The aim of this study was to develop a better understanding of the functional effects of increased retinal stray light caused by various visual disruptions on visual performance. We found a significant and consistent impact of stray light on all three visual tests for all subjects. The stray light effects induced by glistenings are predominantly distributed around two retinal peaks located at an angle of 2.5° and 15°.
9 This means that the standard C-quant is not the most suitable instrument to detect the effects of glistenings, as it operates with a glare source located at an average angle of 7°. The extended C-quant used in this study, which performs stray light measurements at an average angle of 2.5°, is therefore a better instrument for measuring these effects. Another method used to measure the stray light from glistenings is the Scheimpflug technique,
24,25 which quantifies backward light scatter. However, the perception by the patient depends on the forward (retinal) light scatter. Forward stray light levels can be as much as 300 times larger than that of the measured backward scatter due to the fact that glistenings induce Mie scatter.
9 The double-pass technique has the potential to estimate the forward scatter. However, the currently available double-pass method provides a scatter index that is defined only within the central angular range of 0.3°,
30 and was therefore not suitable for this study. A new development in the double-pass technique shows the potential to capture a field of 8°.
31 For cataract cases the forward scatter between 1° and 30° is uniformly distributed over the retina,
26 and the ratio between forward scatter and backward scatter in cataract has been shown to be on average a factor of approximately 2.3,
32 which is much less than that shown for glistenings due to the smaller size (0.7 μm) of the responsible scatterers in cataract.
33
The results of this study show that increased retinal stray light causes decreased visual performance for all of the tests performed, even for stray light levels shown to be typical of those induced by glistenings in IOLs. The relevance of the chosen visual performance tests is supported by the settings for the level of illuminance at the eye and the level of the stimulus luminance. Increased halo size has been reported to be correlated to forward light scatter.
34 Halo size measurement provides a psychophysical test of an otherwise highly subjective factor.
35 The luminance threshold test is a detection task intended to capture the impact of stray light in low-light environments, where both glare source and stimuli are of low strength. Degraded performance for this type of visual task may represent a risk factor under low-light conditions such as night driving. The luminance threshold test was found to be the most sensitive visual performance test executed, and shows the dramatic effect that stray light may have under these conditions. The measure illustrates, for example, that a 20-fold increase in luminance may be necessary under low-light conditions in order to detect a pedestrian crossing the street in the presence of an oncoming car. In light of this fact, the complaints of some cataract patients who have adequate VA may be better understood.
Finally, the CS test measures visual capability in an environment with higher luminance, where resolution tasks are required. Contrast sensitivity with and without glare are also relevant factors in driving.
36 Contrast sensitivity losses of 14% to 20%, as induced by low amounts of retinal stray light, have been predicted.
9 This demonstrates the clinical relevance of small elevated levels of retinal stray light. The retinal stray light effect on CS was largest with a glare source present at 2.5°, causing a veiling luminance deteriorating the contrast of the image on the retina. The effect for a glare source located at 7° is less due to the limited size of the halo radius. Individual variation in the relationship found between CS and stray light, as was measured in this study, is expected among the subjects, as age and neural factors also contribute to the CSF. Decreased CS has been reported in a number of IOL studies with and without glistening,
17–21 and was of the same order of magnitude as measured in our study with the BPM¼ filter. The quick CSF method employed in this study allows for a more sensitive measurement of the CS than the 40% between consecutive levels used in standard contrast vision tests. This may explain why contrast loss has not always been concluded in IOL studies on glistenings. Contrast sensitivity loss with a higher magnitude was found for some types of cataracts.
10,12–20 In particular, these studies showed that the CS loss was largest for the cataract type posterior subcapsular opacity and less for nuclear and cortical opacities. These higher amounts of contrast losses measured in the past are of the same order of magnitude as measured in our study with the BPM3 filter. Although our study induced stray light extraocularly, measuring visual effects with filters is a fair approximation to measurements with scatterings in the eye.
In conclusion, retinal stray light correlates strongly with the outcomes of some methods used to measure visual function. Levels of retinal stray light as induced by glistenings and cataract have a measurable and significant impact on visual function.