of 0.1 logMAR.
Figure 1.
 
Expected distribution of measured changes in individuals who have a true change of 0.1 logMAR, with a 95% TRV range of 0.1 logMAR.
 
Raasch, TW, Bailey, IL, Bullimore, MA. (1998) Repeatability of visual acuity measurement Optom Vis Sci 75,342-348 [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Brown, B, Lovie Kitchin, J. (1993) Repeated visual acuity measurement: establishing the patient’s own criterion for change Optom Vis Sci 70,45-53 [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Vanden Bosch, ME, Wall, M. (1997) Visual acuity scored by the letter-by-letter or probit methods has lower retest variability than the line assignment method Eye 11,411-417 [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Arditi, A, Cagenello, R. (1993) On the statistical reliability of letter-chart visual acuity measurements Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci 34,120-129 [PubMed]
Elliott, DB, Sheridan, M. (1988) The use of accurate visual acuity measurements in clinical anti-cataract formulation trials Ophthalmic Physiol Opt 8,397-401 [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Bailey, IL, Bullimore, MA, Raasch, TW, Taylor, HR. (1991) Clinical grading and the effects of scaling Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci 32,422-432 [PubMed]
Reeves, B, Wood, J, Hill, A. (1991) Vistech VCTS 6500 charts, within- and between-session reliability Optom Vis Sci 68,728-737 [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Reeves, BC, Hill, AR, Aspinall, PA. (1987) The clinical significance of change Ophthalmic Physiol Opt 7,441-446 [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Bland, J, Altman, DG. (1986) Statistical methods for assessing agreement between two methods of clinical measurement Lancet 1(8476),307-310 [PubMed]
Bailey, I, Lovie, J. (1976) New design principles for visual acuity letter charts Am J Optom Physiol Opt 53,740-745 [CrossRef] [PubMed]
. National Academy of Sciences-National Research Council Committee on Vision. (1980) Report of working group 39: Recommended standard procedures for the clinical measurement and specification of visual acuity Adv Ophthalmol 41,103-148 [PubMed]
Ferris, FLd, Kassoff, A, Bresnick, GH, Bailey, I. (1982) New visual acuity charts for clinical research Am J Ophthalmol 94,91-96 [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Altman, DG. (1991) Diagnostic tests Practical Statistics for Medical Research ,409-419 Chapman and Hall London.
Reeves, BC, Wood, JM, Hill, AR. (1993) Reliability of high- and low-contrast letter charts Ophthalmic Physiol Opt 13,17-26 [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Lovie Kitchin, JE. (1988) Validity and reliability of visual acuity measurements Ophthalmic Physiol Opt 8,363-370 [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Rosser, DA, Laidlaw, DA, Murdoch, IE. (2001) The development of a “reduced logMAR” visual acuity chart for use in routine clinical practice Br J Ophthalmol 85,432-436 [CrossRef] [PubMed]
×
×

This PDF is available to Subscribers Only

Sign in or purchase a subscription to access this content. ×

You must be signed into an individual account to use this feature.

×