Abstract
Purpose.:
The McGurk effect is an audiovisual illusion that involves the concurrent presentation of a phoneme (auditory syllable) and an incongruent viseme (visual syllable). Adults with amblyopia show less susceptibility to this illusion than visually normal controls, even when viewing binocularly. The present study investigated the developmental trajectory of McGurk effect susceptibility in adults, older children (10–17 years), and younger children (4–9 years) with amblyopia.
Methods.:
A total of 62 participants with amblyopia (22 adults, 12 older children, 28 younger children) and 66 visually normal controls (25 adults, 17 older children, 24 younger children) viewed videos that combined phonemes and visemes, and were asked to report what they heard. Videos with congruent (auditory and visual matching) and incongruent (auditory and visual not matching) stimuli were presented. Incorrect responses on incongruent trials correspond to high McGurk effect susceptibility, indicating that the viseme influenced the phoneme.
Results.:
Participants with amblyopia (28.0% ± 3.3%) demonstrated a less consistent McGurk effect than visually normal controls (15.2% ± 2.3%) across all age groups (P = 0.0024). Effect susceptibility increased with age (P = 0.0003) for amblyopic participants and controls. Both groups showed a similar response pattern to different speakers and syllables, but amblyopic participants invariably demonstrated a less consistent effect.
Conclusions.:
Amblyopia is associated with reduced McGurk effect susceptibility in children and adults. Our findings indicate that the differences do not simply indicate delayed development in children with amblyopia; rather, they represent permanent alterations that persist into adulthood.
Amblyopia is a neurodevelopmental disorder affecting 3% to 5% of the population, and is the leading cause of monocular blindness worldwide.
1,2 It is defined clinically as a unilateral or bilateral loss of visual acuity caused by abnormal visual experience early in life.
3 In addition to impaired visual acuity,
4 it is well documented that patients with amblyopia exhibit deficits in spatiotemporal tasks, including attention
5,6 and motion perception,
7,8 as well as higher order cognitive tasks, such as number processing,
9 reading,
10 and real world scene perception.
11 Furthermore, there is recent evidence that impairments associated with amblyopia extend beyond vision, and that audiovisual perception may also be affected.
12
Audiovisual integration in adults with amblyopia has been investigated previously by our group using the McGurk effect, an audiovisual illusion that involves the concurrent presentation of a phoneme (auditory syllable) with an incongruent viseme (visual syllable).
13 Visually normal individuals perceive a phoneme that does not match the actual auditory stimulus, due to the dominant influence of the viseme. Even when participants have prior awareness of the effect, the visual signal is sufficiently dominant that the illusion still is effective.
14 When compared to visually normal controls, adults with amblyopia show a less consistent McGurk effect, indicating that they are less susceptible to the illusion, implying altered audiovisual integration.
12
Several studies have examined the developmental trajectory of McGurk effect susceptibility in visually normal children. There is a general trend toward less susceptibility to the illusion at younger ages, with the effect becoming more consistent as the age range of participants moves toward adulthood. In the original study in which the McGurk effect was introduced, preschool aged children (ages 3–4 years) showed a less consistent effect than school-aged children (ages 7–8 years), who in turn showed a less consistent effect than adults.
13 In a more recent study, 4- to 5-year-old children were compared to adults on McGurk performance, and also showed a less consistent effect.
15 However, differences in susceptibility seem to be more pronounced at younger ages and begin to plateau later in development. Visually normal children aged 8 to 16 years showed no relation between age and McGurk effect within that age group.
16 In another study, visually normal children aged 5 to 9 years showed a less consistent McGurk effect when compared to children aged 10 to 14 and 15 to 19 years, but the two older age groups showed no difference from one another.
17 Visually normal adolescents showed a more consistent McGurk effect when compared not only to younger children, but also to adults, implying that older children are less likely to show deficits in task performance.
18 Given the pattern of McGurk susceptibility in these studies, differences in effect consistency may be limited to younger rather than older children.
A recent study showed reduced McGurk effect in children with amblyopia, but it did not examine the variations in performance across age ranges, and it also did not include an adult group.
19 To our knowledge, the developmental trajectory of susceptibility to the McGurk effect has not been investigated previously in amblyopia. In this study, we investigated whether impaired performance is manifest at an early age, or whether it is a consequence of altered visual development, such that children with amblyopia “fall behind” their typically developing peers later in childhood. We examined two separate age groups of pediatric participants on McGurk effect performance to look for differences between younger and older children, and compared their performance to that of our previously reported adult cohort.
12
All participants were fluent English speakers with no known auditory or neurological disorders, and no eye pathology other than amblyopia, strabismus, or ametropia. Participants were assessed by a certified orthoptist for visual acuity (Early Treatment of Diabetes Retinopathy Study [ETDRS] test), stereoacuity (Randot), and eye alignment (cover–uncover test and alternate prism cover test). Amblyopia was defined as a visual acuity of 0.18 logMAR (20/30) or worse in the amblyopic eye, as well as an intraocular difference (IOD) greater than or equal to 0.2 logMAR (2 chart lines difference). Anisometropia was defined as a difference of 1 diopter (D) in either the spheric or astigmatic correction between the two eyes. Strabismic amblyopia refers to amblyopia in the presence of a manifest deviation (heterotropia) on cover test. Mixed amblyopia refers to the presence of anisometropia and manifest strabismus.
Participants were divided into three age groups: younger children (ages 4–9), older children (ages 10–17), and adults (ages 18+). The age groups were selected based on a previous study examining development of audiovisual illusions in children.
17 Data from adult participants were obtained from a previous study by our group
12 and are included here for comparison purposes; no additional adult data have been added to the current study. We tested 62 participants with amblyopia: 28 younger children (12 female; mean age, 6.3 ± 1.3; age range, 4–9 years), 12 older children (3 female; mean age, 11.5 ± 2.0; age range, 10–16 years), and 22 adults (19 female; mean age, 32.8 ± 10.9 years; age range, 20–56 years). Clinical details for the participants with amblyopia are shown in
Tables 1 to
3. We tested 66 visually normal controls: 24 younger children (11 female; mean age, 7.5 ± 1.4; age range, 5–9 years), 17 older children (10 female; mean age, 11.8 ± 2.0; age range, 10–16 years), and 25 adults (16 female; mean age, 31.8 ± 9.4 years; age range, 19–56 years). Control participants had normal or corrected-to-normal visual acuity of 0.1 logMAR (20/25) or better and stereoacuity of 40 seconds of arc or better. Informed consent was obtained from all adult participants and from parents/guardians of all child participants. Assent was obtained from all child participants. The study was approved by the Research Ethics Board at The Hospital for Sick Children. All protocols adhered to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki.
Table 1 Clinical Characteristics of Younger Children With Amblyopia
Table 1 Clinical Characteristics of Younger Children With Amblyopia
Table 2 Clinical Characteristics of Older Children With Amblyopia
Table 2 Clinical Characteristics of Older Children With Amblyopia
Table 3 Clinical Characteristics of Adults With Amblyopia
Table 3 Clinical Characteristics of Adults With Amblyopia
Response accuracy was analyzed for congruent and incongruent trials. For analysis purposes, any non-“ba” responses to an incongruent trial were considered incorrect and grouped together. Response accuracy was the outcome measure that indicated the susceptibility to the McGurk illusion. Lower accuracy indicates a strong McGurk effect, implying that auditory input was influenced strongly by visual input, causing incorrect responses. Response accuracy is reported as mean and standard error in the text and Figures. Greenhouse-Geisser correction was used for any statistical test where the assumption of sphericity was violated. Post hoc tests were corrected for multiple comparisons using the Tukey-Kramer test.
Response accuracy was analyzed using a 2 × 3 ANOVA, with Group (two levels: amblyopia and visually normal) and Age (three levels: younger children, older children, adults) as between-subjects factors. Congruent trials and incongruent trials were analyzed separately. Effects of visual acuity and stereoacuity on response accuracy in participants with amblyopia were analyzed with separate Spearman correlation analyses for incongruent trials.
Speaker and syllable effects on response accuracy were analyzed separately, using repeated measures ANOVAs, with Group and Age as between-subjects factors, and Speaker (Speaker 1, Speaker 2) or Syllable (da, ga, tha, va) as within-subjects factors. Four participants performed the task with a reduced syllable set because they were unable to pronounce all five syllables correctly, and were excluded from syllable analysis.
Supported by Grant MOP 106663 from the Canadian Institutes of Health Research (CIHR), Leaders Opportunity Fund from the Canada Foundation for Innovation (CFI), the John and Melinda Thompson Endowment Fund in Vision Neurosciences, and the Department of Ophthalmology and Vision Sciences at The Hospital for Sick Children.
Disclosure: C. Narinesingh, None; H.C. Goltz, None; R.A. Raashid, None; A.M.F. Wong, None