Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science Cover Image for Volume 54, Issue 10
October 2013
Volume 54, Issue 10
Free
Eye Movements, Strabismus, Amblyopia and Neuro-ophthalmology  |   October 2013
Short-Term Saccadic Adaptation in Patients With Anisometropic Amblyopia
Author Affiliations & Notes
  • Rana Arham Raashid
    Program in Neuroscience and Mental Health, The Hospital for Sick Children, Toronto, Ontario, Canada
  • Agnes M. F. Wong
    Program in Neuroscience and Mental Health, The Hospital for Sick Children, Toronto, Ontario, Canada
  • Manokaraananthan Chandrakumar
    Program in Neuroscience and Mental Health, The Hospital for Sick Children, Toronto, Ontario, Canada
  • Alan Blakeman
    Department of Ophthalmology and Vision Sciences, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada
  • Herbert C. Goltz
    Program in Neuroscience and Mental Health, The Hospital for Sick Children, Toronto, Ontario, Canada
    Department of Ophthalmology and Vision Sciences, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada
  • Correspondence: Agnes M. F. Wong, Department of Ophthalmology and Vision Sciences, The Hospital for Sick Children, 555 University Avenue, Toronto, Ontario, Canada M5G 1X8; [email protected]
Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science October 2013, Vol.54, 6701-6711. doi:https://doi.org/10.1167/iovs.13-12553
  • Views
  • PDF
  • Share
  • Tools
    • Alerts
      ×
      This feature is available to authenticated users only.
      Sign In or Create an Account ×
    • Get Citation

      Rana Arham Raashid, Agnes M. F. Wong, Manokaraananthan Chandrakumar, Alan Blakeman, Herbert C. Goltz; Short-Term Saccadic Adaptation in Patients With Anisometropic Amblyopia. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 2013;54(10):6701-6711. https://doi.org/10.1167/iovs.13-12553.

      Download citation file:


      © ARVO (1962-2015); The Authors (2016-present)

      ×
  • Supplements
Abstract

Purpose.: Amblyopia is a developmental disorder characterized by impairment of spatiotemporal visual processing that also affects oculomotor and manual motor function. We investigated the effects of amblyopia on short-term visuomotor adaptation using a saccadic adaptation paradigm.

Methods.: A total of 8 patients with anisometropic amblyopia and 11 visually-normal controls participated. Saccadic adaptation was induced using a double-step paradigm that displaced a saccadic visual target (at ±19°) back toward central fixation by 4.2° during the ongoing saccade. Three test blocks, preadaptation, adaptation, and postadaptation, were performed sequentially while participants viewed binocularly and monocularly with the amblyopic and fellow eyes (nondominant and dominant eyes in controls) in three separate sessions. The spatial and temporal characteristics of saccadic adaptation were measured.

Results.: Patients exhibited diminished saccadic gain adaptation. The percentage change in saccadic gain was lower in patients during amblyopic eye and binocular viewing compared to controls. Saccadic latencies were longer, and saccadic gains and latencies were more variable in patients during amblyopic eye viewing. The time constants of adaptation were comparable between controls and patients under all viewing conditions.

Conclusions.: The short-term adaptation of saccadic gain was weaker and more variable in patients during amblyopic eye and binocular viewing. Our findings suggest that visual error information necessary for adaptation is imprecise in amblyopia, leading to reduced modulation of saccadic gain, and support the proposal that the error signal driving saccadic adaptation is visual.

Introduction
Saccades are rapid eye movements that bring the image of a target onto the fovea. Because visual resolution is maximized near the center of the fovea, 1 it is crucial that saccades remain accurate to enable high acuity vision. The accuracy of goal-directed saccades is defined by their gain (the ratio of saccadic amplitude to target amplitude), which typically is 0.90 to 0.95 for healthy individuals. 2,3 Because most saccades typically last less than 80 ms, the classic view was that visual feedback mechanisms did not have an important role in maintaining their accuracy. 4,5 However, saccades are not ballistic movements; there now is ample evidence that internal feedback processes monitor the efference copy of the ongoing saccadic motor commands to maintain movement accuracy. 6 More recent studies suggest that visual feedback can modify the in-flight saccadic trajectory. 7,8  
Saccadic accuracy is maintained by a sensorimotor adaptive mechanism known as saccadic adaptation, 9,10 which adjusts saccadic gain by modifying motor commands iteratively in the face of changing neural and mechanical oculomotor system properties due to aging and disease. 11 14 Accurate gain recalibration is achieved by using the spatial error information available at the end of primary saccades to modify the motor command issued for subsequent saccades, until the postsaccadic movement error is minimized or eliminated effectively. Much evidence suggests that the error signal driving saccadic adaptation is visual, 15 17 and is derived most likely from comparing the estimated postsaccadic retinal error to the actual postsaccadic retinal error. 18 22  
Amblyopia is a visual disorder that results from abnormal visual stimulation during early childhood without any structural eye abnormality, and it cannot be corrected optically. 23 Patients with amblyopia are known to have multiple sensory deficits, 24 28 including impaired spatial localization 25 and positional certainty. 26,29,30 We have shown that amblyopia also affects visuomotor functions. For example, patients with amblyopia have impaired planning and execution of visually-guided limb reaching movements, 31,32 altered temporal patterns of eye–hand coordination, 33 and diminished online control of 3D reaching movements. 34 These impairments also extend to oculomotor control, with patients exhibiting reduced saccadic spatial precision 35 and fixation instability. 36 38 However, to our knowledge no study has investigated the sensorimotor adaptation of saccadic gain in response to visual errors in patients with amblyopia. 
In our study, we investigated the effects of anisometropic amblyopia on the short-term adaptation of visually-guided saccades. It is known that the consistency or precision of postsaccadic visual error influences saccadic adaptation; that is, adaptation is strongest when the error is consistent and becomes weaker as the error becomes more variable. 21 We hypothesized that patients with anisometropic amblyopia will exhibit diminished saccadic gain adaptation as a result of decreased spatial precision of the visual error signal. We also hypothesized that patients will exhibit a higher variability in saccadic gain adaptation caused by increased spatial uncertainty in amblyopia. We found that patients with anisometropic amblyopia exhibit a diminished ability to modulate saccadic gain in response to target steps, and their gain also was more variable. 
Methods
Participants
A total of 8 patients with anisometropic amblyopia and 11 controls with normal or corrected-to-normal vision participated. Clinical characteristics are shown in Tables 1 and 2. Amblyopia was defined as visual acuity of 0.2 logMAR or worse in the amblyopic eye, 0.0 logMAR or better in the fellow eye, and an interocular visual acuity difference of ≥0.2 logMAR. Anisometropic amblyopia was defined as amblyopia in the presence of an interocular refractive error difference of ≥1 diopter (D) of spherical or cylindrical power. All anisometropic patients except patient 1 had a microtropia of ≤8 prism diopters (due to a foveal scotoma arising from the anisometropia) as detected using the simultaneous prism and cover test. Patient 1 had a phoria, but no visible manifest deviation on the cover test. Six patients (patients 1, 2, 4, and 6–8) met the criteria for monofixation syndrome. 39 The other two patients (patients 3 and 5) did not have stereopsis, but showed peripheral fusion with a central suppression scotoma. Five patients had moderate amblyopia (visual acuity of 0.3–0.7 logMAR in the amblyopic eye 40 ) and three patients had severe amblyopia (visual acuity of 0.8–1.3 logMAR in the amblyopic eye 41 ). Eye dominance for visually-normal observers was determined using the Dolman method. 42 The participant was instructed to view a distant object with both eyes, looking through a tube held at arms' length bimanually. The observer then alternately closed one eye to determine which eye was dominant. Exclusion criteria were any ocular cause for reduced visual acuity, prior intraocular surgery, or any neurologic disease. All participants provided written consent before participation. The study was approved by the Research Ethics Board at The Hospital For Sick Children and all experimental protocols conformed to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki. 
Table 1
 
Clinical Characteristics of Patients With Anisometropic Amblyopia
Table 1
 
Clinical Characteristics of Patients With Anisometropic Amblyopia
I.D. Age Sex Snellen Visual Acuity (LogMAR) Refractive Error Deviation, Δ* Sensory Fusion Stereoacuity, s of arc
Right Eye Left Eye Right Eye Left Eye Near, 0.33 m Distance, 6 m Worth 4 Dot Bagolini
1 25 F 20/20 (0.00) 20/50 (0.40) −1.50 +1.50 × 80 −3.00 +2.50 × 80 E4 E1 Fused N/A 120
2 18 F 20/20 (0.00) 20/60 (0.48) −1.50 +0.50 × 80 +1.00 +1.25 × 95 LET2 LET2 Fused N/A 200
3 30 M 20/15 (−0.10) 20/200 (1.00) +4.00 +6.00 +1.75 × 90 LXT4 LXT8 Fused N/A Negative
4 44 F 20/200 (1.00) 20/15 (−0.10) +4.50 Plano RET2 RET2 Fused N/A 3000
5 21 F 20/60 (0.48) 20/15 (−0.10) −11.25 −3.00 +0.75 × 15 RET2, ET + E4‡ RET2, ET + E4‡ Suppress Fused Negative
6 25 F 20/15 (−0.10) 20/80 (0.60) +2.25 +1.00 LET2 LET2 Fused N/A 100
7 21 F 20/20 (0.00) 20/125 (0.80) Plano +2.00 LXT4 LXT2 Fused N/A 3000
8 27 F 20/20 (0.00) 20/40 (0.30) +0.25 +2.75 LET1, ET + X4‡ LET1 Fused N/A 140
Table 2
 
Clinical Characteristics of Visually-Normal Participants
Table 2
 
Clinical Characteristics of Visually-Normal Participants
I.D. Age Sex Snellen Visual Acuity (LogMAR) Refractive Error Stereoacuity, s of arc Dominant Eye
Right Eye Left Eye Right Eye Left Eye
1 42 F 20/20 (0.00) 20/20 (0.00) −3.25 −3.25 40 Right
2 34 F 20/20 (0.00) 20/20 (0.00) −1.50 +0.25 × 85 −1.50 +0.25 × 75 40 Right
3 24 M 20/15 (−0.10) 20/15 (−0.10) Plano Plano 30 Left
4 23 M 20/20 (0.00) 20/20 (0.00) −4.50 +2.50 × 98 −5.00 +3.50 × 78 40 Right
5 29 M 20/15 (−0.10) 20/15 (−0.10) −2.00 −1.25 40 Right
6 23 F 20/20 (0.00) 20/15 (−0.10) −2.50 −2.50 +0.50 × 100 40 Right
7 20 M 20/20 (0.00) 20/15 (−0.10) Plano Plano 40 Right
8 21 F 20/20 (0.00) 20/15 (−0.10) Plano Plano 40 Right
9 23 F 20/20 (0.00) 20/15 (−0.10) −1.50 +0.25 × 90 −1.00 +0.25 × 90 40 Right
10 24 M 20/20 (0.00) 20/20 (0.00) Plano Plano 40 Right
11 18 F 20/20 (0.00) 20/20 (0.00) Plano Plano 40 Right
Apparatus
The visual target was a red laser dot (≈0.2° diameter) rear-projected onto a translucent screen using a galvanometer (GSI Group, Bedford, MA). The experiments were conducted in a dimly-lit room with the participants seated 80 cm from the screen and their head stabilized on a chin rest. Eye movements were recorded using a video-based eye tracker (Chronos Vision GmbH, Berlin, Germany) at 200 Hz, with a spatial resolution of 0.1°. 
Experimental Procedure
All participants were corrected to best-corrected visual acuity using contact lenses for the duration of the experiment. We did not record from any participants who were wearing spectacles. Participants were instructed to follow the visual target as quickly and as accurately as possible. A single experimental session (350 trials) comprised three test blocks: preadaptation, adaptation, and postadaptation blocks performed sequentially. Participants took a 1-minute break between blocks to minimize fatigue. 
Pre- and Postadaptation Blocks.
Target steps were presented randomly at ±19° horizontal positions for 800 ms, after an initial fixation period of 750 to 1250 ms. The preadaptation block consisted of 50 trials (30 in the adapting and 20 in the nonadapting direction), while the postadaptation block consisted of 100 trials (65 in the adapting and 35 in the nonadapting direction). 
Adaptation Block.
Saccadic adaptation was induced using a variant of the double-step target paradigm (Fig. 1). 43 Target presentation was similar to the preadaptation block, except that once the eyes started moving, the target (at ±19°) was shifted back toward central fixation by 4.2°. This intrasaccadic target step was triggered once the online eye velocity exceeded 50°/s, and was presented for saccades in one horizontal direction only (the adapting direction), randomized between participants. Vertical catch trials (±19°) also were included to minimize anticipatory eye movements and inattention. 44 The entire adaptation block consisted of 200 trials (120 in the adapting and 60 in the nonadapting direction, plus 20 vertical catch trials interleaved in a pseudorandom order). 
Figure 1
 
(A) The double-step target paradigm: participants fixated a central visual target (F). After a random interval of 750 to 1250 ms, the target stepped to 19° (T1, rightwards in this example). As soon as the primary saccade eye velocity exceeded 50°/s, the target stepped 4.2° back toward initial fixation (T2) before returning to central fixation. Participants were instructed to follow the target as quickly and as accurately as possible. The right panel shows the amplitude versus time graphs for target and eye position for single trials at the beginning (B) and near the end of the adaptation block (C). As adaptation neared completion, the primary saccade brought the eyes to the final target position.
Figure 1
 
(A) The double-step target paradigm: participants fixated a central visual target (F). After a random interval of 750 to 1250 ms, the target stepped to 19° (T1, rightwards in this example). As soon as the primary saccade eye velocity exceeded 50°/s, the target stepped 4.2° back toward initial fixation (T2) before returning to central fixation. Participants were instructed to follow the target as quickly and as accurately as possible. The right panel shows the amplitude versus time graphs for target and eye position for single trials at the beginning (B) and near the end of the adaptation block (C). As adaptation neared completion, the primary saccade brought the eyes to the final target position.
Individual experimental sessions were performed under three viewing conditions in the following order: amblyopic eye (AE) viewing (controls, nondominant eye [NDE]), binocular viewing (BE), and fellow eye (FE) viewing (controls, dominant eye [DE]). Each viewing condition was tested on a separate day at least one week apart to prevent any long-term retention of the adapted saccadic gain from previous training. 45,46  
Data Analysis
Real-time eye position data were differentiated using a five-point quadratic Savitzky-Golay smoothing filter, 47 yielding an online eye velocity profile. Saccades were detected offline using a velocity threshold of 20°/s, and were omitted from analysis if they did not reach the threshold peak velocity of >100°/s, had a latency of <100 ms and/or >500 ms, or were contaminated by eye blinks or exhibited an atypical saccadic profile (e.g., glissades, staircase saccades). 
There were five outcome measures: percentage change in saccadic gain, saccadic gain recovery percentage, saccadic gain variability, saccadic latency, and saccadic latency variability. For the first three measures, mean values for the preadaptation, adaptation, and postadaptation blocks were calculated using the last 25, 30, and 20 trials in the adapting direction of these blocks, respectively. Mean latency and latency variability (the last two measures) for the three experimental blocks were calculated using all the trials in the adapting direction. The percentage change in saccadic gain following adaptation was calculated using the following formulas:      
Actual change in saccadic gain is more sensitive than computing the difference in saccadic gain before and after adaptation, as it accounts for normal saccadic undershooting during preadaptation. 2,3 The calculated percentage value of 0% indicates no adaptation, whereas 100% reflects complete adaptation. The saccadic gain recovery percentage was calculated using the following formula:    
To quantify the time course of adaptation, saccadic gain was modeled to decrease exponentially as a function of increasing number of trials. Individual data were fitted by a nonlinear regression equationDisplay Formula Image not available , where G(t) is the saccadic gain at a given trial t, G 0 is the steady-state gain value (reached at the end of adaptation), ΔG is the change in gain from baseline to steady-state value, and τ is the time constant of adaptation (number of trials). Goodness of fit (R 2) values were computed for each fit.  
Statistical analyses were performed using SAS 9.2. A two-way mixed ANOVA assessed the effects of amblyopia on the percentage change in saccadic gain and percentage of saccadic gain recovery using Group (controls and patients) as the between-subjects factor and Viewing Condition (AE/NDE, BE, and FE/DE) as the within-subjects repeated factor. All other measures were submitted to a three-way mixed ANOVA with Group as the between-subjects factor, and two within-subjects repeated factors: Viewing Condition and Experimental Block (preadaptation, adaptation, and postadaptation). All significant main effects and interactions were tested using post hoc Student's t‐tests for pairwise multiple comparisons. To analyze the temporal characteristics of adaptation, an independent samples t-test was carried out for each viewing condition separately to assess the difference between the time constant and goodness of fit values from patients and controls. Conservative Greenhouse-Geisser corrected P values along with the partial η 2 (η 2 partial ) values are reported for all ANOVA results. 
Preliminary analysis found no difference between the percentage change in gain for saccades adapted in the rightward and leftward direction for either group (F(2,29) = 1.57, P = 0.23). Therefore, adaptation data from either direction were pooled (within each group) for all subsequent analyses. Similarly, data from both eyes were comparable during binocular viewing (F(1,16) = 0.8, P = 0.45). There also was no difference in percentage change in gain obtained from the amblyopic eye as compared to the fellow eye during binocular viewing (t (7) = −0.18, P = 0.86). Therefore, only right eye data were reported (except for control patient 9 and patient 4, whose right eye data were noisy, so left eye data were reported instead). 
Results
Saccadic Gain
Figure 2 depicts saccadic gain changes for a representative control participant (control 6) and patient (patient 4). In general, controls reduced their saccadic gain rapidly during adaptation until reaching a steady state, followed by recovery to baseline gain during postadaptation. Patients with amblyopia responded similarly, but their gain decreased to a lesser extent and depended on the specific viewing condition. 
Figure 2
 
Changes in saccadic gain across all three experimental blocks, shown for control participant 6 (left column) and patient 4 (right column) during nondominant/amblyopic eye (top row), binocular (middle row), and dominant/fellow eye (bottom row) viewing conditions. The dotted vertical lines mark the end of the preadaptation blocks and solid vertical lines mark the end of the adaptation blocks. The control participant showed a consistently high change in saccadic gain during adaptation across all three viewing conditions, while the patient exhibited reduced and more variable change in saccadic gain during adaptation during amblyopic and binocular viewing. The preadaptation blocks were fit with a horizontal line indicating the average gain over that block; the adaptation blocks were fit with the decreasing exponential equation, G(t) = G 0 + Δ Ge−t/τ , and the postadaptation blocks were fit with the increasing exponential equation, G(t) = G 0 + Δ G(1 − e−t/τ ).
Figure 2
 
Changes in saccadic gain across all three experimental blocks, shown for control participant 6 (left column) and patient 4 (right column) during nondominant/amblyopic eye (top row), binocular (middle row), and dominant/fellow eye (bottom row) viewing conditions. The dotted vertical lines mark the end of the preadaptation blocks and solid vertical lines mark the end of the adaptation blocks. The control participant showed a consistently high change in saccadic gain during adaptation across all three viewing conditions, while the patient exhibited reduced and more variable change in saccadic gain during adaptation during amblyopic and binocular viewing. The preadaptation blocks were fit with a horizontal line indicating the average gain over that block; the adaptation blocks were fit with the decreasing exponential equation, G(t) = G 0 + Δ Ge−t/τ , and the postadaptation blocks were fit with the increasing exponential equation, G(t) = G 0 + Δ G(1 − e−t/τ ).
Percentage Change in Saccadic Gain.
Figure 3 shows the percentage change in saccadic gain across all three viewing conditions. A main effect was found for Group (F(1,17) = 19.3, P = 0.0004, η 2 partial = 0.532), where the saccadic gain reduction in patients (54.2 ± 15.7%) was less than in controls (71.9 ± 12.9%). A significant interaction between Group and Viewing Condition also was observed (F(2,34) = 6.0, P = 0.006, η 2 partial = 0.260; Fig. 3). Post hoc analysis revealed that the percentage change in saccadic gain was significantly lower in patients during amblyopic eye (44.6 ± 10.8%) and binocular (53.2 ± 16.7%) viewing when compared to controls (nondominant eye, 71.3 ± 10.7%; binocular, 77.7 ± 11.5%). However, with fellow eye viewing, the saccadic gain change percentage in patients (64.6 ± 13.8%) was similar to controls during dominant eye viewing (66.7 ± 14.7%). The within-group analysis indicated that controls attained the highest percentage change in saccadic gain when viewing binocularly (i.e., binocular advantage). In contrast, patients exhibited a significantly lower percentage change in saccadic gain during amblyopic eye compared to fellow eye viewing, but not binocular viewing. In agreement with adaptation values reported previously, 9,48 52 saccadic gain adaptation did not reach 100% even in normal participants; their gain decreased by approximately 75% of the ideal gain change at the end of adaptation. 
Figure 3
 
Mean percentage change in saccadic gain for controls and patients for the three viewing conditions. Error bars represent SEM. Patients showed a reduced percentage change in saccadic gain compared to controls during amblyopic eye and binocular condition. *P < 0.05.
Figure 3
 
Mean percentage change in saccadic gain for controls and patients for the three viewing conditions. Error bars represent SEM. Patients showed a reduced percentage change in saccadic gain compared to controls during amblyopic eye and binocular condition. *P < 0.05.
To assess the effect of visual acuity deficits on saccadic adaptation, a Pearson's product-moment correlation was performed between the visual acuity of the amblyopic eye and the percentage change in saccadic gain during amblyopic eye viewing. A correlation coefficient (r) of −0.405 (P = 0.32) was found, indicating no significant relation between visual acuity and percentage change in saccadic gain. 
Percentage of Saccadic Gain Recovery.
The saccadic gain recovery percentage was comparable between controls (94.2 ± 3.3%) and patients (95.8 ± 3.4%) across all viewing conditions (F(1,17) = 3.0, P = 0.10, η 2 partial = 0.150). 
Saccadic Gain Variability.
Variability was defined as the standard deviation of the mean saccadic gain. A main effect for Group was observed (F(1,17) = 10.6, P = 0.005, η 2 partial = 0.384). Saccadic gains were more variable for patients (0.055 ± 0.01) compared to controls (0.044 ± 0.01). A significant main effect for Viewing Condition also was found (F(2,34) = 5.4, P = 0.009, η 2 partial = 0.243). Saccadic gain variability was higher when all participants viewed with the amblyopic/nondominant eye (0.053 ± 0.02), compared to binocular (0.046 ± 0.01) and fellow/dominant eye (0.049 ± 0.01) viewing. 
Saccadic Latency
A significant main effect for Experimental Block (F(2,34) = 3.2, P = 0.05, η 2 partial = 0.160) was found, with the post hoc tests indicating that mean saccadic latencies were significantly longer for all participants during the adaptation block (205 ± 26 ms) compared to the preadaptation block (197 ± 25 ms). A significant interaction between Group and Viewing Condition also was observed (F(1.41,23.92) = 3.8, P = 0.049, η 2 partial = 0.184, Fig. 4A). Patients exhibited increased mean saccadic latencies during amblyopic eye viewing (228 ± 34 ms) compared to controls during nondominant eye viewing (199 ± 25 ms). Within the patient group, mean saccadic latencies were significantly longer during amblyopic eye viewing compared to fellow eye (212 ± 24 ms) and binocular (187 ± 15 ms) viewing. In contrast, controls had significantly shorter latencies when viewing binocularly (185 ± 15 ms) compared to monocular viewing (dominant, 201 ± 21 ms; nondominant, 199 ± 25 ms). 
Figure 4
 
(A) Mean saccadic latency shown for controls and patients for all three viewing conditions, averaged over all experimental blocks. (B) Saccadic latency variability shown for controls and patients for all three viewing conditions, averaged across all experimental blocks. Error bars represent SEM. Patients exhibited longer and more variable saccadic latencies when viewing with the amblyopic eye compared to the nondominant eye of controls. *P < 0.05.
Figure 4
 
(A) Mean saccadic latency shown for controls and patients for all three viewing conditions, averaged over all experimental blocks. (B) Saccadic latency variability shown for controls and patients for all three viewing conditions, averaged across all experimental blocks. Error bars represent SEM. Patients exhibited longer and more variable saccadic latencies when viewing with the amblyopic eye compared to the nondominant eye of controls. *P < 0.05.
Saccadic Latency Variability.
There was a main effect for Experimental Block (F(2,34) = 8.8, P = 0.0008, η 2 partial = 0.342), with the post hoc tests revealing that saccadic latencies were less variable during preadaptation (31 ± 9 ms) compared to adaptation (38 ± 12 ms) or postadaptation (36 ± 11 ms). A significant interaction also was observed between Group and Viewing Condition (F(2,34) = 3.7, P = 0.036, η 2 partial = 0.178, Fig. 4B). Patients exhibited higher saccadic latency variability during amblyopic eye viewing (42 ± 14 ms) compared to controls during nondominant eye viewing (33 ± 9 ms). 
Time Course Analysis
Participants' data were fitted reliably using the exponential function, with the exception of a few individuals whose gain decreases were better described by a nonexponential fit in some viewing conditions. Table 3 summarizes the temporal course of saccadic gain changes during adaptation. During amblyopic eye viewing, five of eight patients' data were fit with a robust exponential adaptation curve (Fig. 5); however, their goodness of fit (R 2) values (0.21 ± 0.05) were lower than those of controls (0.33 ± 0.11, t (12) = 2.3, P = 0.04). During binocular viewing, five patients showed exponential time courses, and during fellow eye viewing six patients had exponential time courses. In contrast to the amblyopic eye viewing, the R 2 values were comparable between controls and patients during binocular (t (14) = −0.19, P = 0.86) and fellow eye (t (13) = −0.60, P = 0.56, Fig. 6A) viewing. Patients decreased their saccadic gain, with mean time constants comparable to control time constants during all 3 viewing conditions: amblyopic eye (control = 20 ± 16, patients = 19 ± 6 trials; t (12) = 0.25; P = 0.81), binocular (control = 22 ± 14, patients = 27 ± 7 trials; t (14) = −0.7; P = 0.50), and fellow eye (control = 23 ± 15, patients = 31 ± 18 trials; t (13) = −1.0; P = 0.34; Fig. 6B) viewing. 
Figure 5
 
The time course of changes in saccadic gain for all patients during amblyopic eye viewing. The adaptation trials were fit by the decreasing exponential equation, G(t) = G 0 + ΔGe −t/τ . It is noteworthy that patients 1, 5, and 8 did not exhibit an exponential change in the adapting saccadic gain.
Figure 5
 
The time course of changes in saccadic gain for all patients during amblyopic eye viewing. The adaptation trials were fit by the decreasing exponential equation, G(t) = G 0 + ΔGe −t/τ . It is noteworthy that patients 1, 5, and 8 did not exhibit an exponential change in the adapting saccadic gain.
Figure 6
 
(A) Mean goodness of fit (R 2) values shown for binocular (ncontrols = 11, npatients = 5), dominant/fellow eye (ncontrols = 9, npatients = 6), and nondominant/amblyopic eye viewing conditions (ncontrols = 9, npatients = 5). (B) Mean time constants (τ) shown for controls and patients for all three viewing conditions. Error bars represent SEM. Patients exhibited weaker R 2 values when viewing with the amblyopic eye, but comparable time constants of adaptation under all viewing conditions. *P < 0.05.
Figure 6
 
(A) Mean goodness of fit (R 2) values shown for binocular (ncontrols = 11, npatients = 5), dominant/fellow eye (ncontrols = 9, npatients = 6), and nondominant/amblyopic eye viewing conditions (ncontrols = 9, npatients = 5). (B) Mean time constants (τ) shown for controls and patients for all three viewing conditions. Error bars represent SEM. Patients exhibited weaker R 2 values when viewing with the amblyopic eye, but comparable time constants of adaptation under all viewing conditions. *P < 0.05.
Table 3
 
The Time Constants (τ) and Goodness of Fit (R 2) Values for All Participants in Control and Patient Groups, for NDE/AE, BE, and DE/FE Viewing Conditions
Table 3
 
The Time Constants (τ) and Goodness of Fit (R 2) Values for All Participants in Control and Patient Groups, for NDE/AE, BE, and DE/FE Viewing Conditions
Group I.D. NDE/AE BE DE/FE
τ, n of Trials R 2 τ, n of Trials R 2 τ, n of Trials R 2
Control 1 9 0.318 12 0.377 6 0.227
2 8 0.299 31 0.475 14 0.360
3 N/A N/A 39 0.370 N/A N/A
4 36 0.503 38 0.296 16 0.163
5 28 0.198 4 0.126 N/A N/A
6 17 0.298 29 0.381 20 0.226
7 N/A N/A 10 0.133 50 0.413
8 21 0.437 37 0.486 25 0.278
9 10 0.284 16 0.228 31 0.319
10 4 0.169 25 0.327 35 0.333
11 52 0.423 3 0.187 5 0.162
Mean ± SD 20 ± 16 0.33 ± 0.11* 22 ± 14 0.31 ± 0.13 23 ± 15 0.28 ± 0.09
Patient 1 N/A N/A 23 0.471 25 0.412
2 28 0.210 38 0.239 12 0.174
3 20 0.140 24 0.375 34 0.517
4 17 0.259 25 0.266 39 0.236
5 N/A N/A N/A N/A 60 0.414
6 12 0.170 N/A N/A 15 0.127
7 16 0.246 N/A N/A N/A N/A
8 N/A N/A 24 0.248 N/A N/A
Mean ± SD 19 ± 6 0.21 ± 0.05* 27 ± 7 0.32 ± 0.10 31 ± 18 0.31 ± 0.16
Discussion
We investigated short-term saccadic gain adaptation in patients with anisometropic amblyopia. We found that, when viewing with the amblyopic eye and binocularly, patients with amblyopia exhibited a lower percentage change in saccadic gain compared to controls. The adapted saccadic gain also was more variable in patients across all viewing conditions. The mean time constant of adaptation in patients was comparable to controls in all conditions; however, patients exhibited weaker R 2 values for their exponential fits when viewing with the amblyopic eye. Patients had longer and more variable saccadic latencies during adaptation with amblyopic eye viewing. 
Decreased Modulation of the Saccadic Gain During Adaptation
Patients with amblyopia exhibited a reduced percentage change in saccadic gain whenever the amblyopic eye was involved in viewing (i.e., during amblyopic eye and binocular viewing). Havermann and Lappe recently showed that the consistency of postsaccadic visual error influences saccadic adaptation, and adaptation is strongest when the error is consistent and becomes weaker as the error becomes more variable. 21 It is possible that the decrease in saccadic adaptation we observed may be due to imprecise postsaccadic visual error signals resulting from increased visual noise 29 and spatial uncertainty 25,26 in amblyopia. This is supported by findings in our previous 35 and current studies that the gain of primary saccades is significantly more variable in patients, leading to inconsistent postsaccadic visual error for adaptation. 
The cerebellum is critical for motor learning, including short-term saccadic adaptation. 53,54 The cerebellum-mediated short-term saccadic adaptation has two components. 55 First, the initial motor command for subsequent saccades is modified offline or before saccade execution, accounting for the majority of the total adaptation response. Second, internal feedback of the initial motor command is sent to the cerebellum (via efference copy), allowing the saccadic system to make online modifications that correct for any errors in the saccadic trajectory during execution. This latter component of adaptive gain modulation occurs later in the saccadic trajectory, accounting for the rest of the adaptation response. The reduced saccadic gain adaptation in patients with amblyopia can be explained in this dual-component framework. First, the initial saccadic motor commands may not benefit fully from accurate offline modifications because of the loss of spatial precision in amblyopia. 25,26,29,30,55 Second, the internal feedback processes that use the efference copy of the initial motor command may not function optimally due to imprecise postsaccadic errors as a result of amblyopia. 55 Hence, a decrease in offline and online gain modulation may explain the decreased adaptation we observed in patients with amblyopia. 
Time Course of Saccadic Adaptation
Generally, the temporal course of saccadic adaptation in visually-normal individuals is characterized by an exponential function—the adapting gain undergoes rapid initial change, followed by a more gradual change that asymptotes at a new steady-state gain. 51 For gain-decrease adaptation, the steady-state gain usually is reached within 100 saccades. 49 52,56 Is the reduced adaptation of saccadic gain in patients due to an impairment in the short-term ability to adapt, or is it due to a temporal course difference, such that they require more trials to adapt? 
We found that the majority of patient data (Table 3) could be fit with an exponential time function. In addition, their time constants were comparable to controls, suggesting that saccadic gain decreased in patients at a rate similar to visually-normal people. Inspection of the raw data also revealed no further decrease in saccadic gain near the end of the adaptation period for either patients or controls, indicating that a steady state was reached within 120 adaptation trials. However, due to greater response variability, patients exhibited weaker R 2 values for exponential fits with the amblyopic eye viewing. It is noteworthy that three patients had nonexponential time courses during amblyopic eye viewing (Fig. 5). A similar nonexponential adaptation time course also was observed by Havermann and Lappe when postsaccadic visual error became extremely variable in healthy people (compare Fig. 2 in their study 21 ). While this may suggest that highly imprecise visual error signals in patients may lead to a time course that differs from the stereotypical decreasing exponential waveform, these three patients had only moderate visual acuity loss (0.3–0.5 logMAR). We currently are recruiting a larger number of patients with varied visual acuity loss to investigate further the temporal characteristics of adaptation in amblyopia. 
Longer and More Variable Saccadic Latencies During Adaptation
We found that patients with amblyopia had longer and more variable saccadic latencies compared to controls during adaptation with amblyopic eye viewing. This is consistent with a generalized increase in saccadic latency in patients with amblyopia reported previously by us 33,35,57 and others. 38,58 Moreover, the saccadic latencies were longer for all participants during the adaptation block compared to the preadaptation block. There is a possibility that fatigue could increase the latency of saccades in the adaptation and postadaptation blocks. However, given that the mean saccadic latency in the adaptation block (205 ± 26 ms) was higher than those in the preadaptation (197 ± 25 ms) and postadaptation (201 ± 27 ms) blocks, it is likely that the prolonged latency we observed is due to the adaptation paradigm (as reported in the literature 59 ). Also, because the order of preadaptation, adaptation, and postadaptation blocks always was the same for all participants, fatigue is not expected to affect differentially the saccadic latencies of controls and patients. 
Effect of Visual Acuity Deficit on Adaptation
Is it possible that the reduced saccadic gain adaptation merely reflects the visual acuity deficit alone, rather than a reduced ability that is specific to amblyopia? Two lines of evidence suggest that the diminished adaptation in patients is unique to amblyopia. First, by inducing monocular blur in visually-normal people, 57 we have shown that the spatial uncertainty of saccades evident in amblyopia is not simply due to visual acuity loss alone. Second, no significant correlation was observed here between the visual acuity deficit and the percentage change in saccadic gain during amblyopic eye viewing. Patients with moderate amblyopic eye acuity deficits (better than 0.8 logMAR) exhibited comparable adaptation impairment to those with severe visual acuity deficits (0.8 logMAR and worse). 
Implications of Reduced Saccadic Adaptation in Patients With Amblyopia
Sensorimotor adaptive mechanisms are essential for compensating for changes in saccadic performance that occur in normal aging 14 and disease. 11 13,60 We used a short-term saccadic adaptation paradigm, where a gain decrease is elicited within minutes to hours and typically shows considerable recovery overnight. 52 This is distinct from long-term saccadic adaptation that develops over several days and shows no significant recovery. 11 13,45,46,61 Our results suggest that patients have reduced ability of making short-term modifications to their saccadic gains, for example, as required in response to experimental errors and transient extraocular muscle fatigue. However, over a longer term their mean saccadic gain appears normal because long-term saccadic adaptation is intact. This is supported by the fact that we and others 35,58,62,63 have not found any persistent saccadic dysmetria in patients, suggesting that the mechanisms that mediate long-lasting changes in saccade metrics are intact in amblyopia. 
Insights on the Mechanisms of Short-Term Saccadic Adaptation
Short-term saccadic adaptation has been studied extensively over the past decade. Despite considerable research, the neurophysiological mechanism of saccadic adaptation has not yet been identified definitively to our knowledge. 9 One mechanism that is under active investigation is the nature of the error signal(s) that drive saccadic adaptation. Most studies suggest that the control signal for adaptation is a visual error derived from the comparison of the actual postsaccadic retinal position to the predicted postsaccadic retinal position. 18 22 Amblyopia serves as an excellent visual deprivation model to study the properties of the adapting error signal, and the impairments in saccadic adaptation in patients with amblyopia provide additional support that a reliable visual error signal is important for adaptation. 
In conclusion, to our knowledge our study is the first to investigate short-term sensorimotor adaptation of saccadic eye movements in patients with anisometropic amblyopia. Our results showed that patients exhibit an impaired ability to implement short-term changes in saccadic gain required for maintenance of optimal movement accuracy whenever the amblyopic eye is involved in viewing. We propose that this impaired adaptation results from a reduced capability of the saccadic motor commands to be modified accurately because of imprecise postsaccadic error signals as a result of amblyopia-related visual losses. 
Acknowledgments
Supported by Grants MOP 106663 from the Canadian Institutes of Health Research (CIHR), Leaders Opportunity Fund from the Canadian Foundation for Innovation (CFI), and the Department of Ophthalmology and Vision Sciences at The Hospital for Sick Children. 
Disclosure: R.A. Raashid, None; A.M.F. Wong, None; M. Chandrakumar, None; A. Blakeman, None; H.C. Goltz, None 
References
Jacobs RJ. Visual resolution and contour interaction in the fovea and periphery. Vision Res . 1979; 19: 1187– 1195. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Becker W Fuchs AF. Further properties of the human saccadic system: eye movements and correction saccades with and without visual fixation points. Vision Res . 1969; 9: 1247– 1258. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Troost BT Weber RB Daroff RB. Hypometric saccades. Am J Ophthalmol . 1974; 78: 1002– 1005. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Robinson DA. Oculomotor control signals. In: Bach-y-Rita P Lennerstrand G eds. Basic Mechanisms of Ocular Motility and Their Clinical Implications. 1st ed. New York, NY: Pergamon Press; 1975: 337– 374.
Westheimer G. Eye movement responses to a horizontally moving visual stimulus. AMA Arch Ophthalmol . 1954; 52: 932– 941. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Girard B Berthoz A. From brainstem to cortex: computational models of saccade generation circuitry. Prog Neurobiol . 2005; 77: 215– 251. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Gaveau V Martin O Prablanc C Pelisson D Urquizar C Desmurget M. On-line modification of saccadic eye movements by retinal signals. Neuroreport . 2003; 14: 875– 878. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
West GL Welsh TN Pratt J. Saccadic trajectories receive online correction: evidence for a feedback-based system of oculomotor control. J Mot Behav . 2009; 41: 117– 127. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Hopp JJ Fuchs AF. The characteristics and neuronal substrate of saccadic eye movement plasticity. Prog Neurobiol . 2004; 72: 27– 53. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Pelisson D Alahyane N Panouilleres M Tilikete C. Sensorimotor adaptation of saccadic eye movements. Neurosci Biobehav Rev . 2010; 34: 1103– 1120. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Abel LA Schmidt D Dell'Osso LF Daroff RB. Saccadic system plasticity in humans. Ann Neurol . 1978; 4: 313– 318. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Kommerell G Olivier D Theopold H. Adaptive programming of phasic and tonic components in saccadic eye movements. Investigations of patients with abducens palsy. Invest Ophthalmol . 1976; 15: 657– 660. [PubMed]
Optican LM Zee DS Chu FC. Adaptive response to ocular muscle weakness in human pursuit and saccadic eye movements. J Neurophysiol . 1985; 54: 110– 122. [PubMed]
Warabi T Kase M Kato T. Effect of aging on the accuracy of visually guided saccadic eye movement. Ann Neurol . 1984; 16: 449– 454. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Noto CT Robinson FR. Visual error is the stimulus for saccade gain adaptation. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res . 2001; 12: 301– 305. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Seeberger T Noto C Robinson F. Non-visual information does not drive saccade gain adaptation in monkeys. Brain Res . 2002; 956: 374– 379. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Wallman J Fuchs AF. Saccadic gain modification: visual error drives motor adaptation. J Neurophysiol . 1998; 80: 2405– 2416. [PubMed]
Bahcall DO Kowler E. The control of saccadic adaptation: implications for the scanning of natural visual scenes. Vision Res . 2000; 40: 2779– 2796. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Bonnetblanc F Baraduc P. Saccadic adaptation without retinal postsaccadic error. Neuroreport . 2007; 18: 1399– 1402. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Collins T Wallman J. The relative importance of retinal error and prediction in saccadic adaptation. J Neurophysiol . 2012; 107: 3342– 3348. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Havermann K Lappe M. The influence of the consistency of postsaccadic visual errors on saccadic adaptation. J Neurophysiol . 2010; 103: 3302– 3310. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Wong AL Shelhamer M. Sensorimotor adaptation error signals are derived from realistic predictions of movement outcomes. J Neurophysiol . 2011; 105: 1130– 1140. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
American Academy of Ophthalmology Pediatric Ophthalmology/Strabismus Panel. Preferred Practice Pattern Guidelines. Amblyopia. American Academy of Ophthalmology, San Francisco, CA, 2007. Available at: www.aao.org.ppp. Accessed June 5, 2013.
Bonneh YS Sagi D Polat U. Spatial and temporal crowding in amblyopia. Vision Res . 2007; 47: 1950– 1962. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Levi DM Klein SA. Spatial localization in normal and amblyopic vision. Vision Res . 1983; 23: 1005– 1017. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Levi DM Klein SA Yap YL. Positional uncertainty in peripheral and amblyopic vision. Vision Res . 1987; 27: 581– 597. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
McKee SP Levi DM Movshon JA. The pattern of visual deficits in amblyopia. J Vis . 2003; 3: 380– 405. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Simmers AJ Ledgeway T Hess RF McGraw PV. Deficits to global motion processing in human amblyopia. Vision Res . 2003; 43: 729– 738. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Levi DM Klein SA. Noise provides some new signals about the spatial vision of amblyopes. J Neurosci . 2003; 23: 2522– 2526. [PubMed]
Levi DM Waugh SJ Beard BL. Spatial scale shifts in amblyopia. Vision Res . 1994; 34: 3315– 3333. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Grant S Melmoth DR Morgan MJ Finlay AL. Prehension deficits in amblyopia. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci . 2007; 48: 1139– 1148. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Niechwiej-Szwedo E Goltz HC Chandrakumar M Hirji Z Crawford JD Wong AM. Effects of anisometropic amblyopia on visuomotor behavior, part 2: visually guided reaching. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci . 2011; 52: 795– 803. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Niechwiej-Szwedo E Goltz HC Chandrakumar M Hirji Z Wong AM. Effects of anisometropic amblyopia on visuomotor behavior, III: temporal eye-hand coordination during reaching. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci . 2011; 52: 5853– 5861. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Niechwiej-Szwedo E Goltz HC Chandrakumar M Wong AM. The effect of sensory uncertainty due to amblyopia (lazy eye) on the planning and execution of visually-guided 3D reaching movements. PLoS One . 2012; 7: e31075. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Niechwiej-Szwedo E Goltz HC Chandrakumar M Hirji ZA Wong AM. Effects of anisometropic amblyopia on visuomotor behavior, I: saccadic eye movements. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci . 2010; 51: 6348– 6354. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Ciuffreda KJ Kenyon RV Stark L. Fixational eye movements in amblyopia and strabismus. J Am Optom Assoc . 1979; 50: 1251– 1258. [PubMed]
Gonzalez EG Wong AM Niechwiej-Szwedo E Tarita-Nistor L Steinbach MJ. Eye position stability in amblyopia and in normal binocular vision. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci . 2012; 53: 5386– 5394. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Schor C Hallmark W. Slow control of eye position in strabismic amblyopia. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci . 1978; 17: 577– 581. [PubMed]
Parks MM. The monofixation syndrome. Trans Am Ophthalmol Soc . 1969; 67: 609– 657. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Pediatric Eye Disease Investigator Group. A randomized trial of atropine vs. patching for treatment of moderate amblyopia in children. Arch Ophthalmol . 2002; 120: 268– 278. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Repka MX Kraker RT Beck RW Treatment of severe amblyopia with weekend atropine: results from 2 randomized clinical trials. J Aapos . 2009; 13: 258– 263. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Dolman P. Tests for determining the sighting eye. Am J Ophthalmol . 1919; 2: 287. [CrossRef]
McLaughlin SC. Parametric adjustment in saccadic eye movements. Percept Psychophys . 1967; 2: 359– 362. [CrossRef]
Kowler E Anderson E Dosher B Blaser E. The role of attention in the programming of saccades. Vision Res . 1995; 35: 1897– 1916. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Alahyane N Pelisson D. Long-lasting modifications of saccadic eye movements following adaptation induced in the double-step target paradigm. Learn Mem . 2005; 12: 433– 443. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Robinson FR Soetedjo R Noto C. Distinct short-term and long-term adaptation to reduce saccade size in monkey. J Neurophysiol . 2006; 96: 1030– 1041. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Savitzky A Golay M. Smoothing and differentiation of data by simplified least squares procedures. Analyt Chem . 1964; 36: 1627– 1639. [CrossRef]
Albano JE King WM. Rapid adaptation of saccadic amplitude in humans and monkeys. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci . 1989; 30: 1883– 1893. [PubMed]
Deubel H Wolf W Hauske G. Adaptive gain control of saccadic eye movements. Hum Neurobiol . 1986; 5: 245– 253. [PubMed]
Frens MA van Opstal AJ. Transfer of short-term adaptation in human saccadic eye movements. Exp Brain Res . 1994; 100: 293– 306. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Miller JM Anstis T Templeton WB. Saccadic plasticity: parametric adaptive control by retinal feedback. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform . 1981; 7: 356– 366. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Semmlow JL Gauthier GM Vercher JL. Mechanisms of short-term saccadic adaptation. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform . 1989; 15: 249– 258. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Leigh RJ Zee DS. The saccadic system. In: The Neurology of Eye Movements. 4th ed. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 2006; 116–117, 124– 149.
Prsa M Thier P. The role of the cerebellum in saccadic adaptation as a window into neural mechanisms of motor learning. Eur J Neurosci . 2011; 33: 2114– 2128. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Chen-Harris H Joiner WM Ethier V Zee DS Shadmehr R. Adaptive control of saccades via internal feedback. J Neurosci . 2008; 28: 2804– 2813. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Albano JE. Adaptive changes in saccade amplitude: oculocentric or orbitocentric mapping? Vision Res . 1996; 36: 2087– 2098. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Niechwiej-Szwedo E Kennedy SA Colpa L Chandrakumar M Goltz HC Wong AM. Effects of induced monocular blur versus anisometropic amblyopia on saccades, reaching and eye-hand coordination. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci . 2012; 53: 5354– 5362. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Ciuffreda KJ Kenyon RV Stark L. Increased saccadic latencies in amblyopic eyes. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci . 1978; 17: 697– 702. [PubMed]
Ethier V Zee DS Shadmehr R. Changes in control of saccades during gain adaptation. J Neurosci . 2008; 28: 13929– 13937. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Choi KD Kim HJ Cho BM Kim JS. Saccadic adaptation in lateral medullary and cerebellar infarction. Exp Brain Res . 2008; 188: 475– 482. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Snow R Hore J Vilis T. Adaptation of saccadic and vestibulo-ocular systems after extraocular muscle tenectomy. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci . 1985; 26: 924– 931. [PubMed]
Ciuffreda KJ Kenyon RV Stark L. Abnormal saccadic substitution during small-amplitude pursuit tracking in amblyopic eyes. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci . 1979; 18: 506– 516. [PubMed]
Schor C. A directional impairment of eye movement control in strabismus amblyopia. Invest Ophthalmol . 1975; 14: 692– 697. [PubMed]
Figure 1
 
(A) The double-step target paradigm: participants fixated a central visual target (F). After a random interval of 750 to 1250 ms, the target stepped to 19° (T1, rightwards in this example). As soon as the primary saccade eye velocity exceeded 50°/s, the target stepped 4.2° back toward initial fixation (T2) before returning to central fixation. Participants were instructed to follow the target as quickly and as accurately as possible. The right panel shows the amplitude versus time graphs for target and eye position for single trials at the beginning (B) and near the end of the adaptation block (C). As adaptation neared completion, the primary saccade brought the eyes to the final target position.
Figure 1
 
(A) The double-step target paradigm: participants fixated a central visual target (F). After a random interval of 750 to 1250 ms, the target stepped to 19° (T1, rightwards in this example). As soon as the primary saccade eye velocity exceeded 50°/s, the target stepped 4.2° back toward initial fixation (T2) before returning to central fixation. Participants were instructed to follow the target as quickly and as accurately as possible. The right panel shows the amplitude versus time graphs for target and eye position for single trials at the beginning (B) and near the end of the adaptation block (C). As adaptation neared completion, the primary saccade brought the eyes to the final target position.
Figure 2
 
Changes in saccadic gain across all three experimental blocks, shown for control participant 6 (left column) and patient 4 (right column) during nondominant/amblyopic eye (top row), binocular (middle row), and dominant/fellow eye (bottom row) viewing conditions. The dotted vertical lines mark the end of the preadaptation blocks and solid vertical lines mark the end of the adaptation blocks. The control participant showed a consistently high change in saccadic gain during adaptation across all three viewing conditions, while the patient exhibited reduced and more variable change in saccadic gain during adaptation during amblyopic and binocular viewing. The preadaptation blocks were fit with a horizontal line indicating the average gain over that block; the adaptation blocks were fit with the decreasing exponential equation, G(t) = G 0 + Δ Ge−t/τ , and the postadaptation blocks were fit with the increasing exponential equation, G(t) = G 0 + Δ G(1 − e−t/τ ).
Figure 2
 
Changes in saccadic gain across all three experimental blocks, shown for control participant 6 (left column) and patient 4 (right column) during nondominant/amblyopic eye (top row), binocular (middle row), and dominant/fellow eye (bottom row) viewing conditions. The dotted vertical lines mark the end of the preadaptation blocks and solid vertical lines mark the end of the adaptation blocks. The control participant showed a consistently high change in saccadic gain during adaptation across all three viewing conditions, while the patient exhibited reduced and more variable change in saccadic gain during adaptation during amblyopic and binocular viewing. The preadaptation blocks were fit with a horizontal line indicating the average gain over that block; the adaptation blocks were fit with the decreasing exponential equation, G(t) = G 0 + Δ Ge−t/τ , and the postadaptation blocks were fit with the increasing exponential equation, G(t) = G 0 + Δ G(1 − e−t/τ ).
Figure 3
 
Mean percentage change in saccadic gain for controls and patients for the three viewing conditions. Error bars represent SEM. Patients showed a reduced percentage change in saccadic gain compared to controls during amblyopic eye and binocular condition. *P < 0.05.
Figure 3
 
Mean percentage change in saccadic gain for controls and patients for the three viewing conditions. Error bars represent SEM. Patients showed a reduced percentage change in saccadic gain compared to controls during amblyopic eye and binocular condition. *P < 0.05.
Figure 4
 
(A) Mean saccadic latency shown for controls and patients for all three viewing conditions, averaged over all experimental blocks. (B) Saccadic latency variability shown for controls and patients for all three viewing conditions, averaged across all experimental blocks. Error bars represent SEM. Patients exhibited longer and more variable saccadic latencies when viewing with the amblyopic eye compared to the nondominant eye of controls. *P < 0.05.
Figure 4
 
(A) Mean saccadic latency shown for controls and patients for all three viewing conditions, averaged over all experimental blocks. (B) Saccadic latency variability shown for controls and patients for all three viewing conditions, averaged across all experimental blocks. Error bars represent SEM. Patients exhibited longer and more variable saccadic latencies when viewing with the amblyopic eye compared to the nondominant eye of controls. *P < 0.05.
Figure 5
 
The time course of changes in saccadic gain for all patients during amblyopic eye viewing. The adaptation trials were fit by the decreasing exponential equation, G(t) = G 0 + ΔGe −t/τ . It is noteworthy that patients 1, 5, and 8 did not exhibit an exponential change in the adapting saccadic gain.
Figure 5
 
The time course of changes in saccadic gain for all patients during amblyopic eye viewing. The adaptation trials were fit by the decreasing exponential equation, G(t) = G 0 + ΔGe −t/τ . It is noteworthy that patients 1, 5, and 8 did not exhibit an exponential change in the adapting saccadic gain.
Figure 6
 
(A) Mean goodness of fit (R 2) values shown for binocular (ncontrols = 11, npatients = 5), dominant/fellow eye (ncontrols = 9, npatients = 6), and nondominant/amblyopic eye viewing conditions (ncontrols = 9, npatients = 5). (B) Mean time constants (τ) shown for controls and patients for all three viewing conditions. Error bars represent SEM. Patients exhibited weaker R 2 values when viewing with the amblyopic eye, but comparable time constants of adaptation under all viewing conditions. *P < 0.05.
Figure 6
 
(A) Mean goodness of fit (R 2) values shown for binocular (ncontrols = 11, npatients = 5), dominant/fellow eye (ncontrols = 9, npatients = 6), and nondominant/amblyopic eye viewing conditions (ncontrols = 9, npatients = 5). (B) Mean time constants (τ) shown for controls and patients for all three viewing conditions. Error bars represent SEM. Patients exhibited weaker R 2 values when viewing with the amblyopic eye, but comparable time constants of adaptation under all viewing conditions. *P < 0.05.
Table 1
 
Clinical Characteristics of Patients With Anisometropic Amblyopia
Table 1
 
Clinical Characteristics of Patients With Anisometropic Amblyopia
I.D. Age Sex Snellen Visual Acuity (LogMAR) Refractive Error Deviation, Δ* Sensory Fusion Stereoacuity, s of arc
Right Eye Left Eye Right Eye Left Eye Near, 0.33 m Distance, 6 m Worth 4 Dot Bagolini
1 25 F 20/20 (0.00) 20/50 (0.40) −1.50 +1.50 × 80 −3.00 +2.50 × 80 E4 E1 Fused N/A 120
2 18 F 20/20 (0.00) 20/60 (0.48) −1.50 +0.50 × 80 +1.00 +1.25 × 95 LET2 LET2 Fused N/A 200
3 30 M 20/15 (−0.10) 20/200 (1.00) +4.00 +6.00 +1.75 × 90 LXT4 LXT8 Fused N/A Negative
4 44 F 20/200 (1.00) 20/15 (−0.10) +4.50 Plano RET2 RET2 Fused N/A 3000
5 21 F 20/60 (0.48) 20/15 (−0.10) −11.25 −3.00 +0.75 × 15 RET2, ET + E4‡ RET2, ET + E4‡ Suppress Fused Negative
6 25 F 20/15 (−0.10) 20/80 (0.60) +2.25 +1.00 LET2 LET2 Fused N/A 100
7 21 F 20/20 (0.00) 20/125 (0.80) Plano +2.00 LXT4 LXT2 Fused N/A 3000
8 27 F 20/20 (0.00) 20/40 (0.30) +0.25 +2.75 LET1, ET + X4‡ LET1 Fused N/A 140
Table 2
 
Clinical Characteristics of Visually-Normal Participants
Table 2
 
Clinical Characteristics of Visually-Normal Participants
I.D. Age Sex Snellen Visual Acuity (LogMAR) Refractive Error Stereoacuity, s of arc Dominant Eye
Right Eye Left Eye Right Eye Left Eye
1 42 F 20/20 (0.00) 20/20 (0.00) −3.25 −3.25 40 Right
2 34 F 20/20 (0.00) 20/20 (0.00) −1.50 +0.25 × 85 −1.50 +0.25 × 75 40 Right
3 24 M 20/15 (−0.10) 20/15 (−0.10) Plano Plano 30 Left
4 23 M 20/20 (0.00) 20/20 (0.00) −4.50 +2.50 × 98 −5.00 +3.50 × 78 40 Right
5 29 M 20/15 (−0.10) 20/15 (−0.10) −2.00 −1.25 40 Right
6 23 F 20/20 (0.00) 20/15 (−0.10) −2.50 −2.50 +0.50 × 100 40 Right
7 20 M 20/20 (0.00) 20/15 (−0.10) Plano Plano 40 Right
8 21 F 20/20 (0.00) 20/15 (−0.10) Plano Plano 40 Right
9 23 F 20/20 (0.00) 20/15 (−0.10) −1.50 +0.25 × 90 −1.00 +0.25 × 90 40 Right
10 24 M 20/20 (0.00) 20/20 (0.00) Plano Plano 40 Right
11 18 F 20/20 (0.00) 20/20 (0.00) Plano Plano 40 Right
Table 3
 
The Time Constants (τ) and Goodness of Fit (R 2) Values for All Participants in Control and Patient Groups, for NDE/AE, BE, and DE/FE Viewing Conditions
Table 3
 
The Time Constants (τ) and Goodness of Fit (R 2) Values for All Participants in Control and Patient Groups, for NDE/AE, BE, and DE/FE Viewing Conditions
Group I.D. NDE/AE BE DE/FE
τ, n of Trials R 2 τ, n of Trials R 2 τ, n of Trials R 2
Control 1 9 0.318 12 0.377 6 0.227
2 8 0.299 31 0.475 14 0.360
3 N/A N/A 39 0.370 N/A N/A
4 36 0.503 38 0.296 16 0.163
5 28 0.198 4 0.126 N/A N/A
6 17 0.298 29 0.381 20 0.226
7 N/A N/A 10 0.133 50 0.413
8 21 0.437 37 0.486 25 0.278
9 10 0.284 16 0.228 31 0.319
10 4 0.169 25 0.327 35 0.333
11 52 0.423 3 0.187 5 0.162
Mean ± SD 20 ± 16 0.33 ± 0.11* 22 ± 14 0.31 ± 0.13 23 ± 15 0.28 ± 0.09
Patient 1 N/A N/A 23 0.471 25 0.412
2 28 0.210 38 0.239 12 0.174
3 20 0.140 24 0.375 34 0.517
4 17 0.259 25 0.266 39 0.236
5 N/A N/A N/A N/A 60 0.414
6 12 0.170 N/A N/A 15 0.127
7 16 0.246 N/A N/A N/A N/A
8 N/A N/A 24 0.248 N/A N/A
Mean ± SD 19 ± 6 0.21 ± 0.05* 27 ± 7 0.32 ± 0.10 31 ± 18 0.31 ± 0.16
×
×

This PDF is available to Subscribers Only

Sign in or purchase a subscription to access this content. ×

You must be signed into an individual account to use this feature.

×