As we were interested in determining the benefit of depth perception, we used a peg-placement task with a novel arrangement of the pegboard (Geometric Peg Board 5125; Plan Toys, Plan Creations Co. Ltd., Bangkok, Thailand) with respect to the participant. Instead of placing the pegboard on a table and affording observers a top-down view, we placed the pegboard just below eye height so that depth perception would help greatly in peg placement (
Fig. 1). The pegboard was 30 cm above the table, at a distance of 40 cm from the observer. The observers' head was positioned so that their eyes were 10 cm above the pegboard. The pegboard had four rows, each with four unique shapes—circle, rectangle, triangle, and square. Observers were required to pick up a peg from a fixed position (
Fig. 1, black dot; middle of the second row in the pegboard) and place it in the appropriate position in either the first or third row of the pegboard, as instructed by the experimenter. The first and third row had relative disparities of 15 and 17 arcminutes with respect to the starting position of the peg. Participants were seated with their chin and forehead resting in a head mount, which minimized head movement. Their grasping hand rested on the table at the start of the trial, with thumb and index (middle) finger resting on Velcro markers. Participants were given considerable practice with the task, until they were familiar with it. They were allowed to use either index or middle finger to grasp the block, depending on their preference. A Polhemus (Colchester, VT, USA) Liberty system sampling at 240 Hz was used to record three-dimensional movement data from sensors attached to the thumb, index (or middle) finger, and the wrist. Sensor position was measured with respect to a magnet placed under the table. Grip aperture was calculated as the distance between the thumb and index (middle) finger in three-dimensional space. Instantaneous hand velocity was computed as the average change in position of the thumb and finger sensor between consecutive frames. Hand velocity was smoothed with a temporal Gaussian filter with a temporal
σ of 100 ms. Binocular eye movements were recorded by an EyeLink 1000 Eye Tracker (SR Research, Ottawa, ON, Canada) in a table-mount configuration, angled to image both eyes of the participant. The red and green dots on the pegboard in
Figures 1A and
1B are calibration marks for the eye tracker. Despite positioning the eye tracker optimally during a five-point calibration at the start of each block, the following combination of factors contributed to a loss of track of one or both eye during the block: eccentric eye gaze in patients, increased noise from the eye tracker due to the table-mount configuration, and patients potentially pitching their head slightly during the block.