Abstract
Purpose:
To evaluate whether computerized training with a crowded or uncrowded letter-discrimination task reduces visual impairment (VI) in 6- to 11-year-old children with infantile nystagmus (IN) who suffer from increased foveal crowding, reduced visual acuity, and reduced stereopsis.
Methods:
Thirty-six children with IN were included. Eighteen had idiopathic IN and 18 had oculocutaneous albinism. These children were divided in two training groups matched on age and diagnosis: a crowded training group (n = 18) and an uncrowded training group (n = 18). Training occurred two times per week during 5 weeks (3500 trials per training). Eleven age-matched children with normal vision were included to assess baseline differences in task performance and test–retest learning. Main outcome measures were task-specific performance, distance and near visual acuity (DVA and NVA), intensity and extent of (foveal) crowding at 5 m and 40 cm, and stereopsis.
Results:
Training resulted in task-specific improvements. Both training groups also showed uncrowded and crowded DVA improvements (0.10 ± 0.02 and 0.11 ± 0.02 logMAR) and improved stereopsis (670 ± 249″). Crowded NVA improved only in the crowded training group (0.15 ± 0.02 logMAR), which was also the only group showing a reduction in near crowding intensity (0.08 ± 0.03 logMAR). Effects were not due to test–retest learning.
Conclusions:
Perceptual learning with or without distractors reduces the extent of crowding and improves visual acuity in children with IN. Training with distractors improves near vision more than training with single optotypes. Perceptual learning also transfers to DVA and NVA under uncrowded and crowded conditions and even stereopsis. Learning curves indicated that improvements may be larger after longer training.
Children are categorized as visually impaired (VI) based on their visual acuity and/or visual field restriction. But in childhood, mid- to high-level processes, such as visual search and motion processing, may be impaired too.
1–3 Another debilitating factor is crowding. Crowding refers to a decreased ability to identify objects in clutter.
4,5 Recent studies indicate that perceptual learning can enhance visual performance.
6,7 Perceptual learning is defined as “any relatively permanent and consistent change in the perception of a stimulus array, after practice or experience with this array” (p. 29).
8 It is considered a manifestation of cortical plasticity, and changes may last for months or even years.
6–9 A prerequisite for effective perceptual learning in children is that they cooperate and have a at least a minimal amount of concentration.
10 Here, we evaluated the effect of computerized training on visual performance of children with infantile nystagmus (IN) occurring in association with albinism or occurring solitarily without an afferent visual defect (a condition called idiopathic IN).
Albinism and idiopathic IN are two of the most prevalent diagnoses associated with VI in children: 8% of VI children have albinism and 6.6% have idiopathic IN.
11 Infantile nystagmus is present at birth or develops in the first 3 to 6 months.
12 Idiopathic IN is characterized by a high-frequency horizontal nystagmus, mildly reduced visual acuity (mean acuity 0.3 logMAR), strabismus (7.8–44%), and relatively good stereopsis.
13,14 In adults with albinism, visual acuity varies from 1.3 to 0 logMAR (mean acuity 0.6 logMAR), virtually all have nystagmus,
13 there is high prevalence of strabismus, and stereopsis is often absent.
14–16
Visual acuity of children with IN is often more severely degraded by the presence of distractors than it is in children with normal vision.
17,18 Accumulating evidence indicates that IN is associated with stronger visual crowding and contour interaction.
4,18–20 Factors that contribute to increased crowding and contour interaction in subjects with IN are the presence of amblyopia,
19 fixational instability,
20,21 and nystagmus frequency and amplitude.
18 Crowding is a ubiquitous, well-documented phenomenon in peripheral vision.
5 Crowding can also occur in foveal vision,
22–24 but in the fovea the phenomenon is quite distinct from peripheral crowding. It is mainly attributed to contour interaction (overlap masking
25), inaccurate eye movements,
21 and response competition between flankers and target.
26,27 A recent study on crowding in 3- to 15-year-old children with normal vision demonstrated that vision in children younger than 7 years is dominated by suppressive interactions between contours.
28 Crowding can be measured in two different ways: one is the maximum distance at which a distractor causes degraded perception of the target (the extent), and the second is the amount of loss in visual acuity when the interaction is at its maximum (the intensity).
4 The crowding extent is the minimum distance needed between a threshold-size target and flankers to allow target recognition.
29 The crowding intensity can be captured by the difference between crowded and uncrowded acuity (in logMAR). In this study, we looked at both of these aspects of crowding.
Perceptual learning refers to performance changes brought about through practice or experience that improves a subject's ability to respond to the environment.
30 This form of learning can be explained by changes in low levels of visual processing such as changes in activity in primary visual cortex in correlation with perceptual learning.
31 In addition, there is evidence that high-level cortical processes are involved in perceptual learning.
32 Perceptual learning can be characterized by fast and slow learning.
33 Fast learning typically takes place over the first one to four training sessions (typically a few hundred trials).
33 Slow learning takes place over days.
34 The first phase may reflect the setting up of a task-specific routine for solving a visual problem, while the second phase may indicate long-term structural modification of basic perceptual modules.
35 A well-documented restriction of perceptual learning is that training effects are often very task specific, which can be seen as a manifestation of plasticity in low-level sensory cortical processes.
36 This task specificity may explain why perceptual learning is not yet widely applied in clinical settings.
36
A possible remedy to promote more generalized learning is to combine multiple perceptual learning approaches: the engagement of attention, the use of reinforcement techniques, and the presentation of stimuli in multiple dimensions/orientations.
36 Based on these principles and the known oculomotor deficit in children with IN, we developed a computerized perceptual learning paradigm that integrates two approaches: learning to identify crowded letters, which improves crowded visual acuity and reduces crowding,
6,7,37 and visuomotor training involving the execution of goal-directed saccades under time constraints. We used 10 training sessions with a total of 3.3 hours of perceptual learning. Two previous studies that adopted a short training period (3.4 hours) in adults with amblyopia reported mean acuity improvements of 0.085 logMAR
38 and 0.13 logMAR.
39 There is also evidence that saccade latencies can be shortened by practice.
40 By training children to execute goal-directed saccades, they could learn to disengage attention from the central stimulus and make an accurate and timely saccade to the target. In this way, children might improve their performance on other tasks that rely on eye movements, such as reading. There is indeed evidence that goal-directed saccades are delayed in children with IN even though their visual processing speed remains unaffected.
41
A drawback of the existing literature on perceptual learning efforts to ameliorate crowding is that many studies do not compare the effect of crowded and uncrowded training paradigms,
6 making it unclear what the effect of the crowded intervention is compared to a similar training without crowding. The present study did include an uncrowded training group. The crucial difference between the two training protocols was that the uncrowded training involved identifying an uncrowded optotype in the absence of distractors while the crowded training entailed identifying an optotype under crowded conditions. In addition, we included children with normal vision to assess task performance in a reference group and to assess possible test–retest learning effects.
Our group of children with IN was composed of 36 children, of whom 18 had oculocutaneous albinism and 18 had idiopathic IN. The diagnosis of the children was obtained after a full ophthalmologic investigation at the start of their rehabilitation. We also included 11 children with normal vision. Children with normal vision were not trained because the burden of the training was considered too large for these children given that they had no visual acuity deficits. This control group was included to assess baseline differences in performance on the training task and to evaluate test–retest learning.
Inclusion criteria for all children were age 6 to 11 years, normal birth weight (>3000 g), birth at term (>36 weeks), no perinatal complications, and normal development. For children with IN, additional criteria were no additional impairments and crowded distance visual acuity (DVA) between 0.2 and 1.3 logMAR. Children with normal vision had to have a crowded DVA of 0.1 logMAR or better.
Supplementary Table S1 presents the diagnosis, clinical characteristics, and refraction corrections of participants with IN. Refractive errors of all children with IN were checked no longer than 12 months before the start of the training. Children with prescription glasses always wore them during pre- and postmeasurements and training.
After explanation of the nature and possible consequences of the study, informed consent was obtained from the parents of all participants. The study was approved by the local ethics committee (CMO Arnhem-Nijmegen, The Netherlands) and conducted according to the principles of the Declaration of Helsinki.
Subjects also performed two computer-controlled letter-discrimination tasks on a 32-inch liquid crystal display (LCD) to determine their uncrowded VA and crowding extent under a 500-ms time constraint (
Fig. 1A). Letters were always high-contrast (99.7% Michelson) black Landolt Cs (0.3 cd/m
2) with four possible orientations against a white background (193.8 cd/m
2).
In the single-letter task, a central
C was presented for 500 ms in one of four orientations: up, down, left, and right. Children had to report the orientation of the
C by pressing a corresponding key on the keyboard. Letter size was varied from 0.2 log units below to 0.3 log units above the uncrowded DVA measured during the pretest ophthalmologic assessment (six sizes, 15 trials per size). Feedback was given after each trial by presenting a red or green smiley for 200 ms according to the correctness of the child's response. A Weibull function was fitted to the resulting psychometric response function to find the 62.5% correct performance threshold and the corresponding letter size (in logMAR). This threshold level was chosen because it is halfway between the guess rate (γ, which is 25% for a four-alternative forced-choice task) and the 100% correct rate (see “Data Preprocessing” below).
49
In the crowded letter task, children were instructed to identify a target
C surrounded by six
Cs of the same size in another orientation. This stimulus array was presented either to the left or to the right of the initial fixation. Letter size was kept fixed at 0.15 log units above the subjects' uncrowded VA measured at baseline with the single-letter task. This enlargement was based on pilots in children with normal vision and lies close to the 0.12 log units enlargement used to measure crowding extent in a previous study.
50 In each trial, the first stimulus was a center stimulus patch made up of seven
Cs all in the same orientation (
Fig. 1A, right). The target C had a different orientation than the surrounding
Cs, which were placed at 60° intervals around the target. Trial-by-trial feedback about the correctness of the answer was given by a red or green smiley, which was presented for 200 ms. Target-to-flanker distance (in minutes of visual angle, and measured from center to center) was a multiplication of letter height: ×1.2, ×1.5, ×2.0, ×2.5, ×3, and ×4. The 62.5% correct discrimination thresholds were found by fitting Weibull functions to the resulting psychometric response functions (see “Data Preprocessing” below). The resulting spacing thresholds were expressed in minutes of visual angle and then log transformed to obtain crowding extent scores in logMAR: crowding extent = log
10(spacing threshold).
If the 62.5% correct crowding extent could not be found with this fixed-stimulus procedure, an adaptive staircase procedure was used (Quest).
51 The Quest procedure consisted of two parts: a part in which 40 trials were presented to determine the uncrowded VA, and a part in which 40 trials were run to assess the crowding extent for that letter size. This procedure was used in 18/36 children with IN and 5/11 children with normal vision. In these cases, the letters in the crowded letter task had to be enlarged by 0.37 ± 0.04 logMAR and 0.26 ± 0.02 logMAR, respectively. Thus, children with IN needed more enlargement of the target letter to perform the crowded letter task than children with normal vision.
During the pre- and posttest sessions a chin rest was used to keep children at the appropriate distance from the monitor and an infrared camera, which was used to record their eye movements (for details, see Ref. 51). For children with a DVA > 0.7 logMAR, eye-to-screen distance was 50 cm, and distance between the two groups with Cs was 15° to prevent patches from overlapping. For children with DVA ≤ 0.7 logMAR, eye-to-screen distance was increased to 130 cm and center-to-center distance between the patches was 5° to prevent the second patch from falling outside the screen dimensions and to provide good image quality.
In addition to these letter-discrimination tasks, all children also performed an eye fixation task, a saccade task, and a reading task to assess their oculomotor behavior and reading performance. A detailed description of these tasks and their outcomes is given elsewhere (see Refs. 52, 53).
Statistical analysis was done with SPSS (version 21.0; IBM Corp., Armonk, NY, USA). Baseline performance measures were compared for children with albinism, children with idiopathic IN, and children with normal vision using univariate ANOVAs with Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons. In addition, we checked equality of training groups with an independent samples t-test. Training effects (and test–retest learning) were evaluated with a repeated measures ANOVA with training group entered as the between-subjects variable and pre- and posttest as the within-subjects variable. Diagnosis did not affect training effects and was therefore not included as a factor. In case PREPOST × training-type interactions occurred, post hoc repeated measures ANOVAs were run to evaluate training effects for the two training groups separately. Unless otherwise specified, interaction effects were absent. Results were considered statistically significant if alpha (type I error) was <0.05.
Single-Letter Computer Task.
Crowded Letter Computer Task: Crowding Extent.
Test–Retest Learning.
Binocular Distance Visual Acuity.
Monocular Distance Visual Acuity.
Binocular Near Visual Acuity.
Binocular Distance Crowding Intensity.
Binocular Near Crowding Intensity.
Stereopsis.
The authors thank the parents and children for their participation. In addition, we thank Anne-Lotte Nijhof, Kim Ligtenbarg, Carmen Lageweg, and Debbie Günes-Huurneman for training the children. Finally, we thank Marlijn Anneveldt for running a preliminary analysis of this study.
Supported by the ODAS Foundation and Landelijke Stichting voor Blinden en Slechtzienden (LSBS) which contributed through UitZicht, and Bartiméus Sonneheerdt. The funding organizations had no role in the design or conduct of this research.
Disclosure: B. Huurneman, None; F.N. Boonstra, None; J. Goossens, None