Anisometropia, an interocular difference in refractive error,
1 is an important pediatric refractive error, affecting up to 11% of children, depending on the definition of anisometropia and the age of the population studied.
1–6 Although a number of studies have examined the visual deficits associated with amblyopic anisometropia,
7,8 few studies have investigated the impact of uncorrected anisometropia on functional reading performance, visual information processing (VIP), and reading-related eye movements that are all relevant to children's performance in school. Thus, the minimum level of anisometropia that requires refractive correction in healthy children, separate from the risk of strabismus or amblyopia development, is unclear.
9–11
Some older studies have reported an association between uncorrected anisometropia and impaired reading skills
12–15; however, the mechanisms underlying this association have not been fully established (e.g., foveal suppression, altered binocular coordination, or aniso-accommodative stress).
9,16,17 A significantly higher prevalence of anisometropia has been observed in children classified as “reading disabled” or “poor readers” compared with age- and IQ-matched controls.
12,14 Additionally, Eames
13 reported that a significantly higher proportion of children with uncorrected hyperopic anisometropia were below their chronological reading age (using the Gates Silent Reading test) compared with a control group (65% and 24%, respectively). However, these studies failed to define the criteria used to classify children as “reading disabled,” “poor readers,” or “anisometropic,” and may have included amblyopic anisometropes, which confounds the influence of anisometropia alone on functional measures relevant to school performance.
Other studies have sought to determine the minimum level of anisometropia that is of functional importance, by simulating anisometropia in adults and assessing binocularity (e.g., stereoacuity or suppression). Simulation of both spherical and astigmatic anisometropia as low as 1.00 diopter (D) has been shown to degrade binocular vision
9–11; however, gross fusion remains intact at higher levels of anisometropia simulation under more natural conditions (up to 3.00 D when using Bagolini lenses).
9,10 Binocular rivalry leading to foveal suppression has been suggested as a possible mechanism underlying the reduction in performance in various outcome measures
9,18; however, a recent study suggests that reading performance in adults does not differ significantly under monocular or binocular viewing conditions.
17 This supports the likelihood that a mechanism other than central suppression (e.g., altered vergence demand, aniso-accommodative stress, or altered sensory fusion) degrades reading performance in simulated anisometropia. Importantly, these adult simulation studies did not account for ocular dominance, which may be a potential confounding variable.
9–11 For example, inducing monocular defocus in front of the dominant eye (typically the right eye in 50%–80% of the population) could result in a greater reduction in performance (than if the simulation was induced in front of the nondominant eye) because it is the preferred eye for visual input.
19 The current evidence regarding the association between ocular dominance and functional reading performance is mixed, whereas some studies have reported superior performance in children with “fixed” dominance (an identifiable dominant eye),
20,21 others have found no such association.
22
Although simulation studies offer valuable insight into the impact of uncorrected anisometropia on visual performance, previous studies have been limited to adults and have not included relevant functional tasks as outcome measures.
9–11 The impact of simulated anisometropia on standardized academic-related performance in children has not been investigated in detail. In addition, although children spend 4 to 5 hours each day on academic activities and have been shown to maintain constant near fixation for up to 16 minutes,
23 the impact of uncorrected anisometropia on sustained school-based near tasks has not been established. Therefore, the aim of the current study was to investigate the impact of simulated hyperopic anisometropia, combined with sustained near work, on a range of standardized academic-related measures in children. We also aimed to examine the influence of ocular dominance on changes in the outcome measures in the presence of simulated anisometropia. Our primary hypothesis was that simulated hyperopic anisometropia would significantly impair functional reading performance, VIP, and reading-related eye movements, which would be exacerbated following sustained near work. We also hypothesized that ocular dominance would influence the changes observed in these outcome measures and explored whether changes in stereoacuity underlie any reductions observed in the academic-related outcome measures as a result of simulated hyperopic anisometropia. A repeated measures design was used to control for potential differences between participants (such as socioeconomic status and IQ) and a range of standardized academic-related tasks that mirror common activities conducted in school classrooms were selected as outcome measures.