Abstract
Purpose.:
Space perception beyond the near distance range (>2 m) is important for target localization, and for directing and guiding a variety of daily activities, including driving and walking. However, it is unclear whether the absolute (egocentric) localization of a single target in the intermediate distance range requires binocular vision, and if so, whether having subnormal stereopsis in strabismus impairs one's ability to localize the target.
Methods.:
We investigated this by measuring the perceived absolute location of a target by observers with normal binocular vision (n = 8; mean age, 24.5 years) and observers with strabismus (n = 8; mean age, 24.9 years) under monocular and binocular conditions. The observers used the blind walking-gesturing task to indicate the judged location of a target located at various viewing distances (2.73–6.93 m) and heights (0, 30, and 90 cm) above the floor. Near stereopsis was assessed with the Randot Stereotest.
Results.:
Both groups of observers accurately judged the absolute distance of the target on the ground (height = 0 cm) either with monocular or binocular viewing. However, when the target was suspended in midair, the normal observers accurately judged target location with binocular viewing, but not with monocular viewing (mean slant angle, 0.8° ± 0.5° vs. 7.4° ± 1.4°; P < 0.001, with a slant angle of 0° representing accurate localization). In contrast, the strabismic observers with poorer stereo acuity made larger errors in target localization in both viewing conditions, though with fewer errors during binocular viewing (mean slant angle, 2.7° ± 0.4° vs. 9.2° ± 1.3°; P < 0.0025). Further analysis reveals the localization error, that is, slant angle, correlates positively with stereo threshold during binocular viewing (r2 = 0.479, P < 0.005), but not during monocular viewing (r2 = 0.0002, P = 0.963).
Conclusions.:
Locating a single target on the ground is sufficient with monocular depth information, but binocular depth information is required when the target is suspended in midair. Since the absolute binocular disparity information of the single target is weak beyond 2 m, we suggest the visual system localizes the single target using the relative binocular disparity information between the midair target and the visible ground surface. Consequently, strabismic observers with residual stereopsis localize a target more accurately than their counterparts without stereo ability.
The experiments were conducted in a well-lighted hallway with full cues. The predominant monocular cues to depth afforded by the hallway were the texture gradient cue from the square carpet tiles on the floor and the linear perspective cue from the parallel walls of the hallway. The targets were white Styrofoam spheres of different physical sizes. The Styrofoam spheres were made of high-density foam that has a smooth, durable surface, and could be readily purchased at most arts-and-crafts stores. They were placed at four viewing distances, 2.73, 4.15, 5.51, and 6.93 m, respectively, to subtend a constant visual angle of 0.52°. At each viewing distance, the target could be located at one of four heights above the floor: 0 (i.e., on the floor), 30, 67, and 90 cm. For the targets suspended above the floor, transparent fishing lines that were not visible to the observers were used to suspend the targets from the ceiling. In this way, the targets above the floor were seen as floating in air and not in direct physical contact with any other surfaces. The targets that were 0 and 67 cm above the floor served as the test targets, while those at the other two heights (30 and 90 cm above the floor) served as catch trial targets. The purpose of having the catch trials was to prevent observers from guessing that all the test targets had the same height.
Overall, our findings provided a quantitative assessment of the spatial abilities of strabismic observers beyond the near visual space compared to observers with normal vision. Similar to normal observers, strabismic observers judged absolute distance accurately when the target was placed on the floor with monocular and binocular viewing. However, unlike the normal observers, they misjudged the absolute location when the target was suspended in midair, with monocular viewing being worse than binocular viewing. During binocular viewing of the suspended target, there exists a positive correlation between the overall error of judged locations and the observer's stereo threshold. Therefore, the advantage of binocular viewing indicates the important role of relative binocular disparity information for locating a target beyond the near space, as predicted in the Introduction.
We should add that our data could be influenced by two other vision-related factors. The first is the fact that several of our observers had previously been treated for their strabismus (S2, S3, S4, S5, and S7). It could be that they also had amblyopia accompanying their strabismus. Therefore, besides the reduced stereoacuity, the previous existence of amblyopia in these observers could potentially contribute to their poor task performance in our study.
11 The second factor is a possible underestimation of the stereoacuity deficits of several observers because we only tested their stereopsis at near (40 cm) rather than at distance (6 m), and we only tested stereoacuity one time during recruitment day rather than repeated the testing during the experimental days. While near and distance stereoacuity would remain similar for observers whose angles of deviation were similar at near and far distances, the same might not be true for observers with intermittent exotropia (S1 and S2).
35 It has been found that, at least in children, stereoacuities of intermittent exotropes vary over the course of a single day.
36 Additionally, observer S5 exhibited interocular suppression at distance, suggesting that his stereoacuity at distance would be far worse (or not measureable) than what we have reported in the
Table had we measured his stereopsis at distance. Thus, an argument could be made that our data would be more directly comparable with distance stereoacuity, since we tested targets at the intermediate distance range (2.73–6.93 m). However, the counter argument could be made that testing stereoacuity at 6 m alone would not be completely representative either.
More generally, our empirical findings can be considered in the larger context of the ground theory of space perception. According to the ground theory, the visual system uses the ground surface as a reference frame for localizing objects.
23–25,33,34,37–42 The visual system can construct accurate ground reference frame representation with monocular depth cues (e.g., texture gradient information on the floor).
30,37,42 Therefore, when the target is on the ground where there are ample monocular depth cues, both groups accurately judged the target distance. As a result, the advantage of binocular depth information for locating a target on the ground is minimal. However, an extra computational step is required to correctly determine the egocentric location of the suspended target. Specifically, the visual system must find a reliable, relative distance between the target and the ground surface reference frame. This can be done by scaling the relative binocular disparity between the target and the ground according to the egocentric distance on the ground surface.
24,43,44 Consequently, when the visual system cannot obtain reliable binocular depth information, either because the observers were strabismic or monocular, the midair location of the target cannot be judged accurately.
The current findings also added to the growing body of works on how impaired stereopsis impedes one's daily activities, such as reaching, grasping, walking, and driving.
4–6,9,45,46 In general, space perception, including perceived distance derived from binocular disparity, is used to guide or direct actions.
47 To guide an action, the perceptual system continuously processes stimulus information to update the space representation during action. The updated space representation acts as an online feedback to guide the motor system's movements, until the goal is achieved. Take the example of a person using his/her sight to continuously guide the action of his/her hand reaching for a cup on the
table. To guide the hand action, the perceptual system first constructs a space representation and stores it in the working memory system. The working memory system provides the initial “instruction” to the motor system to start the action. During the action, the working memory system continuously updates the space representation (memory) and uses the updated representation to direct the motor system's next movement. In contrast, the blind walking-gesturing task used in the current study is an example of a visually directed action. The perceived target location is stored in the working memory system. While blind walking, the traversed distance is registered and used to update the observer's online target distance. When the updated target distance becomes zero, the observer stops walking. Clearly, there are some fundamental differences between visually guided and visually directed actions. However, in reality, we often combine these two operations to achieve the goal in reaching and/or navigation.
Finally, our findings along with those of others (see review by Grant and Moseley 2011)
6 serve as a reminder that poor stereopsis as a result of childhood disorders of strabismus and/or amblyopia persists into adulthood, and affects everyday perception and actions in the near and intermediate visual space. It is notable that at the time of testing none of our observers would be classified as amblyopic when referenced to the American Academy of Ophthalmology's guideline for visual acuity deficit.
48 Moreover, several of our strabismic observers were never treated. Therefore, our study also highlights the fact that poor stereopsis can exist in strabismic individuals whose visual acuity is normal by clinical standards. Fortunately, recent advances in cortical plasticity research have shown the ability of adult animals to recover from the effects of early visual deprivation.
49–51 Consistent with this, human psychophysical studies show that perceptual learning can improve the monocular vision of the weak eye.
52–55 Also taking the psychophysical approach, other works from our laboratory have shown that adults with strabismus and/or amblyopia who underwent the push-pull perceptual learning paradigm to recalibrate the interocular excitatory-and-inhibitory balance exhibited improved stereopsis.
56–58 Future work will investigate if this improvement translates into more accurate use of the relative binocular disparity information for space perception in the intermediate distance range.