We then analyzed the influence of fixation on the saccade vector evoked for each eye (when that eye was fixating versus when it was not) in monocular and binocular conditions, as summarized in
Figure 7. In the binocular condition (solid lines), the evoked saccades of the dominant (in black) or the nondominant (in gray) eye were only slightly affected by fixation. For monkey ET1, the amplitude ratios (
Fig. 7A) were very close to 1 for the dominant eye (mean: 1.01, CI: 0.99–1.04,
N = 8) and for the nondominant eye (mean: 0.99, CI: 0.96–1.01,
N = 8). The amplitude ratios for the dominant eye and nondominant eye of monkey XT1 (
Fig. 7B) were also very close to 1 even though the dominant eye showed a little more variability (dominant eye: 1.11, CI: 1.03–1.19,
N = 18; nondominant eye: 1.03; CI: 0.96–1.02,
N = 18). The saccade direction deviations (
Figs. 7C,
7D) were mostly less than 5° for monkeys ET1 (dominant eye: 88% [7/8]; nondominant eye: 75% [6/8],
Fig. 7C) and XT1 (dominant eye: 72% [13/18]; nondominant eye: 56% [10/18],
Fig. 7D). In this binocular viewing condition (solid lines), the two eyes (dominant versus nondominant eye in black and gray, respectively) had similar sensitivities to the fixation because we did not find any significant difference between their amplitude ratios and direction differences in monkey ET1 (
P > 0.05, Kolmogorov-Smirnov test,
N = 8). In monkey XT1, only the amplitude ratios were significantly different between the dominant and nondominant eyes (
P < 0.05, Kolmogorov-Smirnov test,
N = 18).