There have been reports that when vision is not clear, discomfort occurs
32 (Papas B, et al.
IOVS 2003;44:ARVO E-Abstract 3694). This study explored the association between vision and ocular discomfort by evaluating how people scale suprathreshold pneumatic stimuli when vision is clear and defocused. We and others
22,36,37 have shown that suprathreshold scaling using mechanical, chemical, and thermal stimuli can evoke intensity and/or unpleasant (affective) stimulus attributes on the ocular surface. Generally, the estimated magnitude of sensation is proportional to the magnitude of the stimulus applied
16,21,36,38,39; as stimulus intensity increases, so reports of perceived intensity or perceived discomfort increases systematically as a power function (Stevens' power law
3). Though previous studies derived the power exponents of the psychophysical magnitude (transducer) functions for the ocular surface, the perception of sensations when vision is defocused was not examined. Our study demonstrated that the transducer functions for discomfort are different when vision is clear and defocused, despite statistically nonsignificant differences in ratings of discomfort and intensity. The implications of the differences in exponents (though statistically just significant) point to the influence of affective pain mechanisms that are distinct from the processing of simple sensory ‘pain intensity,’ and a possible higher order integration of pain and vision. Interestingly, another study on heat pain suggests that sensory-discriminative (pain intensity) and affective-motivational (discomfort) components of pain can have transducer functions with different exponents.
40
The power exponent in a transducer function describes the relationship between the sensation magnitude (ratings) and the stimulus magnitude (pneumatic stimulus strengths).
2 Exponents greater than 1.0 suggest an accelerating function while exponents less than 1.0 are decelerating functions. The exponent for discomfort as observed in this study is 1.08 for defocused condition, while it is 0.96 for clear visual condition. Although these are both close to 1.0 (a linear relationship between stimulus and intensity), this difference in exponents might reflect the subtle changes in the operating characteristics of the sensory system when the visual conditions are changed from clear to defocused.
Pain is multidimensional, consisting of sensory, affective, and cognitive dimensions.
41 The sensory aspects of pain deal with intensity, locus, duration, and quality of pain, while affective pain reflects unpleasantness (discomfort), emotions, and motivational variables.
42 In our study, in addition to the sensory input by the pneumatic stimuli, there can be potential contributions from affective and cognitive factors such as attentional focus, stimulus context, and expectation of pain that can cause discomfort with defocused vision. The probable influence of these factors on the sensory transducer functions are discussed below based on the mechanisms of pain processing and pain perception.
When multiple stimuli are presented to a sensory system, selective attention occurs to filter out unwanted information and focus attention on a particular stimulus attribute.
43 For example, in the visual system, when a cluttered natural scene is presented to the eye, selective attention mechanisms helps to resolve the competition among the multiple objects in the scene in favor of the object that is relevant for the current task.
44 Furthermore, identification of a single object/attribute yields better performance than a task of judging two or multiple attributes. In an experiment where subjects were presented with two different objects, and asked to identify two different attributes (color of one, orientation of other) at the same time, the subjects' performance was worse than the task if performed with only a single object.
45,46 In our experiment, when two sensory conditions (pneumatic stimulation and defocus) are presented together to the subject, the neuronal activity may compete for representation in the brain, and the pattern of activity might have been different than when only one primary stimulus (pneumatic stimulation and clear vision) is present. These differences in neural activity levels may be reflected through the differences in sensory transducer functions. In contrast, distraction, a process of attending to information unrelated to the painful stimulus can also alter the perceived intensity or emotional reaction to a painful stimulus.
47 Studies
48–50 designed to investigate pain while manipulating attention suggest that subjects rate pain lower when they direct their attention away from the painful stimulus. Similarly, measurement of intensity and unpleasantness of thermal pain under different conditions of attention, suggests that pain is perceived to be more intense and unpleasant when subjects focused attention on noxious heat stimuli than when attention is diverted from the stimuli.
50 In our experiment, it is possible that differences in the exponents of the power functions under clear and defocused conditions arose for similar reasons: subjects' attention to the fixation targets were subtly different when vision was clear or defocused.
The context in which a stimulus is presented can alter the experience of pain through warning signals (temporal context),
51–54 subject's visual attention,
55,56 and knowledge of tissue damaging properties of the stimulus (evaluative context).
57 It is known that the warning of noxious signals through cues, and the expectation of pain that occurs due it can amplify pain perception. For example, when a stimulus is preceded by a cue that denotes high intensity, the noxious input is found to hurt more than when it is preceded by a stimulus that denotes low intensity.
54 In our study, defocus perhaps produced a nocebo like effect and acted as cue for increased discomfort. The nocebo effect is a phenomenon where anticipation and expectation of a negative outcome induces worsening of a symptom.
58 Neuroimaging studies have shown the neural mechanisms responsible for the expectations of pain interact with the regions involved in afferent nociceptive processing and change the perception of pain.
54,59,60 Activation of the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), the prefrontal cortex (PFC), and the posterior insula (PI) occur during anticipation of pain,
51,61 and these areas are also involved in the processing of afferent sensory information.
62 To complement the findings by neuroimaging techniques, there is pharmacological evidence
63,64 that anticipatory anxiety can activate the cholecystokinin A and B receptors that facilitate pain transmission. These neurobiological findings provide potential mechanisms for how defocus can influence affective pain mechanisms and alter the perception of discomfort.
We do not have neurophysiological or anatomical evidence that vision and ocular surface somatosensation can interact. Vision and pain have separate receptors and pathways, and different regions of the brain are responsible for the perception of vision and pain.
65 In addition, the lack of electrophysiological (or other) evidence about the sensory integration of vision and pain highlights the difficulty in meaningfully ascribing a physiological mechanism to relate pain/discomfort and vision in neural terms. However, studies on olfaction
66–68 and audition
69–71 show integration between these senses and vision, and the visual cortex may be activated during a purely olfactory task suggesting the possibility that a sensory process may be influenced by processing of other unrelated sensory information in the brain.
67,72
The human brain can process incoming sensory information through modality specific channels from which a unified and coherent perceptual experience is normally derived. This synergy or interaction among the senses and the fusion of their information, ‘multisensory integration,' occurs through the convergence of information from different sensory systems on a common group of neurons. Sensory inputs from the visual, auditory, and somatosensory systems transmitted through the ascending sensory pathways and descending projections from the cortex are found to converge in various combinations on the superior colliculus (SC) neurons of the midbrain.
73–75 The ability of the SC to integrate multisensory information further depends on higher order processes such as the inputs from a small area of association cortex, the anterior ectosylvian sulcus that determines the final unified experience. The extent to which these multisensory mechanisms can be generalized to the results in our experiment, and the integration between vision and somatosensation is yet to be understood. However, recent work on synesthesia does provide evidence that disparate senses (previously believed to be not connected in any meaningful physiological or perceptual way) can interact in some subjects,
76,77 suggesting at least that potential visual-somatosensory interactions are not totally ludicrous.
Another interesting finding in this study was that the average ratings of intensity and discomfort were dissimilar, and pain intensity was rated higher than discomfort. Intensity and pain unpleasantness are two distinct dimensions of pain
50,78 and they can have different relationships to the nociceptive stimulus. Other investigators
79–81 have also demonstrated that pain descriptor scales can provide independent measures of the pain dimensions and it is possible to alter one dimension and leave the other unchanged.
The power exponents obtained by suprathreshold scaling were different under varying visual conditions, in contrast to the results of the repeated measures ANOVA that suggested that the ratings of discomfort and intensity do not change with differing conditions. The inability of repeated measures ANOVA to detect differences may be because defocus does not alter discomfort or intensity ratings, or perhaps the sample size was too small to uncover potential differences in ratings that may exist when the visual conditions are changed. The power functions characterized each subject's sensation at different stimulus intensities, whereas the repeated measures ANOVA assessed the mean ratings under the two viewing conditions at different (nonquantitative) stimulus levels. The power exponents were derived for each individual participant and a paired t-test was performed to investigate the differences in exponents between clear and defocused conditions, and the details in the subjects' transducer functions appear to provide more sensitive information for this experiment on the processing of ocular surface sensations than just the comparison of means.
Studies that examine the impact of contact lens wear on ocular surface sensitivity show equivocal results, with some reporting a difference in thresholds, while some report no differences.
82–88 These studies primarily report sensory detection thresholds with contact lens wear and there are no reports of how contact lens wearers respond to suprathreshold stimulation of the ocular surface. However, it has been demonstrated that contact lens wearers discriminate suprathreshold pneumatic stimuli differently than nonlens wearers.
89 We do not know how these discrimination differences can be related to the scaling of pneumatic stimuli using magnitude estimation techniques, but it can point to probable differences in operating characteristics that can exist between lens and nonlens wearers. In our study, no statistical differences in sensory transducer functions were found between lens and nonlens wearers. This finding should be interpreted with caution, as the sample size was not balanced between the lens- and nonlens-wearing groups and there could be potential effects uncovered due to the lack of sample size.
In summary, the results of the study indicate that the operating characteristics of the ocular surface transducer functions are subtly different under clear and defocused visual conditions, suggesting that defocus may alter the experience of ocular comfort. The differences in the perception of discomfort does not appear to be attributable to the differences in detection threshold or sensory intensity, but from the influences of affective pain mechanisms or a probable higher order sensory integration between vision and pain. Ratings of pain intensity and discomfort have different relationships to the magnitude of pneumatic stimuli, reflecting the different dimensions of pain mechanisms.