October 2015
Volume 56, Issue 11
Free
Retina  |   October 2015
Assessment of Visual and Chromatic Functions in a Rodent Model of Retinal Degeneration
Author Affiliations & Notes
  • Francisco Segura
    Department of Surgery, Gynecology and Obstetrics, University of Zaragoza, Zaragoza, Spain
    Aragon Institute for Health Research (IIS Aragón), Aragón, Spain
  • Ana Sánchez-Cano
    Aragon Institute for Health Research (IIS Aragón), Aragón, Spain
    Department of Applied Physics, University of Zaragoza, Zaragoza, Spain
  • Sebastián Jarabo
    Department of Applied Physics, University of Zaragoza, Zaragoza, Spain
  • Carmen López de la Fuente
    Aragon Institute for Health Research (IIS Aragón), Aragón, Spain
    Department of Applied Physics, University of Zaragoza, Zaragoza, Spain
  • Nicolás Cuenca
    Laboratory of the Neurobiology of the Visual System, Department of Physiology Genetics and Microbiology, University of Alicante, Alicante, Spain
  • María P. Villegas-Pérez
    Department of Ophthalmology, University of Murcia, Murcia, Spain
  • Isabel Pinilla
    Department of Surgery, Gynecology and Obstetrics, University of Zaragoza, Zaragoza, Spain
    Aragon Institute for Health Research (IIS Aragón), Aragón, Spain
    Department of Ophthalmology, Lozano Blesa University Hospital, Zaragoza, Spain
  • Correspondence: Isabel Pinilla, Department of Ophthalmology, Lozano Blesa University Hospital, San Juan Bosco 15, 50009 Zaragoza, Spain; isabel.pinilla@telefonica.net
Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science October 2015, Vol.56, 6275-6283. doi:https://doi.org/10.1167/iovs.15-17257
  • Views
  • PDF
  • Share
  • Tools
    • Alerts
      ×
      This feature is available to authenticated users only.
      Sign In or Create an Account ×
    • Get Citation

      Francisco Segura, Ana Sánchez-Cano, Sebastián Jarabo, Carmen López de la Fuente, Nicolás Cuenca, María P. Villegas-Pérez, Isabel Pinilla; Assessment of Visual and Chromatic Functions in a Rodent Model of Retinal Degeneration. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 2015;56(11):6275-6283. https://doi.org/10.1167/iovs.15-17257.

      Download citation file:


      © ARVO (1962-2015); The Authors (2016-present)

      ×
  • Supplements
Abstract

Purpose: We evaluated the photoreceptor response of pigmented P23H and normal pigmented Long Evans (LE) rats over time using functional tests in variable lighting conditions.

Methods: Pigmented P23H rats were studied by optomotor testing and electroretinogram (ERG) recordings at P30, P150, and P240. Pigmented LE rats were used as a normal wild-type control. Stimuli were modified with colored filters. Neutral density filters were used to reduce luminance.

Results: Age-related decreases in visual acuity (VA) and contrast sensitivity (CS) were observed in P23H rats. Good correlations in measurements without filter and with green filter were observed between LE and P23H P30 rat values. Differences between groups were smaller with red and purple filters. A strong relationship with luminance was observed in LE rats (VA and CS) and with P23H P30 rats (CS). A decline in the ERG responses of P23H rats was consistent with the gradual loss of photoreceptors. Differences in a- and b-wave amplitudes with different colored filters were negligible with the exception of the red filter, which resulted in smaller responses.

Conclusions: Visual function parameters decreased with age in pigmented P23H rats. Irrespective of luminance, color filter, and retinal degeneration, minimum thresholds of VA and CS were found. Smaller differences than expected were found using color filters. Responses to functional tests at long wavelengths were observed, where there is very low photoreceptor spectral sensitivity. The use of filters with functional testing could minimize light-induced retinal damage in rats.

Rats are nocturnal animals with a rod-dominated retina. However, their retina also contains a small number of cones.1 Rats maintain an order of magnitude difference in the numbers of cones with maximum sensitivity in the short wavelength (S-cones, with a peak of 358 nm) compared to the middle and long wavelengths (ML-cones, peaking at approximately 509 nm).2,3 Middle and long wavelength cones constitute approximately 90% of cones; the other 10% are S-cones that are thought to be UV-sensitive elements, but they probably do not appreciably contribute to the photopic system of the rat.4 The maximum responses of the two types of cones and rods of rats have been widely studied by electroretinography (ERG), however the contribution of each type of cone to the visual system is not clear.3,5 
Retinitis pigmentosa (RP) is the most common form of inherited photoreceptor degeneration.6,7 It comprises a group of diseases characterized by a progressive anatomical and functional loss of rod and cone photoreceptors.8 Autosomal dominant RP is responsible for most RP cases,9 and most cases are due to rhodopsin mutations. Cones are lost after the rod degeneration. The transgenic albino P23H rat is a well-studied model of autosomal dominant RP. In pigmented RP rodent models, a progressive deterioration also occurs in retinal function and anatomy. Because an important role of retinal pigmentation is to prevent light damage, we studied heterozygous pigmented P23H rats. These animals undergo slower retinal degeneration, provide a closer model to human RP, and allow straightforward evaluation by functional testing.10 
Retinal damage related to exposure to intense visible light has been studied widely.11,12 The retinas of genetically inbred albino rodents have been shown to be particularly susceptible to photic injury induced by moderate and high levels of light exposure.1315 Not all rhodopsin mutations seem to have the same susceptibility to light damage. Other genetic factors also have been reported to be involved in the light sensitivity of different rodent strains.13,14,16 There is a direct relationship between light and retinal damage, so that a longer duration of exposure or higher intensity of light results in greater retinal damage.17 Using measurement techniques that involve less exposure to light could assess the contribution of the different photoreceptors and minimize damage related to light on the retina of animal models. 
Electroretinographic evaluation is one of the most widely used methods to determine visual function in animal models.10,1820 The introduction of functional devices, such as the OptoMotry system (OptoMotry; CerebralMechanics, Lethbride, Alberta, Canada),21,22 a noninvasive method used to examine the response of the rat in vivo, allows for the study of cone and rod degeneration over time and allows for the in vivo evaluation of therapeutic interventions in the same animal. Optokinetic tracking (OKT) measurements have the advantage of robust behavior and do not require the rat to be trained, allowing a rapid evaluation of visual features, such as visual acuity (VA) and contrast sensitivity (CS).10,2326 
The aim of our study was to evaluate the photoreceptor response of pigmented P23H and pigmented normal Long Evans (LE) rats over time, and to identify the relative contributions of rods and cones using functional tests in different lighting conditions. 
Methods
Animals
Pigmented transgenic rats, heterozygous for the P23H rhodopsin mutation, were bred from a cross between normal pigmented LE rats and transgenic albino homozygous P23H line 1. Animals were studied by optomotor testing and ERG recordings at P30, P150, and P240. Long Evans rats (P90) were used as the wild-type control. Eight animals of each group were studied. 
Transgenic rats were obtained from Dr M. LaVail (University of California, San Francisco, San Francisco, CA, USA), bred in a colony at the University of Zaragoza, Zaragoza, Spain, and maintained under a 12-hour light/dark cycle. Animals were housed and handled with the authorization and supervision of the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee from the University of Zaragoza. Procedures were performed in accordance with the ARVO Statement for the Use of Animals in Ophthalmic and Vision Research. 
Visual Acuity and Contrast Sensitivity Evaluation
To evaluate visual parameters, 8 pigmented P23H rats were measured at P30, P150, and P240. Eight LE rats were evaluated at P90. The assessment of VA and CS was performed using an OptoMotry system (OptoMotry, CerebralMechanics).21,22 The device consists of a testing chamber created with four screens facing into a square. Animals were placed on a platform in the center of the square. A virtual cylinder comprised of a vertical sine wave grating was projected in 3D coordinate space and rotated around the animal. A video camera, situated above the animal, provided real time feedback on another screen. Rats were allowed to move freely on the platform, and the spatial frequency of the grating was maintained at the animal's viewing position by recentering the cylinder on the rat's head. The cylinder was rotated at a constant speed (12°/s). 
The experimenter judged whether the rats made tracking motions with reflexive head and neck movements following the stimulus. Animals were assessed for tracking behavior for 5 seconds, and then a gray stimulus appeared, to reduce the possibility of adapting to the stimulus. Spatial frequency thresholds were calculated by systematically increasing the spatial frequency of the grating at 100% contrast until the animals no longer responded. This threshold was considered the maximum VA. A CS curve was generated by identifying the minimum contrast that generates tracking over a range of spatial frequencies. 
Filter Specifications
The grating was modified with colored filters (green, red, and purple) placed in front of the screens. Transmittance of the filters from 300 to 850 nm was measured in a spectrophotometer, and curves of transmittance were plotted (Fig. 1). The total visible transmittance was 69.82%, 23.56%, and 13.76% for the green, red, and purple filter, respectively. Neutral density (ND) 12% filters were used in combination with colored filters to reduce the luminance. In that case, transmittance was 9.61%, 2.54%, and 1.46%, respectively. 
Figure 1
 
Curves of transmittance of colored filters from 300 to 850 nm. Red, continuous line; green, dashed line; purple, dotted line.
Figure 1
 
Curves of transmittance of colored filters from 300 to 850 nm. Red, continuous line; green, dashed line; purple, dotted line.
Colored filters were selected as a consequence of the peaks of maximum response in S-cones (358 nm) and ML-cones (509 nm). The green filter was selected with the closest curve of transmittance to the ML-cones.27 The purple filter allowed a good transmission between 370 and 480 nm, and after 590 nm, to minimize the ML-cone response. The red filter gave a good value of transmittance only after 570 nm. 
ERG Recordings
Pigmented P23H rats were studied by ERG recordings at P30, P150, and P240. Eight animals were studied for each time point. Eight normal LE rats at age P90 were used as wild-type controls. In both groups, four animals were tested with each colored filter and another four rats without filters. 
Rats were adapted to darkness overnight and prepared for recording under a dim red light. Animals were anesthetized with an intraperitoneal injection of a mixture of ketamine (90 mg/kg) and xylazine (10 mg/kg), and maintained on a heating pad at a stable temperature. The ketamine/xylazine anesthetic shows robust ERGs with large a-wave and b-wave amplitudes, and low eye movements.28 Pupils were dilated by applying a topical drop of 1% tropicamide (Colircusí Tropicamida; Alcon, Barcelona, Spain). A topical drop of 2% Methocel (OmniVision, Puchheim, Germany) was instilled in each eye before situating the corneal electrode. Furthermore, a drop of 0.9% saline was applied occasionally to the cornea to prevent dehydration and to allow electrical contact with the recording electrode (gold wire loop). Two 25-gauge platinum needles inserted under the scalp, behind the eyes, served as the reference electrodes, with a ground electrode located in the tail. All experiments were performed in absolute darkness. Stimulus presentation and data acquisition were provided by the Espion system from Diagnosys LLC (Cambridge, UK). 
Mixed b-Wave
Dark-adapted b-waves show aggregate rod and cone pathway contributions. To describe them, recordings of 3 to 8 single flash presentations of 10 μs duration were displayed. Stimuli were presented at 10 increasing intensities varying from −3.70 to 2.86 log cd/m2 in luminance. Interstimuli intervals (ISI) were increased to minimize the effects of bleaching on the rods, which could reduce the b-wave amplitude during successive flashes. The ISI was elevated from 10 seconds at lowest stimulus intensity (−3.70 log cd/m2) up to 120 seconds at highest stimulus intensity (2.86 log cd/m2). The amplitude of the a-wave was measured from the baseline to the trough of the a-wave. The amplitude of the b-wave was measured from the trough of the a-wave up to the peak of the b-wave. The results of a- and b-waves were averaged for different recordings. To determine if the ERG response remained, criterion amplitudes were established at 20 μV for a- and b-waves. 
Isolation of the Cone Response Using a Double Flash Protocol
The double flash protocol was similar to previous studies.29 A probe flash was presented 1 second after a conditioning flash. The role of the first flash is to temporarily saturate rods so that they do not respond to the probe flash. The necessary intensity of the conditioning flash for complete rod bleaching was set to 1.4 log cd/m2. The probe flash intensity also was 1.4 log cd/m2. The response to the probe flash, preceded by the conditioning flash, was taken as a reflection of cone-driven activity. A rod-driven b-wave was obtained by subtracting the cone-driven response from the mixed response (obtained by the conditioning flash alone). The results were averaged for 3 recordings, with an ISI of 100 seconds to assure full recovery of rod responsiveness. 
Results
VA and CS Evaluation
In P23H rats, VA was progressively lost with age as shown in the measurements without filters (Fig. 2). Visual acuity values were 0.542 ± 0.011 cycles/deg for LE and 0.445 ± 0.002 cycles/deg for P23H at P30, and 0.424 ± 0.008 cycles/deg at P150 and 0.347 ± 0.019 cycles/deg at P240. In the LE group, smaller values with the green filter (0.508 ± 0.010 cycles/deg) and even smaller values with the other filters (0.438 ± 0.006 cycles/deg for ND 12% to 0.346 ± 0.008 cycles/deg for red + ND 12%) were observed. Smaller differences were found in the P23H groups between measured values without a filter and the worst value from colored filters (0.042 cycles/deg at P30, 0.020 cycles/deg at P150, and 0.040 cycles/deg at P240). 
Figure 2
 
Visual acuity of LE (P90) and P23H rats (P30, P150, and P240) using different filters. Error bars: SEM.
Figure 2
 
Visual acuity of LE (P90) and P23H rats (P30, P150, and P240) using different filters. Error bars: SEM.
A similar trend was found in CS curves (Fig. 3). In the LE rats, peaks of 52.63 ± 5.43 (with no filter) and 35.13 ± 1.23 (ND 12% filter) were obtained for a spatial frequency of 0.089 cycles/deg. The filter addition gave smaller values, between 26.99 ± 1.60 and 18.36 ± 0.64. The behavior was different with the green filter, which gave similar values to those obtained without filters, 49.29 ± 2.87. Similar results were found in the P23H groups. Peaks of 39.86 ± 0.85 and 33.87 ± 0.94 (P30), 31.11 ± 1.16 and 25.99 ± 1.21 (P150), and 31.54 ± 1.78 and 26.81 ± 1.97 (P240) were obtained for measurements without filters and with the ND 12% filter, respectively. Colored filters gave smaller values, between 24.47 ± 0.95 and 17.99 ± 0.57 (P30, excluding the values obtained with the green filter that were similar to those obtained without filters), 24.95 ± 1.09 and 18.38 ± 0.89 (P150), and 21.72 ± 1.28 and 12.17 ± 0.77 (P240). As expected, LE rats had better results in all cases. Similar results without filters and with green filters were observed between LE and P23H P30 rat values. Differences between groups were lower with red and purple filters. 
Figure 3
 
Contrast sensitivity as a function of spatial frequency with colored filters and adding an ND filter (ND 12%). Measures of the wild-type control LE (black circle), P23H at P30 (white circle), P150 (triangle), and P240 (square) were done. Each point represents the mean of 8 animals. Error bars: SEM.
Figure 3
 
Contrast sensitivity as a function of spatial frequency with colored filters and adding an ND filter (ND 12%). Measures of the wild-type control LE (black circle), P23H at P30 (white circle), P150 (triangle), and P240 (square) were done. Each point represents the mean of 8 animals. Error bars: SEM.
Relationship Between Visual Parameters and Luminance
Luminance of the screens with different filters was measured by a luminance meter LP 471 Lum 2 (Delta Ohm, Padua, Italy). Visual acuity (Fig. 4A) and CS results (Fig. 4B) were represented as a function of luminance. The LE rats' VA showed a strong relationship with luminance, achieving an R2 value of 0.94. No relationships were found in the P23H rat groups, with R2 values <0.7. Furthermore, a relationship of CS with luminance also was observed in the LE rats (R2 = 0.91) and the P23H rats at P30 (R2 = 0.85), and a lower relationship was found at P150 (R2 = 0.73). 
Figure 4
 
Visual acuity (A) and contrast sensitivity (B) as a function of luminance from normal LE rats (black circle) recorded at age P90 and from pigmented P23H rats recorded at P30 (white circle), P150 (triangle), and P240 (square). Each point represents the mean of 8 rats. Vertical bars: SEM. Lines of fit with R2 > 0.8 are plotted: R2 = 0.94 in the LE group for VA (A), R2 = 0.91 and R2 = 0.85 in LE rats (continuous line) and the P23H group at P30 (dashed line), respectively, for contrast sensitivity (B).
Figure 4
 
Visual acuity (A) and contrast sensitivity (B) as a function of luminance from normal LE rats (black circle) recorded at age P90 and from pigmented P23H rats recorded at P30 (white circle), P150 (triangle), and P240 (square). Each point represents the mean of 8 rats. Vertical bars: SEM. Lines of fit with R2 > 0.8 are plotted: R2 = 0.94 in the LE group for VA (A), R2 = 0.91 and R2 = 0.85 in LE rats (continuous line) and the P23H group at P30 (dashed line), respectively, for contrast sensitivity (B).
ERG Recordings
In pigmented P23H rats, the scotopic a-wave (Fig. 5A) reached a maximum value of 113 ± 10 μV at P30. This value represents 37% of the same value in normal LE rats (304 ± 34 μV). Despite being affected at early ages, a-waves from P23H rats still could be evoked to P150 (25 ± 5 μV), but they were almost negligible at P240 (<20 μV), the latest age studied. At all ages of P23H rats, b-waves (Fig. 5B) were less affected than a-waves, reaching maximum values of 1092 ± 38 μV (P30), 475 ± 45 μV (P150), and 301 ± 21 μV (P240) with regard to 1178 ± 101 μV (LE at P90). 
Figure 5
 
Electroretinography amplitude versus luminance series of mixed scotopic a-waves (A) and of mixed scotopic b-waves (B) from normal LE rats (black circle) recorded at P90, and from pigmented P23H rats recorded at P30 (white circle), P150 (triangle), and P240 (square). Each point represents the mean of 8 animals. Vertical bars: SEM. (C) Application of the double flash protocol in LE (P90) and P23H rats (P30, P150, and P240). Left traces: mixed response obtained with a conditioning flash alone. Middle traces: isolated cone response obtained with a probe flash preceded by the conditioning flash. Right traces: rod response obtained by subtracting the isolated cone response from the mixed b-wave. Stimulus presentation is at the beginning of each trace. Each trace represents the mean of 8 rats.
Figure 5
 
Electroretinography amplitude versus luminance series of mixed scotopic a-waves (A) and of mixed scotopic b-waves (B) from normal LE rats (black circle) recorded at P90, and from pigmented P23H rats recorded at P30 (white circle), P150 (triangle), and P240 (square). Each point represents the mean of 8 animals. Vertical bars: SEM. (C) Application of the double flash protocol in LE (P90) and P23H rats (P30, P150, and P240). Left traces: mixed response obtained with a conditioning flash alone. Middle traces: isolated cone response obtained with a probe flash preceded by the conditioning flash. Right traces: rod response obtained by subtracting the isolated cone response from the mixed b-wave. Stimulus presentation is at the beginning of each trace. Each trace represents the mean of 8 rats.
Figure 5C illustrates the results of the ERG recordings obtained by the application of the double flash protocol from control LE (P90) and P23H rats at P30, P150, and P240. The first flash elicited a mixed rod-cone response (left traces), and the second flash (one second delay) elicited a pure cone response (middle traces). The right traces were calculated by subtracting the middle traces from the left traces to obtain the rod-driven contribution to the ERG response. The rod-driven responses were affected by age in the P23H rats: a- and b-waves reached values of 40 and 391 μV at P30, <20 and 188 μV at P150, and 32 and 109 μV at P240, compared to 165 and 516 μV in the LE rats. The rod-driven contribution to the mixed scotopic b-waves was higher in LE rats (46%) compared to P23H rats, remaining stable for all ages (38% at P30, 40% at P150, and 37% at P240). 
Figure 6 shows the ERG amplitudes as a function of luminance and the results of the double flash protocol from the LE (P90) and P23H rats (P30, P150, and P240), measured with different colored filters. In the P23H group, similar results in the a- and b-wave values were obtained by adding different filters (Figs. 6A, 6B), excluding the P30 a-wave (value changed from 65 μV with the red filter to 152 μV without the filter) and the b-wave at P150 (from 309 μV without the filter to 608 μV with the green filter). Larger differences were observed in LE rats, but we were not able to identify any pattern between a- and b-waves and colored filters (Figs. 6A, 6B). 
Figure 6
 
Electroretinographic amplitudes as a function of luminance and the application of the double flash protocol from LE (P90) and P23H rats (P30, P150, and P240) measured with different colored filters. For each group of rodents: ERG amplitude versus luminance series of mixed scotopic a-waves (A) and of mixed scotopic b-waves (B) from rats without filters (black circle), and with green (white circle), red (triangle), and purple (square) filters. Each point represents the mean of 4 rats. Vertical bars: SEM. (C) Application of the double flash protocol in rats without filters, and with green, red and purple filters. Left traces: mixed response obtained with a conditioning flash alone. Middle traces: isolated cone response obtained with a probe flash preceded by the conditioning flash. Right traces: rod response obtained by subtracting the isolated cone response from the mixed b-wave. Stimulus presentation is at beginning of each trace. Each trace represents the mean of 4 rats.
Figure 6
 
Electroretinographic amplitudes as a function of luminance and the application of the double flash protocol from LE (P90) and P23H rats (P30, P150, and P240) measured with different colored filters. For each group of rodents: ERG amplitude versus luminance series of mixed scotopic a-waves (A) and of mixed scotopic b-waves (B) from rats without filters (black circle), and with green (white circle), red (triangle), and purple (square) filters. Each point represents the mean of 4 rats. Vertical bars: SEM. (C) Application of the double flash protocol in rats without filters, and with green, red and purple filters. Left traces: mixed response obtained with a conditioning flash alone. Middle traces: isolated cone response obtained with a probe flash preceded by the conditioning flash. Right traces: rod response obtained by subtracting the isolated cone response from the mixed b-wave. Stimulus presentation is at beginning of each trace. Each trace represents the mean of 4 rats.
There were differences in the a-waves of rod-driven responses in all groups (Fig. 6C). A-waves from P23H rats were negligible (<20 μV) when using red and purple filters for all ages. In LE rats, the results were smaller with the same filters (52 μV for red and 160 μV for purple, compared to 242 μV for green and 266 μV without a filter). No clear relationship between the b-wave of rod-driven responses and colored filters was found. However, there were differences in the rod-driven contribution to mixed scotopic b-waves, with a higher percentage using green filters (more than 40% in all cases) and no filters (more than 50%) compared to red and purple filters (less than 40% and, except one case, less than 33%). 
Discussion
The present study has applied functional testing (OKT and ERG) to an animal model of progressive retinal degeneration (pigmented P23H rat) and a wild-type control (LE rat) to evaluate the photoreceptor response, and the relative contributions of rods and cones in different lighting conditions. To our knowledge, there have been no examples of using these functional tests with in vivo photoreceptor responses and adding color filters to isolate the different photoreceptor responses. 
It long has been known that rodents have two types of cones.30 Most cone photoreceptors are ML-cones, responsible for the green photopic sensitivity of the rat. The role of the S-cones under photopic lighting conditions has been less studied. Based on the peaks of maximum absorbance of cones and rods, we selected colored filters with a specific spectral sensitivity curve (Fig. 1) for isolating the responses of different photoreceptors in vivo. Using this method, new information could be obtained about the changes in the different cone photoreceptors following the rod loss in this autosomal model of rhodopsin gene mutation. Cone evaluation and preservation should be one of the main issues after therapeutic approaches. 
Comparing the results obtained with age, a decrease in visual function (VA and CS) was observed in P23H rats, consistent with the progressive cell loss in this retinal degeneration model. Thus, decreased values of VA (above 0.300 cycles/deg) and CS (above 30, or 3.3%) were found at advanced ages in degenerative rats. The late loss of cone function (compared to rods) in the rhodopsin mutation transgenic rat line31,32 could be an explanation of the results. 
Considering the results given with colored filters (Fig. 2), differences of VA between measures without filters and the worst value with colored filters in the P23H rats (0.042 cycles/deg at P30, 0.020 cycles/deg at P150, and 0.040 cycles/deg at P240) were small. Larger differences were found in LE rats (from 0.542 cycles/deg without filters to 0.346 cycles/deg, the worst obtained value with colored filters). The same trend was observed in the CS curves (Fig. 3), although the differences were larger. With the choice of these filters, a good correlation between no filter and the green filter (as in LE and P23H P30 groups) would be expected, with no response or a very low response using the red and purple filters due to the possible contribution of S-cones in photopic vision. However, the minimum thresholds of VA (above 0.300 cycles/deg) and CS (above 12, or 8.3%) were obtained again. The minimum transmittance of the colored filters (<7% at 509 nm, peak of the M-cones) and that the light levels were within the photopic range (with a lowest luminance value of 1.5 cd/m2) were enough to generate an OKT response, which could explain these results. 
If VA and CS are plotted as a function of luminance (Fig. 4) as others have done,33 a good correlation is observed in LE rats for VA (R2 = 0.94) as CS (R2 = 0.91) and for CS (R2 = 0.085) in P23H at P30 regardless of the colored filter used. A stronger relationship between OKT response and luminance in the slightly degenerate and nondegenerate retina could be an explanation for these results. 
Recently, it has been observed that modest increases in ambient light cause a rapid and significantly reversible loss of cone and rod function.34 Cones are damaged rapidly in the rat retina when rod damage is accelerated by raising ambient illumination. There is a close dependence of cone integrity on rod integrity, compared to the life-long persistence of cone function in the scotopic rat.32 Reducing environmental lighting35 in combination with other treatments36 may delay retinal degeneration arising from rhodopsin mutations. Using green filters to repeatedly test the effect of therapeutic interventions in rats in vivo could minimize the light-induced damage. Greater age-related variations were obtained with green filters, as well as larger values, so significant changes could be easily distinguished from statistical deviations (Fig. 7). 
Figure 7
 
Contrast sensitivity (at a frequency of 0.089 cycles/deg) as a function of age in P23H rats measured without filters (black circle) and with colored filters: green (white circle), red (triangle), and purple (square). Each point represents the mean of 8 rats. Lines of fit are plotted: without filters (continuous line), and green (medium dashed line), red (dashed line), and purple filters (dotted line).
Figure 7
 
Contrast sensitivity (at a frequency of 0.089 cycles/deg) as a function of age in P23H rats measured without filters (black circle) and with colored filters: green (white circle), red (triangle), and purple (square). Each point represents the mean of 8 rats. Lines of fit are plotted: without filters (continuous line), and green (medium dashed line), red (dashed line), and purple filters (dotted line).
The decreased ERG response with age also was clear, as already has been described (Fig. 5). The a-wave change was particularly clear, and the a-wave appears to be a better parameter than the b-wave to judge the efficacy of therapeutic manipulation in this rat model of retinal disease generated by photoreceptors.10,18 Values of 304 ± 34 μV for the LE rats and 113 ± 10 μV (P30), 25 ± 5 μV (P150), and <20 μV (no response, P240) for the P23H rats were consistent with the gradual loss of photoreceptors. So, the long-term survival of some cones in transgenic rats could not be responsible for the minimum values of VA and CS, considering the ERG results. Thus, minimum OKT thresholds are achievable in pigmented P23H rats without a full complement of photoreceptors, and it appears that rods and cones may not be necessary for minimum values of VA and CS, as others have concluded.37 
In P23H rats, there were no marked differences in most of the a- and b-waves with different colored filters (Figs. 6A, 6B), except in the P30 a-wave and the P150 b-wave, which could be related to the smaller measurements with the red filter and without a filter, respectively. In LE rats, a similar pattern between a- and b-waves and colored filters was not found (Figs. 6A, 6B). However, the worst results were given by red filter, which has been shown to be the most limiting in the photoreceptor response. 
The results of the ERG recordings obtained by the application of the double flash protocol showed differences in all groups (Fig. 6C). A-waves of rod-driven responses from P23H rats were negligible (<20 mV) using red and purple filters for all ages. In LE rats, smaller results were found using the same filters. Moreover, there were differences in the rod-driven contribution to mixed scotopic b-waves, with larger values with green filters (>40%) and without filters (>50%) when compared to red and purple filters (<33%). These results, although not conclusive, are in agreement with the filter characteristics. Using more animals to study each filter, evaluating the possible influence of the anesthesia,28 and choosing filters with a more suitable transmittance curve could allow a more detailed analysis. 
In conclusion, visual function parameters decrease with age in pigmented P23H rats. Irrespective of luminance, color filter, and retinal degeneration, minimum thresholds of visual acuity and contrast sensitivity were found. Smaller differences than expected were found using color filters, which might reflect that color differences are of minor contribution for rat vision. Responses to functional testing at long wavelengths were observed, where there is very low photoreceptor spectral sensitivity. The use of filters with functional testing could minimize light-induced retinal damage in rats. 
Acknowledgments
Supported by the Spanish Ministry of Health ISCIII (PI13/01124, PS0901854), DGA Group B99, ISCIII RETICS RD12/0034/0010, and by a Zaragoza University Grant FPUZ-2011-BIO-02 (FS). 
Disclosure: F. Segura, None; A. Sánchez-Cano, None; S. Jarabo, None; C. López de la Fuente, None; N. Cuenca, None; M.P. Villegas-Pérez, None; I. Pinilla, None 
References
Walls GL. The visual cells of the white rat. J Comp Psychol. 1934 ; 18: 363–366.
Szél Á, Röhlich P, Caffé AR, Van Veen T. Distribution of cone photoreceptors in the mammalian retina. Microsc Res Tech. 1996 ; 35: 445–462.
Jacobs GH, Fenwick JA, Williams GA. Cone-based vision of rats for ultraviolet and visible lights. J Exp Biol. 2001 ; 204: 2439–2446.
Szél Á, Röhlich P. Two cone types of rat retina detected by anti-visual pigment antibodies. Exp Eye Res. 1992 ; 55: 47–52.
Jacobs GH, Williams GA, Fenwick JA. Influence of cone pigment coexpression on spectral sensitivity and color vision in the mouse. Vision Res. 2004 ; 44: 1615–1622.
Hartong DT, Berson EL, Dryja TP. Retinitis pigmentosa. Lancet. 2006 ; 368: 1795–1809.
Congdon N. Causes and prevalence of visual impairment among adults in the United States. Arch Ophthalmol. 2004 ; 122: 477–485.
Dryja TP. Retinitis pigmentosa and stationary night blindness. In: Scriver C Beaudet A, Sly W, Valle D. The Metabolic and Molecular Bases of Inherited Disease. New York: McGraw-Hill; 2001: 5903–5933.
Wilson JH, Wensel TG. The nature of dominant mutations of rhodopsin and implications for gene therapy. Mol Neurobiol. 2003 ; 28: 149–158.
Cuenca N, Fernández-Sánchez L, Sauvé Y, et al. Correlation between SD-OCT, immunocytochemistry and functional findings in an animal model of retinal degeneration. Front Neuroanat. 2014; 8: 151.
Roberts JE. Ocular photoxicity. J Photochem Photobiol B. 2001 ; 64: 136–143.
Organisciak DT, Vaughan DK. Retinal light damage: mechanisms and protection. Prog Retin Eye Res. 2010; 29: 113–134.
LaVail MM, Gorrin GM, Repaci MA, Thomas LA, Ginsberg HM. Genetic regulation of light damage to photoreceptors. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1987 ; 28: 1043–1048.
LaVail MM, Gorrin GM, Repaci MA. Strain differences in sensitivity to light-induced photoreceptor degeneration in albino mice. Curr Eye Res. 1987 ; 6: 825–834.
Naash MI, LaVail MM, Anderson RE. Factors affecting the susceptibility of the retina to light damage. Prog Clin Biol Res. 1989 ; 314: 513–522.
LaVail MM, Gorrin GM, Repaci MA, Yasumura D. Light-induced retinal degeneration in albino mice and rats: Strain and species differences. Prog Clin Biol Res. 1987 ; 247: 439–454.
Vaughan DK, Coulibaly SF, Darrow RM, Organisciak DT. A morphometric study of light-induced damage in transgenic rat models of retinitis pigmentosa. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2003 ; 44: 848–855.
Machida S, Kondo M, Jamison JA, et al. P23H rhodopsin transgenic rat: correlation of retinal function with histopathology. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2000; 41: 3200–3209.
Pinilla I, Lund RD, Sauvé Y. Enhanced cone dysfunction in rats homozygous for the P23H rhodopsin mutation. Neurosci Lett. 2005 ; 382: 16–21.
Cuenca N, Pinilla I, Sauvé Y, Lu B, Wang S, Lund RD. Regressive and reactive changes in the connectivity patterns of rod and cone pathways of P23H transgenic rat retina. Neuroscience. 2004 ; 127: 301–317.
Prusky GT, Alam NM, Beekman S, Douglas RM. Rapid quantification of adult and developing mouse spatial vision using a virtual optomotor system. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2004 ; 45: 4611–4616.
Douglas RM, Alam NM, Silver BD, Mcgill TJ, Tschetter WW, Prusky GT. Independent visual threshold measurements in the two eyes of freely moving rats and mice using a virtual-reality optokinetic system. Vis Neurosci. 2005 ; 22: 677–684.
Prusky GT, Alam NM, Douglas RM. Enhancement of vision by monocular deprivation in adult mice. J Neurosci. 2006 ; 26: 11554–11561.
Umino Y, Everhart D, Solessio E, et al. Hypoglycemia leads to age-related loss of vision. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2006; 103: 19541–19545.
Alexander JJ, Umino Y, Everhart D, et al. Restoration of cone vision in a mouse model of achromatopsia. Nat Med. 2007 ; 13: 685–687.
McGill TJ, Lund RD, Douglas RM, et al. Syngeneic Schwann cell transplantation preserves vision in RCS rat without immunosuppression. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2007; 48: 1906–1912.
Radlwimmer FB, Yokoyama S. Genetic analyses of the green visual pigments of rabbit (oryctolagus cuniculus) and rat (rattus norvegicus). Gene. 1998 ; 218: 103–109.
Nair G, Kim M, Nagaoka T, et al. Effects of common anesthetics on eye movement and electroretinogram. Doc Ophthalmol. 2011 ; 122: 163–176.
Pinilla I, Lund RD, Sauvé Y. Contribution of rod and cone pathways to the dark-adapted electroretinogram (ERG) b-wave following retinal degeneration in RCS rats. Vision Res. 2004 ; 44: 2467–2474.
Jacobs GH, Neitz J, Deegan JF II, Retinal receptors in rodents maximally sensitive to ultraviolet light. Nature. 1991; 353: 655–656.
Chrysostomou V, Stone J, Valter K. Life history of cones in the rhodopsin-mutant p23h-3 rat: Evidence of long-term survival. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2009 ; 50: 2407–2416.
Chrysostomou V, Valter K, Stone J. Cone-rod dependence in the rat retina: variation with the rate of rod damage. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2009; 50: 3017–3023.
Umino Y, Solessio E, Barlow RB. Speed spatial, and temporal tuning of rod and cone vision in mouse. J Neurosci. 2008 ; 28: 189–198.
Chrysostomou V, Stone J, Stowe S, Barnett NL, Valter K. The status of cones in the rhodopsin mutant P23H-3 retina: light-regulated damage and repair in parallel with rods. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2008; 49: 1116–1125.
Organisciak DT, Darrow RM, Barsalou L, Kutty RK, Wiggert B. Susceptibility to retinal light damage in transgenic rats with rhodopsin mutations. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2003 ; 44: 486–492.
Fernández-Sánchez L, Lax P, Pinilla I, Martín-Nieto J, Cuenca N. Tauroursodeoxycholic acid prevents retinal degeneration in transgenic P23H rats. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2011 ; 52: 4998–5008.
McGill TJ, Prusky GT, Douglas RM, et al. Discordant anatomical, electrophysiological, and visual behavioral profiles of retinal degeneration in rat models of retinal degenerative disease. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2012 ; 53: 6232–6244.
Figure 1
 
Curves of transmittance of colored filters from 300 to 850 nm. Red, continuous line; green, dashed line; purple, dotted line.
Figure 1
 
Curves of transmittance of colored filters from 300 to 850 nm. Red, continuous line; green, dashed line; purple, dotted line.
Figure 2
 
Visual acuity of LE (P90) and P23H rats (P30, P150, and P240) using different filters. Error bars: SEM.
Figure 2
 
Visual acuity of LE (P90) and P23H rats (P30, P150, and P240) using different filters. Error bars: SEM.
Figure 3
 
Contrast sensitivity as a function of spatial frequency with colored filters and adding an ND filter (ND 12%). Measures of the wild-type control LE (black circle), P23H at P30 (white circle), P150 (triangle), and P240 (square) were done. Each point represents the mean of 8 animals. Error bars: SEM.
Figure 3
 
Contrast sensitivity as a function of spatial frequency with colored filters and adding an ND filter (ND 12%). Measures of the wild-type control LE (black circle), P23H at P30 (white circle), P150 (triangle), and P240 (square) were done. Each point represents the mean of 8 animals. Error bars: SEM.
Figure 4
 
Visual acuity (A) and contrast sensitivity (B) as a function of luminance from normal LE rats (black circle) recorded at age P90 and from pigmented P23H rats recorded at P30 (white circle), P150 (triangle), and P240 (square). Each point represents the mean of 8 rats. Vertical bars: SEM. Lines of fit with R2 > 0.8 are plotted: R2 = 0.94 in the LE group for VA (A), R2 = 0.91 and R2 = 0.85 in LE rats (continuous line) and the P23H group at P30 (dashed line), respectively, for contrast sensitivity (B).
Figure 4
 
Visual acuity (A) and contrast sensitivity (B) as a function of luminance from normal LE rats (black circle) recorded at age P90 and from pigmented P23H rats recorded at P30 (white circle), P150 (triangle), and P240 (square). Each point represents the mean of 8 rats. Vertical bars: SEM. Lines of fit with R2 > 0.8 are plotted: R2 = 0.94 in the LE group for VA (A), R2 = 0.91 and R2 = 0.85 in LE rats (continuous line) and the P23H group at P30 (dashed line), respectively, for contrast sensitivity (B).
Figure 5
 
Electroretinography amplitude versus luminance series of mixed scotopic a-waves (A) and of mixed scotopic b-waves (B) from normal LE rats (black circle) recorded at P90, and from pigmented P23H rats recorded at P30 (white circle), P150 (triangle), and P240 (square). Each point represents the mean of 8 animals. Vertical bars: SEM. (C) Application of the double flash protocol in LE (P90) and P23H rats (P30, P150, and P240). Left traces: mixed response obtained with a conditioning flash alone. Middle traces: isolated cone response obtained with a probe flash preceded by the conditioning flash. Right traces: rod response obtained by subtracting the isolated cone response from the mixed b-wave. Stimulus presentation is at the beginning of each trace. Each trace represents the mean of 8 rats.
Figure 5
 
Electroretinography amplitude versus luminance series of mixed scotopic a-waves (A) and of mixed scotopic b-waves (B) from normal LE rats (black circle) recorded at P90, and from pigmented P23H rats recorded at P30 (white circle), P150 (triangle), and P240 (square). Each point represents the mean of 8 animals. Vertical bars: SEM. (C) Application of the double flash protocol in LE (P90) and P23H rats (P30, P150, and P240). Left traces: mixed response obtained with a conditioning flash alone. Middle traces: isolated cone response obtained with a probe flash preceded by the conditioning flash. Right traces: rod response obtained by subtracting the isolated cone response from the mixed b-wave. Stimulus presentation is at the beginning of each trace. Each trace represents the mean of 8 rats.
Figure 6
 
Electroretinographic amplitudes as a function of luminance and the application of the double flash protocol from LE (P90) and P23H rats (P30, P150, and P240) measured with different colored filters. For each group of rodents: ERG amplitude versus luminance series of mixed scotopic a-waves (A) and of mixed scotopic b-waves (B) from rats without filters (black circle), and with green (white circle), red (triangle), and purple (square) filters. Each point represents the mean of 4 rats. Vertical bars: SEM. (C) Application of the double flash protocol in rats without filters, and with green, red and purple filters. Left traces: mixed response obtained with a conditioning flash alone. Middle traces: isolated cone response obtained with a probe flash preceded by the conditioning flash. Right traces: rod response obtained by subtracting the isolated cone response from the mixed b-wave. Stimulus presentation is at beginning of each trace. Each trace represents the mean of 4 rats.
Figure 6
 
Electroretinographic amplitudes as a function of luminance and the application of the double flash protocol from LE (P90) and P23H rats (P30, P150, and P240) measured with different colored filters. For each group of rodents: ERG amplitude versus luminance series of mixed scotopic a-waves (A) and of mixed scotopic b-waves (B) from rats without filters (black circle), and with green (white circle), red (triangle), and purple (square) filters. Each point represents the mean of 4 rats. Vertical bars: SEM. (C) Application of the double flash protocol in rats without filters, and with green, red and purple filters. Left traces: mixed response obtained with a conditioning flash alone. Middle traces: isolated cone response obtained with a probe flash preceded by the conditioning flash. Right traces: rod response obtained by subtracting the isolated cone response from the mixed b-wave. Stimulus presentation is at beginning of each trace. Each trace represents the mean of 4 rats.
Figure 7
 
Contrast sensitivity (at a frequency of 0.089 cycles/deg) as a function of age in P23H rats measured without filters (black circle) and with colored filters: green (white circle), red (triangle), and purple (square). Each point represents the mean of 8 rats. Lines of fit are plotted: without filters (continuous line), and green (medium dashed line), red (dashed line), and purple filters (dotted line).
Figure 7
 
Contrast sensitivity (at a frequency of 0.089 cycles/deg) as a function of age in P23H rats measured without filters (black circle) and with colored filters: green (white circle), red (triangle), and purple (square). Each point represents the mean of 8 rats. Lines of fit are plotted: without filters (continuous line), and green (medium dashed line), red (dashed line), and purple filters (dotted line).
×
×

This PDF is available to Subscribers Only

Sign in or purchase a subscription to access this content. ×

You must be signed into an individual account to use this feature.

×