For statistical analysis we compared infants matched by both their corrected age and chronological age, considering response gain as well as responses for near (0.33 m) and distance (2 m). Vergence measurements were all within the linear range of the photorefractor across the range tested, so all infants' vergence gains were calculated using responses at four distances. For accommodation, out-of-range points were excluded and gains were calculated from the responses to the three remaining distances. Gains thus calculated are likely to be a slight underestimate of the true gain. Such exclusions occurred most frequently at 8 to 9 weeks corrected age. Here the median accommodation response for the 0.33-m target of the full data set (using out-of-range points that we know are inaccurate) was 0.34 D more than the mean of the more selected data. If the median from the full dataset had been used to calculate the gain, it would have increased the gain by 0.12. At other ages, differences were less. Four accommodation data points were available for 93% of the target runs for the full-term infants and 90% of those from the preterm infants.
Results of the ANOVAs comparing response gains and responses at 2 and 0.33 m between groups are shown in
Table 2, and post hoc significant differences are indicated in
Figures 3 (vergence) and 4 (accommodation).
Again, we compared groups matched by both corrected and chronological age. When matched by their corrected age, there were the expected significant developmental improvements in all infants. Preterm infants relaxed their accommodation significantly less at 2 m than the full-term infants, but there were no other overall group differences. There were significant age × group interactions in four of the six comparisons, but post hoc testing showed that differences were significant only at 6 to 7 weeks of age (
Figs. 3,
4), where the preterm infants underconverged for near and overaccommodated for distance targets. Subsequently, up to 24 to 27 weeks, there were no differences in accommodation and vergence responses between full-term and preterm infants matched by their corrected age.
When infants were matched by chronological age, there were significant preterm/full-term group differences for all comparisons except accommodation at 2 m. Full-term infants showed more appropriate responses than the chronologically age-matched preterm infants (gain closer to 1, responses closer to the target demand). There was also a significant age × group interaction for all comparisons except accommodation at 0.33 m. Post hoc testing showed that the majority of significant differences were found between infants aged between 10 and 16 weeks and were particularly clear at 10 to 11 weeks of age. While the full-term infants' responses appeared to have matured (were similar to responses at the oldest age tested), those of the preterm infants were still immature.
To test the linearity of vergence and accommodative responses for each group, we calculated correlation coefficients (r2) for individual stimulus response slopes where four data points (at 0.33, 0.5, 1, and 2 m) were available. Infants matched by their corrected age demonstrated similar linearity of response; for example, for vergence at 12 to 13 weeks, mean r2 were 0.94 and 0.91, respectively, for full-term and the corrected-age preterm infants. However, when matched by chronological age, 12- to 13-week preterm infants demonstrated less linear vergence (r2 = 0.77 for preterm infants and 0.94 for full-term infants) (t = 2.57, P = 0.019), not significantly different from full-term infants at 6 to 7 weeks. Similar analysis for accommodation showed that mean r2 for the full-term and the corrected-age preterm infants did not differ significantly (0.74 and 0.77 respectively), but preterm infants of the same chronological age had a lower mean r2 of only 0.53 (t(39) = 2.4, P = 0.02), again not significantly different from full-term infants at 6 to 7 weeks.