Abstract
Purpose:
To investigate the topographic changes of the dark-adapted multifocal electroretinogram (mfERG) across adulthood in the central retina and compare the topography between macular versus extramacular, nasal versus temporal, and inferior versus superior retinal areas.
Methods:
Sixty-five subjects (18–88 years) received a comprehensive dilated eye examination to ensure the health of their retina and were tested with a dark-adapted mfERG protocol using a 61-hexagon pattern. The lens absorption of each subject was also estimated using a heterochromatic flicker photometry (HFP) paradigm.
Results:
The response amplitude and latency of the dark-adapted mfERG showed a significant change with age, which was best described with a linear model. All the retinal areas examined demonstrated similar aging effects. The extramacular and temporal retina showed higher response amplitude and faster response latency when compared with the macular and nasal retinae, respectively. No difference was found in response amplitude and latency between the inferior and superior retina. The HFP results also showed a significant correlation with age, consistent with senescent increases in short wavelength absorption by the crystalline lens. However, the change in lens absorption did not exceed the magnitude of the change in response amplitude and latency.
Discussion:
Our results indicate that there is a decline in dark-adapted retinal activity as measured with the mfERG. These aging processes affect rods and rod-bipolar cells. Their decrease in response can be attributed to both optical and neural factors.
Vision under dark-adapted, or scotopic, conditions is initiated by the rod photoreceptors that reach their maximal sensitivity after approximately 40 minutes in the dark. Many factors determine dark-adapted vision. One such factor is normal aging.
1 Another is pathology, either retinal (i.e., AMD, diabetic retinopathy)
2,3 or systemic (i.e., vitamin A deficiency, Diabetes Mellitus).
4,5 Because the risk of these retinal and systemic pathologies increases with age, compromised dark-adapted vision might be the result of these two factors. Older subjects show a slowing of the dark-adaptation process and they may require up to 90 minutes to reach their maximum sensitivity. Furthermore, older adults also show a reduction in absolute sensitivity, irrespective of the dark-adaptation time.
1 Either way, the common denominator is that the function and kinetics of the rod system are altered. Due to high spatial and temporal summation of the scotopic system, however, detectable changes might not occur after neuronal loss has already taken place.
6 Several studies have also suggested that the rod system is more vulnerable to aging and disease than the cone system,
7,8 making it an ‘ideal' candidate for early detection of retinal pathologies.
In vitro photoreceptor topography of excised human retina
8 showed a greater loss of rods in the central 4 mm (∼14°) of the retina compared with the periphery. Approximately 34,000 rods are lost per year from early adulthood to the nineties, with the greatest loss occurring between 1 and 2 mm from the fovea. There is, however, reorganization of the remaining rods so that the covered area and rhodopsin density change little throughout adulthood.
8–10 Furthermore, nonuniform rod loss is not reflected by variations in scotopic sensitivity across the retina,
11 suggesting that postreceptoral mechanisms need to be taken into consideration. A suitable test to resolve this discrepancy is the dark-adapted ERG as the origins of its components are post receptoral, namely the b-wave. When the dark-adapted ERG is coupled with the multifocal technique,
12 it offers spatial information that can be used to assess local changes in dark-adapted function. Hood et al.
13 first recorded the dark-adapted multifocal electroretinogram (mfERG) on humans, and Nusinowitz et al.
14 on mice, and later many found useful applications in assessing diseases of the retina.
15–20
None of the aforementioned studies looked into the effect of age on the dark-adapted mfERG, with the exception of Feigl et al.
19 They tested the differences between younger (age range 28–36 years) and older (age range 64–75 years) subject groups. In this study, we sampled across adulthood to obtain objective spatially resolved information about dark-adapted function throughout the central retina (∼40°). These results provide normative data for dark-adapted mfERGs throughout adulthood that could be used to detect abnormalities in aging or pathologic conditions.
Heterochromatic flicker photometry was used to estimate the relative optical density of each subject's ocular media. The blue and red phosphors of a CRT monitor (FlexScan; EIZO, Hakusan, Japan) were flickered in 20-Hz sinusoidal counter-phase. The two lights formed an annular disk of approximately10° to 40° inner/outer diameter using a ViSaGe graphics card and the Visual Psychophysics Engine (Cambridge Research Systems Ltd, Rochester, UK). The central 10° of the stimulus was removed to avoid confounding effects of macular pigment absorption. The luminances of the two disks were yolked so that the sum of the red and blue phosphor luminance was always 30 cd/m2. The task for the subjects was to change the relative luminance of the two disks so as to minimize the perceived flicker. For example, if the subject set the ratio R/(R+B) (where R is the luminance of the red phosphor and B is the luminance of the blue phosphor) at 0.5, then R=B, the point of minimum flicker. A lower ratio means that the ocular media of this subject absorbed more short-wavelength light; greater short-wave radiance was required for equiluminance. Knowing that the lens absorbs mainly at short wavelengths and its optical density increases with age, we would expect that the R/(R+B) would decrease as a function of age.
This study describes rod-mediated function as measured with the dark-adapted mfERG. In general, there is a significant negative correlation between age and amplitude as well as a significant positive correlation between age and response latency. The current study also examined the topography of the dark-adapted mfERG. Statistically significant differences in amplitude and latency were found between nasal and temporal retina and between the macular and extramacular areas. No significant difference was found between inferior and superior retina. No statistically significant difference was found in the amplitude and latency slopes for the three paired locations examined here, meaning that no retinal area was found to be more vulnerable to normal age-related change with the mfERG. In the current study, we also estimated the relative degree of short-wavelength absorbance by the aging ocular media using an HFP paradigm so as to disentangle the neural losses from those due to reduced retinal illuminance in the aging eye. A significant negative correlation between age and the HFP ratio was found as expected, suggesting an increased absorbance of short-wavelength light by the ocular media of the older adults. When comparing the results between the dark-adapted mfERGs and HFP experiments, the slopes in the mfERG response amplitude were greater, in general, than that of the HFP ratio. Therefore, the difference between the slopes of response amplitude and HFP slope can be at least partly attributed to senescent retinal mechanisms and not optical factors alone.
As already mentioned, this study fills a gap in the literature regarding the aging of the mfERG responses throughout adulthood. A previous study by Feigl et al.
19 examined age-related change in the dark-adapted mfERG response between a small cohort of younger (
n = 10, 28–36 years) and older adults (
n = 10, 64–75 years). Feigl et al.
19 did not find a difference in amplitude between the two age groups. Given the variability of the response amplitude in our study, a small number of participants might not be sufficient to reveal any age-related differences. However, the authors
17 did find a significant delay in the older adults' response latency. The variability in response latency is, in general, much lower than that of the amplitude variability (∼0.2 log units compared with ∼0.8 log units for the response amplitude in our study), which could explain the discrepancy in amplitude differences between the two studies. Also, we recruited a broader age range with approximately 10 subjects per decade, which allowed us to examine changes in the dark-adapted mfERG across adulthood.
Our results are not directly comparable with those obtained with full-field dark-adapted ERGs due to differences in rod population and neural sampling in the extreme peripheral retina. Nevertheless, it is worth noting that Birch et al.
25 found an age-related decline in full-field dark-adapted ERG amplitude in a cohort of subjects with ages ranging between 1 week and 79 years, with the vast majority of their subjects being between 5 and 25 years. Our results are consistent with their results, even if they can be only qualitatively compared, as both studies agree that there is a decline in the dark-adapted response amplitude with age.
The decrease in response amplitude and increase in response latency in the dark-adapted mfERG could be attributed to several factors. One factor is the difference in the spectral composition of the retinal image across the different ages. The higher absorption of short-wavelength light by the aging ocular media means that the retinal illuminance is reduced in older adults at the area of the visible spectrum where the rods are most sensitive. Age-adjusted corrections could be made for the retinal illuminance following lens absorption models,
6,26,27 but in this study, we directly estimated the relative density of the ocular media based on HFP. The difference between the amplitude versus age slopes and the HFP ratio versus age slope shows that the lens absorption indeed is a factor contributing to the age-dependent reduction in response. However, increased lens absorption cannot completely explain the loss in the dark-adapted mfERG signal in the older adults.
Additional factors that may have contributed to our results could be age-related rod apoptosis as well as changes in the permeability of the RPE/Bruch's membrane complex. Curcio et al.
8 showed that the number of rods significantly decreased within a 6-mm radius from the center of the retina (∼20°) as a function of age. The peak dropout was found at approximately 2-mm eccentricity (∼7°) while the rate of rod death is approximately 340,000 rods/year in the central 4-mm eccentricity (∼13°). Experiments that estimate rod kinetics (namely the slope of the rod-phase in dark adaptation measurements) also point to changes in the rhodopsin regeneration rate with age.
1,2 Older adults show slower rates of dark adaptation when compared with younger adults. One possible explanation might be that the RPE/Bruch's membrane complex that facilitates nutrient transport from the choriocapillaris to the photoreceptors becomes less efficient over time.
1,7 The transport of nutrients necessary for the retinoid cycle, such as vitamin A through the RPE/Bruch's membrane complex might be obstructed in older adults and alter the rod-response. Of course, we need to acknowledge that dark adaptation responses are much more complex in nature than the responses we describe in this study, as they potentially involve mechanisms beyond the rods and bipolar cells that mediate dark-adapted ERG responses.
In addition to the overall age-related changes, we examined the variation in scotopic mfERG responses between several retinal areas. A significant difference in response amplitude and latency was found between the macular and extramacular areas, with the larger amplitude and faster response latency in the extramacular area. These findings can probably be explained by the rod distribution and rod convergence across the retina. The peak rod density appears at approximately 20° eccentricity, an area that falls in the extramacular group in our configuration, and could explain the elevated P1 amplitude. Similarly, increased rod convergence to bipolar cells in the peripheral retina, might mean faster bipolar cell latency due to the sum of many more rod responses converging onto a single bipolar cell. This would be consistent with psychophysical results of Arden and Weale
28 demonstrating faster dark adaptation rates with larger stimuli.
Nasal-temporal asymmetries in the retina are well documented in various studies that involve primarily cone sensitivity
29,30 but also rod sensitivity,
31 and they all point to the temporal visual field (nasal retina) exhibiting better performance. Between the nasal and temporal retina, rod density is slightly higher in the nasal retina, especially in the periphery, however, the presence of the optic disk creates a drop in the rod density and consequently in rod sensitivity at this area. The average area of the optic disk is roughly 10 mm
2, while the area of the nasal retina that has been stimulated in our experiments is approximately 54 mm
2. Hence, the optic disk occupies 20% of the nasal retina, which could explain the lower response amplitude we found in this study. The density of the rods is similar between the superior and inferior retina, thus reflecting the similar response amplitudes between the two regions found here.
The findings of the present study demonstrate the localized changes in the rod-mediated activity of the central retina that occur during adulthood and provide reference data for studies of retinal and macular diseases. Although normal aging appears to affect the dark-adapted mfERG responses similarly across retinal locations, as we have shown earlier,
20 the mfERG has the power to detect localized changes in retinal function in ocular pathologies that manifest themselves in particular regions of the retina (i.e., AMD). Moreover, the spatial resolution of the scotopic mfERG has an advantage over psychophysical methods for revealing retinal abnormalities that might be masked by downstream visual processes.
The authors thank S.M. Garcia for subject recruitment, coordination, and testing.
Supported by National Institutes of Health Grants AG04058 and EY014743 (Bethesda, MD, USA).
Disclosure: A. Panorgias, None; M. Tillman, None; E.E. Sutter, Electro-Diagnostic Imaging, Inc. (I, E, C, R), P; A. Moshiri, None; C. Gerth-Kahlert, None; J.S. Werner, None