The results of the dark adaptation kinetics portion of our study addressed the possibility of visual cycle enhancement in a more direct fashion, where more efficient visual photopigment regeneration would presumably result in faster adaptation. As shown in
Figure 2, subjects with higher MPOD exhibited significantly faster visual adaptation for the three levels of mesopic stimuli. In a fashion similar to the spatial resolution data, as the stimulus became more challenging (i.e., lower level), the impact of MPOD on performance appeared to become greater. One may contend that MP could not possibly have such an effect at a retinal location (12°) for which its density is negligible. As noted in the Introduction and Results sections, as much as 25% of the total retinal carotenoids are accounted for by those deposited in the rod outer segments.
36,37 Based on the antioxidant capability of L and Z, this makes the idea of a relationship between MPOD and enhanced visual cycle/photopigment regeneration in the parafoveal retina plausible, and may indeed be at least partly responsible for what Renzi and Hammond
56 have termed “neural efficiency.” Whatever the case, the effect is strong enough to manifest as a statistically significant performance difference for those with high MP, compared to those with low MP. The recent data of Patryas et al.
33 shows modest relationships between MPOD and rod-mediated, but not cone-mediated, dark adaptation kinetics. There are a few factors that could reconcile the discrepancies between their study and ours. As Patryas et al.
33 note, their results may have been influenced by the large age range (15–68 years) and relatively small sample size (
n = 33); nearly half of the subjects in their sample were aged 55 years or older. The rate of dark adaptation is strongly affected by age, and this effect is quite variable among people.
57 It could be, therefore, that any potential effect of MPOD on dark adaptation kinetics was simply masked by the variability inherent in older subjects' measures. Another potential source of variability in any dark adaptation experiment is the strength of the pretest bleach. Owing to variability in subject pupil size, Patryas et al.
33 have achieved bleaches ranging from 30% to 98%. Given that older subjects tend to exhibit smaller resting pupil diameter (i.e., “senile miosis,” as reported, e.g., in Sloane et al.
48), the presumably disproportionate bleach in the older subjects in the sample of Patryas et al.
33 may have introduced systematic variation into their data. Nevertheless, their rod kinetics data very nearly achieves a statistically significant relationship with MPOD. As noted in the Methods section, we were able to achieve a 90% bleach in all of our test subjects by using Maxwellian view, whereby the subject's pupil is effectively bypassed via optics.