Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science Cover Image for Volume 58, Issue 7
June 2017
Volume 58, Issue 7
Open Access
Eye Movements, Strabismus, Amblyopia and Neuro-ophthalmology  |   June 2017
Binocular Therapy for Childhood Amblyopia Improves Vision Without Breaking Interocular Suppression
Author Affiliations & Notes
  • Manuela Bossi
    UCL Institute of Ophthalmology, University College London, London, United Kingdom
  • Vijay K. Tailor
    National Institute for Health Research Biomedical Research Centre at Moorfields Eye Hospital and UCL Institute of Ophthalmology, London, United Kingdom
  • Elaine J. Anderson
    UCL Institute of Ophthalmology, University College London, London, United Kingdom
    UCL Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, University College London, London, United Kingdom
  • Peter J. Bex
    Department of Psychology, Northeastern University, Boston, Massachusetts, United States
  • John A. Greenwood
    Experimental Psychology, University College London, London, United Kingdom
  • Annegret Dahlmann-Noor
    National Institute for Health Research Biomedical Research Centre at Moorfields Eye Hospital and UCL Institute of Ophthalmology, London, United Kingdom
  • Steven C. Dakin
    UCL Institute of Ophthalmology, University College London, London, United Kingdom
    National Institute for Health Research Biomedical Research Centre at Moorfields Eye Hospital and UCL Institute of Ophthalmology, London, United Kingdom
    School of Optometry and Vision Science, University of Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand
  • Correspondence: Steven C. Dakin, School of Optometry and Vision Science, University of Auckland, 85 Park Road, Grafton, Auckland 1023, New Zealand; [email protected]
Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science June 2017, Vol.58, 3031-3043. doi:https://doi.org/10.1167/iovs.16-20913
  • Views
  • PDF
  • Share
  • Tools
    • Alerts
      ×
      This feature is available to authenticated users only.
      Sign In or Create an Account ×
    • Get Citation

      Manuela Bossi, Vijay K. Tailor, Elaine J. Anderson, Peter J. Bex, John A. Greenwood, Annegret Dahlmann-Noor, Steven C. Dakin; Binocular Therapy for Childhood Amblyopia Improves Vision Without Breaking Interocular Suppression. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 2017;58(7):3031-3043. https://doi.org/10.1167/iovs.16-20913.

      Download citation file:


      © ARVO (1962-2015); The Authors (2016-present)

      ×
  • Supplements
Abstract

Purpose: Amblyopia is a common developmental visual impairment characterized by a substantial difference in acuity between the two eyes. Current monocular treatments, which promote use of the affected eye by occluding or blurring the fellow eye, improve acuity, but are hindered by poor compliance. Recently developed binocular treatments can produce rapid gains in visual function, thought to be as a result of reduced interocular suppression. We set out to develop an effective home-based binocular treatment system for amblyopia that would engage high levels of compliance but that would also allow us to assess the role of suppression in children's response to binocular treatment.

Methods: Balanced binocular viewing therapy (BBV) involves daily viewing of dichoptic movies (with “visibility” matched across the two eyes) and gameplay (to monitor compliance and suppression). Twenty-two children (3–11 years) with anisometropic (n = 7; group 1) and strabismic or combined mechanism amblyopia (group 2; n = 6 and 9, respectively) completed the study. Groups 1 and 2 were treated for a maximum of 8 or 24 weeks, respectively.

Results: The treatment elicited high levels of compliance (on average, 89.4% ± 24.2% of daily dose in 68.23% ± 12.2% of days on treatment) and led to a mean improvement in acuity of 0.27 logMAR (SD 0.22) for the amblyopic eye. Importantly, acuity gains were not correlated with a reduction in suppression.

Conclusions: BBV is a binocular treatment for amblyopia that can be self-administered at home (with remote monitoring), producing rapid and substantial benefits that cannot be solely mediated by a reduction in interocular suppression.

Amblyopia is a developmental disorder of vision with a prevalence of 2% to 5%,1 defined as a monocular (rarely binocular) reduction of the best-corrected visual acuity (henceforth, acuity) in an otherwise healthy eye. Amblyopia is caused by a prolonged period of abnormal retinal stimulation (mainly) due to strabismus (ocular misalignment), anisometropia (refractive imbalance), or both (combined) and leads to functional deficits, including reduced contrast sensitivity,2 poor spatial localization,3 poor stereovision,4 and foveal crowding.5 
Typically, amblyopia is treated only if the interocular acuity difference between the amblyopic eye (AE) and the fellow eye (FE) is at least 0.2 logMAR.6 Current treatment commences with 12 to 24 weeks of wearing prescribed optical correction, which improves AE acuity to normal levels in 27% to 32% of cases.7,8 Otherwise, treatment to promote the use of the AE is administered, which consists of patching the FE (2–12 h/d)9 or blurring the FE with atropine eye drops10 for up to 24 months.11,12 Such occlusion therapies improve acuity in approximately 70% of patients by 0.2 logMAR or more.9 However, their impact on binocular vision is less certain13 and amblyopia recurs within a year in approximately 25% of patients younger than 8 years.14,15 Moreover, compliance is poor: on average, only 44% of the prescribed daily dose is received in 58% of days ascribed for treatment.16 
Central to current treatment is the idea of a critical period for visual development. In humans, acuity and contrast sensitivity are adversely affected by periods of monocular deprivation before the age of 10 years, even though adult-like performance is reached at 6 years.17 However, the notion that amblyopia is not treatable outside of this period has been challenged by studies finding that adults forced to use their AE show substantial improvements in contrast sensitivity,18 crowded acuity,19 and stereopsis.20 
Interocular suppression (henceforth, suppression) is widely considered to be central to the mechanisms underlying amblyopia, although functional definitions vary. When measured with a dichoptic motion-coherence task,21 suppression has been quantified as the contrast offset between the eyes at which binocular integration fails.22,23 Others have measured suppression as the “effective contrast ratio” necessary to perform a dichoptic phase-alignment task.24 Recently, some of us have developed a test measuring the contrast mixture of dichoptically presented letter-pairs that leads observers to switch from using one eye to the other.25 In terms of physiological mechanism, animal models have shown that prolonged monocular deprivation leads to weakened excitatory drive from the deprived eye and hence imbalanced activation of binocular cortical neurons.2629 The resulting suppression is thought to be the result of an active inhibitory cortical mechanism.30 
Stronger suppression is associated with more severe amblyopia23,31 and has traditionally been viewed as an adaptive mechanism (to avoid double-vision).32 In contrast, it has been proposed that suppression has a causative role in amblyopia, making it a candidate target for therapy.31 Notably, Hess and colleagues have developed “antisuppression” therapy, which uses games (with elements split across the eyes to promote binocularity) as a mean of treating amblyopia in adults3335 and children.3638 After 2 to 6 weeks of treatment, visual acuity improves by an average of approximately 0.15 logMAR and stereopsis was measurable in approximately 45% of participants (for the first time in approximately two-thirds of them). However, a randomized controlled trial of an alternative binocular therapy (iBIT) reports only modest success with children (mean acuity gain: 0.08 logMAR).39 Modern “perceptual learning” treatments have also yielded positive results in adults and older children via monocular training (using the AE) on psychophysical tasks18,40 or video-game play,41,42 as have hybrid approaches that interleave a monocular task with dichoptic video-game play.43 For a review on monocular and binocular behavioral training methodologies, see Reference 44. 
Although the changes in acuity and binocularity elicited by such therapies are widely cited as examples of cortical plasticity41,45 effected through a change in suppression,31 in reality the mechanism(s) remains poorly understood. Here, we describe a new variant of binocular therapy: balanced binocular viewing treatment (BBV), which uses dichoptic movies that are matched in visibility across the eyes. Our procedure is designed to be both an effective home-based treatment (engaging a high level of compliance) and a platform for exploring how binocular therapies work. 
Methods
Participants
Twenty-four children (14 female) aged 3.5 to 11.3 years (mean age: 6.6 ± 2.9 years), with anisometropic (group 1), strabismic, or combined mechanism amblyopia (both in group 2), were recruited from the Richard Desmond Children's Eye Centre at Moorfields Eye Hospital, London. The treatment was allowed for a maximum of 8 weeks (group 1; pilot study, see Discussion) or 24 weeks (group 2). Our research followed the tenets of the Declaration of Helsinki and we obtained informed consent from caregivers and assent from children before enrollment. Our recruitment procedure and treatment regimen were approved by the local NHS Research Ethics Committee. 
Children were included if their amblyopia (defined as an interocular acuity difference of 0.2 logMAR or greater and FE acuity equal or better than 0.2 logMAR) persisted after a minimum of 16 weeks of optical treatment, and if acuity in the AE was unchanged on two consecutive visits, 8 weeks apart. Exclusion criteria included prior amblyopia treatment other than optical correction, presence of paralytic or restrictive squint, other preexisting visual deficit (e.g., cataract) or significant neurological or behavioral problems. Amblyopia was defined as anisometropic if there was a difference of at least 1 diopter (D) in spherical equivalent or 1.5 D in astigmatism between the two eyes. Combined mechanism amblyopia was also associated with heterotropia: either ≤10 D (microstrabismus) or with a larger angle of deviation. Children were classified as strabismic amblyopes if a manifest interocular misalignment greater than 10 D was present (convergent or divergent), but no anisometropia. Participants' details are summarized in Table 1
Table 1
 
Baseline Details of Participants (n = 24)
Table 1
 
Baseline Details of Participants (n = 24)
All children received optical treatment before participating in our study. The mean period of optical treatment was 28 ± 12 weeks: 30 ± 16 weeks in children with anisometropia, 25 ± 9 weeks in those with strabismus, and 29 ± 12 weeks in those with combined mechanism amblyopia. 
Equipment
Our treatment uses a computer system capable of presenting three-dimensional (3D) movies, which is installed in the child's home (Fig. 1A). The monitor operates at 1920 × 1080-pixel resolution at 120 Hz (60 Hz per eye). Movies were presented using software written in MATLAB (MathWorks, Ltd., Cambridge, MA, USA) and Psychtoolbox (http://www.psychtoolbox.org, in the public domain).46 Shutter glasses (nVidia Corp., Santa Clara, CA, USA) were used to independently control the image presented to the two eyes. These were mounted in a customized children's ski mask to ensure comfort, while maintaining a snug fit over spectacle correction. The monitor was linearized in software based on a series of luminance measurements (made by placing a Minolta LS110 photometer [Konica Minolta, Tokyo, Japan] behind a single lens of a pair of goggles) to achieve a midgray of 45 cd/m2 to each eye. Children were provided with a keypad to make responses to the suppression task, and were encouraged to use the 95-cm-long cable to ensure they maintained viewing distance at approximately 1 m. 
Figure 1
 
(A) Treatment system: a personal computer, 3D-capable monitor, response-keypad, infrared emitter, and goggle-mounted shutter-glasses. (B) (Top) The child's view through AEs and FEs. The system applied sufficient blur to the FE to match acuity with the AE. (Bottom) Although movies are 2D, a shift in the relative position of each eye's image modulates the perceived depth of the movie. The zero-disparity textured background is also visible, which provides a vergence lock. Movie image © copyright 2008, Blender Foundation, www.bigbuckbunny.org, available under the Creative Commons Attribution License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/3.0/).
Figure 1
 
(A) Treatment system: a personal computer, 3D-capable monitor, response-keypad, infrared emitter, and goggle-mounted shutter-glasses. (B) (Top) The child's view through AEs and FEs. The system applied sufficient blur to the FE to match acuity with the AE. (Bottom) Although movies are 2D, a shift in the relative position of each eye's image modulates the perceived depth of the movie. The zero-disparity textured background is also visible, which provides a vergence lock. Movie image © copyright 2008, Blender Foundation, www.bigbuckbunny.org, available under the Creative Commons Attribution License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/3.0/).
Treatment Regimen
Treatment consisted of 1 hour per day spent viewing movies (selected by children/carers) while wearing the goggles. Movies were presented dichoptically and the horizontal offset between the two eyes was continuously modulated to generate a percept of gradually changing depth. A zero-disparity textured background was presented to both eyes to encourage stable vergence. The child's view of the movie (Fig. 1B, inset) was “balanced” by blurring the image presented to the FE, so that the child's monocular acuity was matched across eyes. To determine the level of blur required, we ran two tasks. Task 1 (Fig. 2A) quantified AE acuity as the scaling required to support identification of the orientation of a crowded Visual-Acuity-Man (“VacMan”).47 Targets were presented monocularly at 75% contrast with four flanking “ghosts” (spaced at twice the target width). They were masked with a 25% contrast phase–scrambled version of the stimulus that was visible across both eyes (to provide a vergence lock). Stimuli were scaled using an adaptive staircase (QUEST).48 Over 45 trials, this converged on the scaling that produced 83% correct identification. Task 2 (Fig. 2B) presented similar VacMan stimuli to the FE (scaled with the AE threshold from task 1) and then used QUEST to determine the level of isotropic Gaussian blur that elicited 83% VacMan identification. This level of blur was applied to the image presented to the FE during movie presentation, ensuring that the images presented to the two eyes were equally visible. 
Figure 2
 
The psychophysical tasks used (A, B) to establish blur level for the FE and (C) to quantify suppression during treatment. (A) Crowded “VacMan” stimulus used to measure acuity in the AE. (B) Stimuli used to estimate the blur level required to match the performance of the FE to the AE. (C) Suppression/compliance task. VacMan is flanked by two ghosts either positioned on the left and the right (as shown) or above and below (dashed outlines). Each ghost was a mixture of one dark and one light ghost presented to different eyes on each side (illustrated within the white circles). We quantify suppression as the mixture of luminance (L)-increments and -decrements required for the child to be equally likely to report either ghost as “whiter.” (D) Sample staircases for two observers. Gray text and horizontal line indicate the estimated balance point.
Figure 2
 
The psychophysical tasks used (A, B) to establish blur level for the FE and (C) to quantify suppression during treatment. (A) Crowded “VacMan” stimulus used to measure acuity in the AE. (B) Stimuli used to estimate the blur level required to match the performance of the FE to the AE. (C) Suppression/compliance task. VacMan is flanked by two ghosts either positioned on the left and the right (as shown) or above and below (dashed outlines). Each ghost was a mixture of one dark and one light ghost presented to different eyes on each side (illustrated within the white circles). We quantify suppression as the mixture of luminance (L)-increments and -decrements required for the child to be equally likely to report either ghost as “whiter.” (D) Sample staircases for two observers. Gray text and horizontal line indicate the estimated balance point.
During treatment, the movie was interrupted every minute by an interactive game used to measure suppression (Fig. 2C). Two dichoptically presented “ghosts” flanked a central VacMan, either above/below or left/right. We told children “VacMan wants to eat the whitest ghost; which ghost looks the whitest?” They responded (up/down/left/right) using a keypad. Each ghost was composed of one dark and one light component, presented dichoptically to each eye. The luminance of the components was set using an interocular contrast ratio (R; 0–100%), which determines the relative strength of FE and AE stimulation as 0% = FE fully suppressed, 50% = balanced vision and 100% = AE fully suppressed. For a background luminance of Lback (here, 45 cd/m2) with a maximum increment/decrement of Lrange (here, 45 cd/m2), we made stimuli with the following algorithm: 
  1.  
    Randomly select if ghosts fall above/below or left/right (Fig. 2C) of VacMan.
  2.  
    Randomly assign the light/dark FE/AE polarity ghost to one side of VacMan, with the opposite polarity on the other side; for example, Fig. 2C shows a dark/light FE/AE ghost on the left and a light/dark FE/AE ghost on the right of VacMan.
  3.  
    Use R to set ghost component-luminance values. The luminance of the light ghost on one side of VacMan and the dark ghost on the other side are matched increments and decrements: Lback ± (R/100)*Lrange. For example, in Figure 2C, R = 75%. Thus, for C = R/100 = 0.75: 45 ± C *45 = 78.8 and 11.3 cd/m2, respectively. Similarly, the light ghost (right) and dark ghost (left) are Lback ± (1-C)*Lrange = 45 ± 0.25*45 = 56.3 or 33.8 cd/m2.
Initially R was set to 80% and then adjusted (by ±10%) on each trial, using a one-up-one-down staircase procedure, according to whether a response was consistent with reliance on the FE or the AE. For R = 75% (Fig. 2C) the AE sees a substantially stronger white ghost; someone with balanced vision would report that they perceive the whiter ghost on the “left” leading to a reduction in R on the next trial. Thus, R converges on the level necessary for the stimulus delivered to either eye to drive the report with equal probability. For an observer neglecting the AE, R > 50% indicating that a stronger signal is required in the amblyopic eye for that ghost to be reported as whiter. 
Note that each child performed one trial per minute with the procedure restarting each time the child switched movies, so the number of trials contributing to any one suppression estimate varied according to the time spent watching a given movie in one session. If the child chose a location in which no ghost was presented, he or she was either not paying attention or not viewing through the goggles (because balanced ghosts were invisible without the 3D shutter glasses). The number of such errors was used to quantify attention/compliance. The system e-mailed the experimenter a daily update of the time children had spent engaged in movie viewing and their performance on this task. 
We treated the first cohort of participants (group 1: n = 8, all anisometropes) for a maximum of 8 weeks. At standard orthoptic assessments that occurred alongside BBV (see next section), we observed gains in acuity and stereoacuity that did not reach a plateau. Therefore, for the second group of children (group 2: all combined or strabismic amblyopia) we extended the maximum period of treatment to 24 weeks. 
Orthoptic Assessment
An experienced orthoptist performed a battery of tests at baseline (pretreatment) and after 4 and 8 weeks of therapy. They assessed best-corrected visual acuity using a crowded logMAR test at 3 m (Thompson v2000 software; Thompson Software solutions, Hertfordshire, UK), stereoacuity using the Frisby near stereotest,49 and ocular motility and ocular alignment at 3 m (distance) and 33 cm (near) using the prism cover test. Finally, we attempted to make a clinical measure of suppression using a Bagolini filter bar (also known as Sbisa bar) (Haag-Streit UK, Harlow, UK). This test uses red filters of increasing density to quantify the reduction in luminance of a target (presented to the fixating eye) required to induce diplopia. This test was difficult to administer (only seven of the children approached were able to perform it). Given the small sample size, and reports of poor test-retest reliability of this test,50 we do not consider these data further. 
Participants eligible for a longer course of treatment (group 2; see Results) were also assessed at 16 and 24 weeks. Treatment was discontinued after 4 weeks if the acuity fell below baseline, or if the interocular acuity difference (IOAD) had improved to normal levels (0.2 logMAR or less). At subsequent visits, children were considered to have reached a plateau if acuity failed to improve by 0.1 logMAR from their preceding visit. Children who were advised to discontinue home therapy were referred back to the hospital eye clinic to receive standard occlusion therapy, if IOAD was still 0.2 logMAR or greater or AE acuity did not recover to within 0.1 logMAR. 
Outcome Measures
We used crowded logMAR acuity as our primary outcome measure. We expressed changes in visual function as follows: 
  •  
    AE logMAR acuity.
  •  
    Residual IOAD after treatment.
  •  
    Proportion of deficit corrected, defined as (AEbaseline − AEexit)/(AEbaseline − FEexit).51
In addition, we explored stereopsis (Frisby test) and suppression (ghost task, described above). We also quantified compliance as the mean time spent watching movies per day (“daily dose”) and the mean cumulative time spent watching movies (“total dose”) and adherence as the percentage of the days that treatment was received. Other factors that may have contributed to the outcome measures (treatment duration, type of amblyopia, initial severity of amblyopia, and age) also were evaluated51 and are described in the Supplementary Materials
Results
Two children did not attend the 4-week appointment and were excluded from analyses, leaving 22 children. Group 1 thus consisted of seven children with anisometropic amblyopia (mean age 9.5 years; 4 females). A total of 15 children were included in group 2 (mean age 5.2 years), 6 with strabismic amblyopia (mean age 5.75 years; 2 females) and 9 with combined mechanism amblyopia (mean age 4.7 years; 6 females). Where necessary, results are reported separately for children included in group 1 (allowed maximum 8 weeks) or 2 (allowed up to 24 weeks on treatment). 
Acuity
Figures 3A and 3B plot the difference in logMAR acuity from baseline (BL), as measured for each child during his or her clinical assessments. Specifically, data for children with pure anisometropic amblyopia (n = 7; square symbols) or strabismic amblyopia (n = 6; triangle symbols) are reported in Figure 3A, whereas those with combined mechanism amblyopia are in Figure 3B (n = 9; circle symbols). Individual declines or improvements in vision are represented by values falling above or below the dashed horizontal line, respectively (no change from BL = 0 logMAR acuity difference). The individual values measured before starting and after completing BBV treatment (entry versus exit logMAR acuity) are plotted in Figure 3C. As per the study protocol, children were treated for up to either 8 weeks (group 1; n = 7: IDs 1–8 in Table 1) or 24 weeks (group 2; n = 15: IDs 9–24). Among children in group 2, 5 did not improve further after 8 weeks (IDs: 9, 10, 17, 19, and 24; making 12 children in total released at this time point), whereas 4 children remained in treatment for 16 weeks (IDs: 11, 16, 18, and 23) and 6 for 24 weeks (IDs: 12–15, 20, 22), depending on the measured improvement in acuity. Note that acuity continued to improve beyond 8 weeks for some children, suggesting that those whose treatment was terminated at this point (because this was the limit of our approved protocol for group 1) would have received further benefit from continued treatment. 
Figure 3
 
(A, B) Acuity difference in the AE compared with baseline (BL) during treatment; changes below or above the dashed line, respectively, represent improvements or deteriorations in vision. Participants' amblyopia type was pure anisometropic (n = 7; [A]: squares), pure strabismic (n = 6; [A]: triangles), or combined (n = 9; [B]: circles). Thick lines show the mean change in acuity difference for each type of amblyopia (aniso. and strab. in [A]; combined amb. in [B]). Symbol-color codes the age of participants (age indicated in parentheses, in legends of parts [A, B]; from blue-younger to red-older children). Identity codes (1–24) are given next to individual lines and in the legends of (A) and (B) and label individual data point in (C). Note that group 1 (A) is anisometropic (i.e., treated for a maximum of 8 weeks), and group 2 (A, B) is strabismic and combined (treated for a maximum of 24 weeks; line length gives length of treatment). (C) Comparison of pre- and posttreatment acuity for each child. Points below the diagonal line are improvement, with the shaded region indicating gains less than 0.15 logMAR (considered critical of test-retest reliability).44 The dashed vertical line (at 0.6 logMAR) represents the cutoff between mild-to-moderate and severe amblyopia.
Figure 3
 
(A, B) Acuity difference in the AE compared with baseline (BL) during treatment; changes below or above the dashed line, respectively, represent improvements or deteriorations in vision. Participants' amblyopia type was pure anisometropic (n = 7; [A]: squares), pure strabismic (n = 6; [A]: triangles), or combined (n = 9; [B]: circles). Thick lines show the mean change in acuity difference for each type of amblyopia (aniso. and strab. in [A]; combined amb. in [B]). Symbol-color codes the age of participants (age indicated in parentheses, in legends of parts [A, B]; from blue-younger to red-older children). Identity codes (1–24) are given next to individual lines and in the legends of (A) and (B) and label individual data point in (C). Note that group 1 (A) is anisometropic (i.e., treated for a maximum of 8 weeks), and group 2 (A, B) is strabismic and combined (treated for a maximum of 24 weeks; line length gives length of treatment). (C) Comparison of pre- and posttreatment acuity for each child. Points below the diagonal line are improvement, with the shaded region indicating gains less than 0.15 logMAR (considered critical of test-retest reliability).44 The dashed vertical line (at 0.6 logMAR) represents the cutoff between mild-to-moderate and severe amblyopia.
When data from all the children were combined (regardless of amblyopia type, n = 22), mean acuity in the AE improved from 0.78 ± 0.35 to 0.51 ± 0.34 logMAR, a significant mean gain of 0.27 ± 0.22 logMAR (1-sample paired t-test, t5.83, P < 0.001). Vision in the FE remained stable, improving slightly: the mean gain (−0.05 ± 0.11 logMAR, mean baseline: 0.02 ± 0.13 logMAR) was statistically (P = 0.04) but not clinically (gain <0.2 logMAR) significant. Mean acuity gain in severe amblyopia (bold in Table 1 for the n = 14 with acuity worse than 0.6 logMAR) was 0.32 ±0.24 logMAR, versus 0.18 ±0.14 logMAR in mild-to-moderate amblyopia (n = 8). Acuity gains for group 1 (whose treatment was curtailed at 8 weeks, all anisometropic amblyopes) was 0.26 ± 0.28, and for group 2 (maximum treatment of 24 weeks, combined and strabismic amblyopes) was 0.27 ± 0.19 logMAR. There was no significant difference in acuity gains between group 1 and 2 (2-sample t-test(df:20), P = 0.863). 
After treatment, 15 children reached IOAD ≤ 0.6 logMAR (6 of whom started with severe amblyopia), including 7 children (1 severe) who recovered to ≤0.3 logMAR. No further treatment was required for ID15, whose IOAD improved from 0.34 to 0.1 logMAR and ID1, whose AE acuity reached 0.04 logMAR (although final IOAD was 0.24 logMAR). The mean “proportion of deficit corrected” was 32% ± 26%, with substantial gains (>60%) in two children (IDs 15 and 22, 71% and 69%), and poor (<10%) in three children (IDs 8, 19 and 24). In 7 children, improvement was between 10% and 30%, and in the remaining 10 children, between 30% and 60%. 
Maintenance of Acuity Gains After End of Treatment
At the time of writing, clinical follow-up data were available for 11 children, 7 of whom received standard treatment following BBV. Their mean acuity gain was 0.39 ± 0.25 logMAR at completion of BBV treatment and 0.34 ± 0.30 logMAR after an additional mean follow-up time of 47 ± 10 weeks logMAR. Seven children attended a follow-up at 2 years (mean 95 ± 30 weeks after stopping BBV), with +0.01 ± 0.23 logMAR mean change in acuity logMAR from BBV completion (four of seven gained 0.15 ± 0.1 logMAR; three of seven regressed 0.23 ± 0.16 logMAR). 
Stereoacuity
Only children with purely anisometropic amblyopia (group 1) had measurable stereoacuity at baseline, with a median of 170 arcsec (interquartile 230 arcsec; Fig. 4). Following treatment, six of seven children had significantly improved stereoacuity. Overall (n = 7), the median stereoacuity value at exit was 85 arcsec (interquartile 30 arcsec) and mean improvement was 165 ± 182 arcsec, significant with a Wilcoxon signed rank test (paired, z = 2.298, P = 0.0215 at α = 0.05). The one participant whose stereoacuity did not improve (ID3) had good stereoacuity at entry. We transformed data to logarithmic seconds of arc to calculate “real change” in stereoacuity and to allow for comparisons between consecutive visits. Prior studies have found the test-retest reliability of stereoacuity measurements using the near Frisby test in children to be 0.3 log arcsec, with “real change” defined as a doubling of stereoacuity expressed in octaves.52 Here, mean stereoacuity gain was 0.40 log arcsec (±0.32), with all but ID3 exhibiting an improvement in stereoacuity ≥1 octave (Fig. 4B). Mean improvement was 1.33 octaves (i.e., a factor of 2.6 improvement). Of the children having unmeasurable baseline stereopsis, three showed progression after BBV treatment, reaching 600 (ID9), 85 (ID11), and 110 (ID14) arcsec at 1-year follow-up. For the remaining children, the Frisby measure was inconclusive. The gain in stereoacuity significantly correlated with both the initial level of acuity in the AE (r = 0.97, P = 0.0003) and its absolute improvement (r = 0.85, P = 0.02), but did not correlate with the proportional gain in acuity (r = 0.44, P = 0.32). 
Figure 4
 
Stereoacuity (when measurable) for children who completed treatment. (A) Before, during, and after treatment (at 0, 8, and 16 weeks respectively), (B) Pre- versus posttreatment. All children for whom data are shown (n = 7) had purely anisometropic amblyopia (group 1). Symbols are colored to reflect the relative age of each child compared with their peers (red = oldest). Boxed legend in (A) shows individual gain in stereoacuity (arcsec). For six participants, stereoacuity gains exceeded the test-retest variability threshold of 0.3 log arcsec and a step of one octave (shaded area).
Figure 4
 
Stereoacuity (when measurable) for children who completed treatment. (A) Before, during, and after treatment (at 0, 8, and 16 weeks respectively), (B) Pre- versus posttreatment. All children for whom data are shown (n = 7) had purely anisometropic amblyopia (group 1). Symbols are colored to reflect the relative age of each child compared with their peers (red = oldest). Boxed legend in (A) shows individual gain in stereoacuity (arcsec). For six participants, stereoacuity gains exceeded the test-retest variability threshold of 0.3 log arcsec and a step of one octave (shaded area).
Interocular Suppression
Figure 5 shows individual suppression data from the ghost task. Note that ID4 initially did not comply with this task and their (incomplete) data were excluded from the analyses of suppression. The mean suppression at entry was 72.3% (SD 12.02%) and at exit was 72.6% (SD 12.3%). Overall, these values are in line with comparable estimates for adult amblyopes (e.g., 75%)25 and not significantly different from one another (t-test(df:20): P = 0.98). We do not observe the substantial reductions in suppression observed in other studies of binocular therapy.53 Indeed, a statistically significant reduction in interocular suppression was observed in only 6 of the 22 children, of whom 4 had combined mechanism and 2 purely strabismic amblyopia. Further, 5 children showed a significant increase in suppression (ID4 excluded), while 10 children showed no significant change. For each individual, we calculated the linear regression trend line (bold lines in Fig. 5) for daily estimates of R (quantifying the patient's binocularity: 0% fully reliant on AE, 100% fully reliant on the FE). 
Figure 5
 
Day-by-day estimates of suppression (R: % reliance on FE, see Treatment Regimen) for participants with anisometropia (upper row; group 1) and/or with strabismus (group 2). Participants' ID numbers are indicated at the bottom left of each subplot. Here, 50% means “balanced vision,” 100% indicates complete reliance on the FE (i.e., complete suppression of the AE), and 0% indicates complete suppression of the FE. Green symbols pool data within three periods (beginning, middle, end; binned around the individual duration of BBV; note ID4 was not compliant for a period, hence the middle bin is missing). We derived black trend lines from linear regression analysis of daily estimates. An asterisk after the child's ID indicates a significant reduction in suppression for that child (P < 0.05).
Figure 5
 
Day-by-day estimates of suppression (R: % reliance on FE, see Treatment Regimen) for participants with anisometropia (upper row; group 1) and/or with strabismus (group 2). Participants' ID numbers are indicated at the bottom left of each subplot. Here, 50% means “balanced vision,” 100% indicates complete reliance on the FE (i.e., complete suppression of the AE), and 0% indicates complete suppression of the FE. Green symbols pool data within three periods (beginning, middle, end; binned around the individual duration of BBV; note ID4 was not compliant for a period, hence the middle bin is missing). We derived black trend lines from linear regression analysis of daily estimates. An asterisk after the child's ID indicates a significant reduction in suppression for that child (P < 0.05).
We performed additional analyses to examine changes within and across sessions. First, for within-session changes, we analyzed runs containing at least 30 trials (“long sessions”; an average of 36.5% of all runs across 21 children) and divided these runs into three parts. We then compared the average stimulus balance in the second and third part (excluding the first part where the staircase may not be close to convergence), computing a linear regression between values to determine if the slope was significantly different from 0. According to this analysis, for each child (excluding ID4) an average of 9% of “long sessions” involved a significant change in suppression within session. However, such changes were not biased toward increasing suppression (49.8% ± 27.8% of cases) or decreasing suppression (50.3% ± 27.8%). Second, across sessions, we note that Kehrein et al.54 reported an increase in suppression during the first 30 days of occlusion therapy, followed by a return to baseline in the following month. We performed a similar analysis comparing suppression over the first and second 30 days of treatment using regression analysis. Mean slopes over the first and second 30 days were 0.0688 (SD 0.4220) and 0.0018 (SD 0.6245), respectively, a nonsignificant difference (t(20)=0.41, P = 0.68). Thus, occlusion may exert greater (but short-lived) influence on interocular suppression than binocular therapies. 
Figure 6A shows suppression at entry versus exit from treatment (ID4 did not have a complete set of data and was excluded). There was no systematic trend in the change of suppression with treatment: suppression decreased in some children (points below the unity line) but increased in others (points above unity). Figure 6B plots improvement in acuity for the AE versus the difference in suppression, obtained by averaging each child's daily suppression measures. There was a nonsignificant tendency for more improvement in acuity to be associated with modified suppression (Pearson's r = 0.19, P = 0.40; ID4 excluded), especially when suppression significantly changed, either increasing (r = −0.47; P = 0.80) or decreasing (r = −0.13; P = 0.35). For observers with stable suppression (n = 10), we observed a mean gain in acuity of 0.16 ± 0.15 logMAR, whereas for observers whose suppression decreased (n = 6) the change in acuity was 0.40 ± 0.15 logMAR and for those whose suppression increased (n = 5; ID4 excluded) the change was 0.22 ± 0.18 logMAR. A post hoc power calculation for Pearson's r supports our analyses being appropriate to detect higher correlations (for n = 22 − 1(df), r = 0.58 significant, with 80% power). In Figure 6B, we highlight the range of uncertainty for each child by adding error bars (denoting 95% confidence intervals; horizontal bars for acuity gain, verticals for change in suppression). To do this, we first estimated confidence on acuity gain from the typical test-retest variability of logMAR acuity results in children (±0.15 logMAR), and on change in suppression by resampling our indices. We then bootstrapped on pairs of derived values, obtaining at each repetition two sets of changes to find the relative correlation (one set from acuity-paired values and one from suppression-paired values). The mean r across repetitions was 0.161 (mean SD 0.132) confirming the lack of a strong correlation. 
Figure 6
 
Estimates of suppression for 21 children (ID4 excluded; IDs numbers to label the correspondent data points; markers colored from blue-younger to red-older children across all recruited children) (A) Suppression (R) is similar at entry compared with exit from treatment. (B) There is only a modest correlation of gain in visual acuity (VA) with change in suppression (r = 0.193), which does not reach statistical significance (P = 0.402). The horizontal dashed line represents the level of “balanced vision.” VA gains outside of the shaded region are considered clinically significant (≥ 0.2 logMAR). Error bars indicate 95% confidence intervals on acuity (horizontal) and suppression (vertical) estimates.
Figure 6
 
Estimates of suppression for 21 children (ID4 excluded; IDs numbers to label the correspondent data points; markers colored from blue-younger to red-older children across all recruited children) (A) Suppression (R) is similar at entry compared with exit from treatment. (B) There is only a modest correlation of gain in visual acuity (VA) with change in suppression (r = 0.193), which does not reach statistical significance (P = 0.402). The horizontal dashed line represents the level of “balanced vision.” VA gains outside of the shaded region are considered clinically significant (≥ 0.2 logMAR). Error bars indicate 95% confidence intervals on acuity (horizontal) and suppression (vertical) estimates.
Compliance
On average, adherence (calculated as the percentage of days when treatment was received) was 68.0% ± 12.2%, meaning that children watched a movie on more than two-thirds of the days on which the equipment was available to them. The mean total dose (across the whole treatment duration) was 75 hours 14 minutes, with a mean daily dose of 54 ± 14.5 minutes (range, 25–89 minutes). Figure 7A shows that none of the children used the system for less than 20 minutes per day (30% of the prescribed dose). Good compliance (20–50 minutes) was demonstrated by 7 children, and excellent compliance (>50 minutes a day) by 15 children, with mean adherence of 63.4% and 70.5%, respectively. Five children exceeded the prescribed dose. A previous study on a monocular video-game therapy for amblyopia showed a marginally significant correlation between gain in acuity and longer daily sessions in children.42 We find that a greater final gain in acuity was not significantly associated with greater daily dose (r = 0.234, P = 0.296; Fig. 7A) or with a higher percentage of days on treatment (r = 0.0001, P = 0.9998). One might expect dedication to therapy to improve with age, but we did not find significant correlations of age either with the daily dose (r = −0.05, P = 0.8) or with the number of treatment days (r = −0.38, P = 0.08). 
Figure 7
 
Compliance and attention. (A) Daily dose in minutes is plotted against acuity (VA) gains in logMAR for the AE. The trend line shows how a higher daily dose (individual mean number of minutes per day spent watching movies) was associated with greater improvement in VA. (B) Percentage of lapse trials on the whitest-ghost task (“invalid responses,” i.e., where the child either indicated a location where a ghost was not present or did not respond at all) as a function of age. Younger children are more prone to lapsing, possibly indicating poorer attention to the task.
Figure 7
 
Compliance and attention. (A) Daily dose in minutes is plotted against acuity (VA) gains in logMAR for the AE. The trend line shows how a higher daily dose (individual mean number of minutes per day spent watching movies) was associated with greater improvement in VA. (B) Percentage of lapse trials on the whitest-ghost task (“invalid responses,” i.e., where the child either indicated a location where a ghost was not present or did not respond at all) as a function of age. Younger children are more prone to lapsing, possibly indicating poorer attention to the task.
As a measure of attention paid to the task, we classified responses on the ghost task either as “valid” (the child indicated a ghost in a location where there was one) or as “lapses” (the child indicated a position were no ghost was present). On average, 23.1% of responses over all runs were “lapses” (SD 20.7%). Figure 7B shows a significant negative correlation between the proportion of “lapses” and the age of the child (r = −0.54, P = 0.01). Although we note a high number of lapses, particularly in some younger children, this does not seem to have greatly affected our estimates of suppression (which remain stable across many days; Fig. 5). In particular, although some children with noisier suppression data did make more lapses (e.g., IDs 15, 22), others did not (e.g., IDs 10, 11). This is because our suppression estimate was tolerant of lapses because lapsing generated “no-response” (not a random response) so that the same stimulus level was presented until a valid response was made, and run lengths were long enough (average 30 trials, SD 14.8 trials) that staircases converged even with frequent lapsing. We note that children generally tolerated the interruption of the “ghost” task surprisingly well, possibly because they are accustomed to media content being regularly interrupted by commercials. 
Discussion
We describe a novel treatment for amblyopia, BBV therapy, which matches stimulation across the eyes and consists of 1 hour per day viewing “binocularly balanced” movies at home through shutter glasses. The procedure involves the blurring of the image received by the FE, at a level such that monocular FE acuity for the blurred stimulus was equal to monocular acuity for the AE (for an unfiltered stimulus as measured at treatment induction). Twenty-two children (3–11 years), with anisometropic, strabismic, or combined amblyopia, completed our study, spending on average 75 hours 14 minutes on treatment, which led to a mean gain in acuity in the AE of 0.27 ± 0.22 logMAR. This gain is clinically significant (i.e., >0.2 logMAR),55 and matches or exceeds those reported using alternative binocular treatments (Table 2). In terms of rate of improvement, although patching requires 120 hours of treatment for every one line of logMAR acuity gained,56 our approach is more than four times faster, yielding a similar benefit in only 28 hours of movie viewing. 
Table 2
 
Current and Proposed Treatments for Amblyopia
Table 2
 
Current and Proposed Treatments for Amblyopia
In terms of stereoacuity, we obtained reliable measures (pre- and posttreatment) only in children with pure anisometropic amblyopia, who frequently maintain a degree of binocularity, especially at low spatial frequencies,57 which remain visible to both eyes. For these children, stereoacuity reached normal values49 in all but one child, exceeding reports for occlusion13 and other binocular therapies (see Table 2). 
The treatment duration varied across children, depending on the responsiveness of each child, as evaluated during orthoptic assessment in clinic, and between groups of amblyopia type, to address concerns about inducing diplopia. A pilot group of children (group 1) was treated for 8 weeks only (according to the standard interval adopted in clinic to evaluate a visual treatment). Although acuity improved in six of seven of these children (mean gain: 0.3 ± 0.26 logMAR; ID8 stable), we do not report a significant change in suppression. In the absence of any adverse events, we went on to apply BBV to children in group 2, with strabismic (n = 6) and combined mechanism amblyopia (n = 9), looking at longer-term effects after up to 24 weeks on treatment. 
This difference in treatment duration limits the validity of comparing responsivity across children, especially because gains had not plateaued in at least some children who were released from BBV. However, we can still consider the effect of treatment length and compliance on the therapeutic outcome. Standard occlusion therapies, which are likely to improve acuity in 50% to 85% of children,58 are fundamentally limited by levels of concordance (i.e., agreement on treatment regimen between patient and clinician) falling below 50%,56 and compliance, especially at a young age (<50% in 4- to 7-year-old children16). Recent studies of various binocular treatments have shown that children whose compliance is less than 50% either showed significantly lower gain in logMAR acuity38 or required longer treatment durations to reach comparable outcomes.59 In older children, the success of monocular training (in addition to patching) was related to the total amount of training (across sessions), provided a minimum daily compliance level of 15 minutes of practice per session was met.42 In our study, on average, children who spent 8 or 16 weeks on BBV showed high levels of compliance (87% ± 21% and 84% ± 28%), with similar gains in AE acuity (0.2 ± 0.24 and 0.24 ± 0.11 logMAR, respectively). Those eligible for 24 weeks received 98% ± 46% of the prescribed dose and gained 0.41 ± 0.16 logMAR (see also Supplementary Materials). Interestingly, those children whose BBV treatment lasted longer spent a higher proportion of days on treatment (66%, 70%, and 71%, respectively over 8, 16, and 24 weeks). These results suggest that high levels of compliance, as both daily and total perseverance, can produce positive treatment outcomes (although such correlations did not reach significance in our study, perhaps due to the generally high levels of compliance). More generally, alternative treatments (to replace or augment occlusion) engage higher levels of compliance than occlusion alone.60 Leaving open the question of whether occlusion is necessary, these studies highlight the importance of compliance in amblyopia treatment. Given the lack of clarity regarding the mechanism supporting improvements in vision, further study is required to determine the extent to which compliance contributes to the superior (more rapid) therapeutic response from binocular therapies. 
Although it is widely assumed that the likelihood of positive treatment outcomes decreases with age,61 recent interventions have highlighted the possibility of improving vision in amblyopia at almost any age.62 As highlighted in the introduction, “perceptual learning” (PL) approaches (involving monocular training on gameplay41,42 or psychophysical tasks18,40) have proven effective in this regard. Gains obtained on the trained task generally transfer to acuity (showing approximately 1–2 logMAR lines of improvement, after approximately 30–50 hours training depending on the severity of amblyopia).40 Dichoptic PL also has been found to improve stereoacuity.43 Similar improvements have been found with binocular interventions, such as “antisuppression” (AST)63 or interactive-binocular (I-Bit)39 therapies that seek to reduce a notional suppressive drive from the FE by equating the visibility of stimuli across the eyes. However, related studies, including games therapy in children37 and adults59,64 did not exclude participants who had previously undergone occlusion therapy, making it impossible to disentangle the influence of previous treatments on outcome. Further, only a few previous studies checked for stable vision before treatment induction.37,38 This leaves open the possibility that at least some of the therapeutic benefits of the treatment originate from ongoing benefits of optical treatment. Note that we included only children with no history of occlusion therapy and whose acuity stabilized after minimum 16 weeks of optical treatment. 
In terms of stereoacuity, only children with measurable (generally poor) stereoacuity at entry showed significant improvements following our BBV treatment. This is consistent with earlier work (with occlusion51 or alternative treatment44), which indicated that the initial level of vision may limit treatment outcomes. Among PL approaches, there is evidence for a small advantage of dichoptic game play over monocular movie viewing in improving stereoacuity,43 although monocular game play can also be effective.40 AST treatment has been shown to significantly improve stereoacuity in adults3335 but not always in children.37,38 Further research (e.g., within randomized controlled trials) is needed to determine which components of these therapies are critical for triggering improvements in specific visual functions (such as stereoacuity) and to establish the wider applicability of these treatments.65 
To equalize acuity across the two eyes, we individualized the level of Gaussian blur applied to the image viewed by the FE during movie viewing (i.e., the high spatial fequencies were attenuated in proportion to the AE acuity deficit). This is in contrast to the fixed blur levels used in treatments that rely on, for example, Bargerter translucent filters.66 In contrast, ASTs manipulate the contrast of the signal to balance visibility across the eyes, and update this level as the child's vision changes during therapy. The fact that we observe substantial gains in acuity indicates that fixed levels of blur penalization and contrast penalization are both effective for treatment. 
Whether and how these methods are related to suppression in amblyopia is hotly debated. Some hold that suppression is a cause of amblyopia31,67 and that ASTs strengthen binocular combination by breaking this suppression, allowing monocular and binocular vision to improve. If this were the case, we would expect better outcomes (i.e., improved acuity) to be associated with greater reductions in suppression. We, like an earlier study,53 do not find strong evidence for such an association, although we note that our approach did not produce substantial changes in suppression at all, unlike at least some binocular gaming therapies53 and occlusion therapies.54 That our therapy is effective in the absence of changes in suppression is, however, consistent with the idea that suppression cannot be the sole cause of amblyopic visual loss. An alternative view is that suppression is a consequence of amblyopia to avoid diplopia.32 If so, a temporary disruption of binocularity (e.g., using repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation) should have no impact on monocular function. That it does68 supports the presence of reduced (not lost) functionality of the AE, possibly as a result of suppression. 
As mentioned above, there is no consensus as to the best way to quantify interocular suppression. The method we use to quantify suppression is related to one we have developed previously25 and uses dichoptic “dark and light” symbol-pairs (“ghosts”). We quantify suppression as the contrast ratio that leads the viewer to be equally likely to report that the symbol presented to either eye is “the whitest.” Although we cannot rule out that the task used to probe suppression will influence the pattern of suppression reported,69 we think it is unlikely that performance on these different dichoptic tasks is mediated by fundamentally different mechanisms. A further study comparing the currently available methods to measure suppression (clinical and psychophysical) is ongoing. In the present study, we found that the gain in vision achieved under BBV did relate to reduced suppression; however, the correlation was low and failed to reach significance. The absence of a significant relationship between suppression and acuity outcomes greatly limits the risk of inducing intractable diplopia as an adverse side effect from a binocular therapy. 
If gains in acuity do not originate from a reduction in suppression, what does produce them? Current theories of the neural basis of amblyopia focus on the consequence of abnormal input from the AE for neural encoding within the lateral geniculate nucleus and primary visual cortex (V1). The following are candidate models: a reduction in the number and/or sensitivity of neurons driven by the AE (undersampling),7072 positional disorganization of visual receptive fields and associated distortions of retinotopic mapping (disarray),7375 and increased variability in the response of binocular cortical neurons (elevated noise).76 Animal models of amblyopia have produced results to support each of these mechanisms,30 although the magnitude of these deficits in V1 rarely matches the scale of the behavioral deficits, suggesting an additional role for brain areas beyond V1.27 In human vision, Clavagnier et al.73 recently reported findings from functional magnetic resonance imaging population receptive field (pRF) mapping (in vivo estimation of visual receptive field size and density)77,78 from the FE and AE of patients with amblyopia. They observed larger pRFs in the fovea of amblyopia patients across areas V1 to V3, but normal cortical magnification across these areas, which could arise either through undersampling, positional disarray, or both. This fits with the more general finding of increased receptive field sizes in binocular V1 neurons following retinal lesions.79 Given the dependency of the size of RFs (and by extension pRFs) on visual experience in these studies, we consider the reduction in RF size to be the most reasonable current candidate for the mechanism producing therapeutic response. This in turn could be driven by reductions in undersampling and/or disarray within a range of cortical regions, as above. Importantly, the dissociation between suppression and acuity gains in our study suggests that although suppression may play a causal role in these amblyopic deficits,31 the mechanism underlying such deficits can be altered by treatment without concomitant changes in binocularity that modify suppression. 
Conclusions
Our BBV treatment engages high levels of compliance and leads to substantial gains in visual function after a relatively short period of treatment. BBV is currently the only unsupervised binocular vision treatment that also supports remote monitoring of compliance and suppression. Our findings thus far indicate that a reduction in interocular suppression is not the basis of the observed improvements in visual acuity. 
Acknowledgments
Supported by a UCL Impact award and by the Special Trustees of Moorfields Eye Hospital (award code 160145). 
Disclosure: M. Bossi, None; V.K. Tailor, None; E.J. Anderson, None; P.J. Bex, None; J.A. Greenwood, None; A. Dahlmann-Noor, None; S.C. Dakin, None 
References
Attebo K, Mitchell P, Cumming R, Smith W, Jolly N, Sparkes R. Prevalence and causes of amblyopia in an adult population. Ophthalmology. 1998; 105: 154–159.
Levi DM, Harwerth RS. Spatio-temporal interactions in anisometropic and strabismic amblyopia. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1977; 16: 90–95.
Levi DM, Klein SA. Spatial localization in normal and amblyopic vision. Vision Res. 1983; 23: 1005–1017.
McKee SP, Levi DM, Movshon JA. The pattern of visual deficit in amblyopia. J Vis. 2003; 3 (5): 380–405.
Levi DM, Klein SA. Vernier acuity, crowding and amblyopia. Vision Res. 1985; 25: 979–991.
Powell C, Hatt SR. Vision screening for amblyopia in childhood. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2009; 3: CD005020.
Cotter SA, Edwards AR, Wallace DK,et al.; Pediatric Eye Disease Investigator Group. Treatment of anisometropic amblyopia in children with refractive correction, NIH-PA, Editor. Ophthalmology. 2006; 113: 895–903.
Cotter SA, Foster NC, Holmes JM,et al.; Pediatric Eye Disease Investigator Group. Optical treatment of strabismic and combined strabismic-anisometropic amblyopia. Ophthalmology. 2012; 119: 150–158.
Stewart CE, Moseley MJ, Fielder AR. Amblyopia therapy: an update. Strabismus. 2011; 19: 91–98.
Li T, Shotton K. Conventional occlusion versus pharmacologic penalization for amblyopia. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2009; 4: CD006460.
Repka MX, Wallace DK, Beck RW,et al.; Pediatric Eye Disease Investigator Group. Two-year follow-up of a 6-month randomized trial of atropine vs patching for treatment of moderate amblyopia in children. Arch Ophthalmol. 2005; 123: 149–157.
Tailor V, Bossi M, Greenwood JA, Dahlmann-Noor A. Childhood amblyopia: current management and new trends. Br Med Bull. 2016; 119: 75–86.
Wallace DK, Lazar EL, Melia M,et al.; Pediatric Eye Disease Investigator Group. Stereoacuity in children with anisometropic amblyopia. J AAPOS. 2011; 15: 455–461.
Bhola R, Keech RV, Kutschke P, Pfeifer W, Scott WE. Recurrence of amblyopia after occlusion therapy. Ophthalmology. 2006; 113: 2097–2100.
Holmes J, Beck RW, Kraker RT,et al.; Pediatric Eye Disease Investigator Group. Risk of amblyopia recurrence after cessation of treatment. J AAPOS. 2004; 8: 420–428.
Wallace MP, Stewart CE, Moseley MJ, Dtephes DA, Fielder AR. Compliance with occlusion therapy for childhood amblyopia. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2013; 54: 6158–6166.
Lewis TL, Maurer D. Multiple sensitive periods in human visual development: evidence from visually deprived children. Dev Psychobiol. 2005; 46: 163–183.
Polat U, Ma-Naim T, Belkin M, Sagi D. Improving vision in adult amblyopia by perceptual learning. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2004; 101: 6692–6697.
Hussain Z, Webb BS, Astle AT, McGraw PV. Perceptual learning reduces crowding in amblyopia and in the normal periphery. J Neurosci. 2012; 32: 474–480.
Xi J, Jia WL, Feng LX, Lu ZL, Huang CB. Perceptual learning improves stereoacuity in amblyopia. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2014; 55: 2384–2391.
Black JM, Thompson B, Maehara G, Hess RF. A compact clinical instrument for quantifying suppression. Optom Vis Sci. 2011; 88: E334–E343.
Black JM, Hess RF, Cooperstock JR, To L, Thompson B. The measurement and treatment of suppression in amblyopia. J Vis Exp. 2012; 70: e3927.
Narasimhan S, Harrison ER, Giaschi DE. Quantitative measurement of interocular suppression in children with amblyopia. Vision Res. 2012; 66: 1–10.
Kwon M, Lu ZL, Miller A, Kazlas M, Hunter DG, Bex PJ. Assessing binocular interaction in amblyopia and its clinical feasibility. PLoS One. 2014; 9: e100156.
Kwon M, Wiecek E, Dakin SC, Bex PJ. Spatial-frequency dependent binocular imbalance in amblyopia. Sci Rep. 2015; 5: 17181.
Sengpiel F, Blakemore C. The neural basis of suppression and amblyopia in strabismus. Eye (Lond). 1996; 10: 250–258.
Kiorpes L, Kiper DC, O'Keefe LP, Cavanaugh JR, Movshon JA. Neuronal correlates of amblyopia in the visual cortex of macaque monkeys with experimental strabismus and anisometropia. J Neurosci. 1998; 18: 6411–6424.
Movshon JA, Eggers HM, Gizzi MS, Hendrickson AE, Kiorpes L, Boothe RG. Effects of early unilateral blur on the macaque's visual system. III. Physiological observations. J Neurosci. 1987; 7: 1340–1351.
Wiesel TN, Hubel DH. Single cell responses in striate cortex of kittens deprived of vision in one eye. J Neurophysiol. 1963; 26: 1003–1017.
Sengpiel F, Baddeley RJ, Freeman TC, Harrad R, Blakemore C. Different mechanisms underlie three inhibitory phenomena in cat area 17. Vision Res. 1998; 38: 2067–2080.
Li J, Thompson B, Lam CS, et al. The role of suppression in amblyopia. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2011; 52: 4169–4176.
Holopigian K, Blake R, Greenwald MJ. Selective losses in binocular vision in anisometropic amblyopes. Vision Res. 1986; 26: 621–630.
To L, Thompson B, Blum JR, Maehara G, Hess RF, Cooperstock JR. A game platform for treatment of amblyopia. IEEE Trans Neural Syst Rehabil Eng. 2011; 19: 280–289.
Hess RF, Thompson B, Black JM, et al. An iPod treatment of amblyopia: an updated binocular approach. Optometry. 2012; 83: 87–94.
Hess RF, Mansouri B, Thompson B. A binocular approach to treating amblyopia: antisuppression therapy. Optom Vis Sci. 2010; 87: 697–704.
Knox PJ, Simmers AJ, Gray LS, Cleary M. An exploratory study: prolonged periods of binocular stimulation can provide an effective treatment for childhood amblyopia. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2012; 53: 817–824.
Li SL, Jost RM, Morale SE, et al. A binocular iPad treatment for amblyopic children. Eye. 2014; 28: 1246–1253.
Birch EE, Li SL, Jost RM, et al. Binocular iPad treatment for amblyopia in preschool children. J AAPOS. 2015; 19: 6–11.
Herbison N, MacKeith D, Vivian A, et al. Randomised controlled trial of video clips and interactive games to improve vision in children with amblyopia using the I-BiT system. Br J Ophthalmol. 2016; 100: 1511–1516.
Levi DM, Li RW. Perceptual learning as a potential treatment for amblyopia: a mini-review. Vision Res. 2009; 49: 2535–2549.
Bavelier D, Levi DM, Li RW, Dan Y, Hensch TK. Removing brakes on adult brain plasticity: from molecular to behavioral interventions. J Neurosci. 2010; 30: 14964–14971.
Hussain Z, Astle AT, Webb BS, McGraw PV. The challenges of developing a contrast-based video game for treatment of amblyopia. Front Psychol. 2014; 5: 1210.
Vedamurthy I, Nahum M, Huang SJ, et al. A dichoptic custom-made action video game as a treatment for adult amblyopia. Vision Res. 2015; 114: 173–187.
Tsirlin I, Colpa L, Goltz HC, Wong AM. Behavioral training as new treatment for adult amblyopia: a meta-analysis and systematic review. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2015; 56: 4061–4075.
Mitchell DE, Duffy KR. The case from animal studies for balanced binocular treatment strategies for human amblyopia. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt. 2014; 34: 129–145.
Brainard DH. The psychophysics toolbox. Spat Vis. 1997; 10: 433–436.
Greenwood JA, Tailor VK, Sloper JJ, Simmers AJ, Bex PJ, Dakin SC. Visual acuity, crowding, and stereo-vision are linked in children with and without amblyopia. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2012; 53: 7655–7665.
Watson AB, Pelli DG. QUEST: a Bayesian adaptive psychometric method. Percept Psychophys. 1983; 33: 113–120.
Anketell PM, Saunders KJ, Little JA. Stereoacuity norms for school-age children using the Frisby stereotest. J AAPOS. 2013; 17: 582–587.
Piano M, Newsham D. A pilot study examining density of suppression measurement in strabismus. Strabismus. 2015; 23: 14–21.
Stewart CE, Fielder AR, Stephens DA, Moseley MJ. Treatment of unilateral amblyopia: factors influencing visual outcome. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2005; 46: 3152–3160.
Adams WE, Leske DA, Hatt SR, Holmes JM. Defining real change in measures of stereoacuity. Ophthalmology. 2009; 116: 281–285.
Vedamurthy I, Nahum M, Bavelier D, Levi DM. Mechanisms of recovery of visual function in adult amblyopia through a tailored action video game. Sci Rep. 2015; 5: 8482.
Kehrein S, Kohnen T, Fronius M. Dynamics of interocular suppression in amblyopic children during electronically monitored occlusion therapy: first insight. Strabismus. 2016; 24: 51–62.
Taylor K, Powell C, Hatt SR, Stewart C. Interventions for unilateral and bilateral refractive amblyopia. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2012; 4: CD005137.
Stewart CE, Moseley MJ, Stephens DA, Fielder AR. Treatment dose-response in amblyopia therapy: the Monitored Occlusion Treatment of Amblyopia Study (MOTAS). Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2004; 45: 3048–3054.
Levi DM, McKee SP, Movshon JA. Visual deficits in anisometropia. Vision Res. 2011; 51: 48–57.
Birch EE. Amblyopia and binocular vision. Prog Retin Eye Res. 2013; 33: 67–84.
Hess RF, Babu RJ, Clavagnier S, Black J, Bobier W, Thompson B. The iPod binocular home-based treatment for amblyopia in adults: efficacy and compliance. Clin Exp Optom. 2014; 97: 389–398.
Suttle CM. Active treatments for amblyopia: a review of the methods and evidence base. Clin Exp Optom. 2010; 93: 287–299.
Scheiman MM, Hertle RW, Beck RW,et al.; Pediatric Eye Disease Investigator Group. Randomized trial of treatment of amblyopia in children aged 7 to 17 years. Arch Ophthalmol. 2005; 123: 437–447.
Levi DM. Pathophysiology of binocular vision and amblyopia. Curr Opin Ophthalmol. 1994; 5: 3–10.
Hess RF, Mansouri B, Thompson B. Restoration of binocular vision in amblyopia. Strabismus. 2011; 19: 110–118.
Hess RF, Mansouri B, Thompson B. A new binocular approach to the treatment of amblyopia in adults well beyond the critical period of visual development. Restor Neurol Neurosci. 2010; 28: 793–802.
Tailor V, Bossi M, Bunce C, Greenwood JA, Dahlmann-Noor A. Binocular versus standard occlusion or blurring treatment for unilateral amblyopia in children aged three to eight years. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2014; 11: CD011347.
Agervi P, Kugelberg U, Kugelberg M, Simonsson G, Fornander M, Zetterström C. Treatment of anisometropic amblyopia with spectacles or in combination with translucent Bangerter filters. Ophthalmology. 2009; 116: 1475–1480.
Hess RF, Thompson B, Baker DH. Binocular vision in amblyopia: structure, suppression and plasticity. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt. 2014; 34: 146–162.
Hess RF, Thompson B. New insights into amblyopia: binocular therapy and noninvasive brain stimulation. J AAPOS. 2013; 17: 89–93.
Harrad R. Psychophysics of suppression. Eye. 1996; 10: 270–273.
Levi DM, Klein SA, Sharma V. Position jitter and undersampling in pattern perception. Vision Res. 1999; 39: 445–465.
Sharma V, Levi DM, Klein SA. Undercounting features and missing features: evidence for a high-level deficit in strabismic amblyopia. Nat Neurosci. 2000; 3: 496–501.
Hess RF, Anderson SJ. Motion sensitivity and spatial undersampling in amblyopia. Vision Res. 1993; 33: 881–896.
Clavagnier S, Dumoulin SO, Hess RF. Is the cortical deficit in amblyopia due to reduced cortical magnification, loss of neural resolution, or neural disorganization? J Neurosci. 2015; 35: 14740–14755.
Hess RF, Field DJ. Is the spatial deficit in strabismic amblyopia due to loss of cells or an uncalibrated disarray of cells? Vision Res. 1994; 34: 3397–3406.
Hussain Z, Svensson CM, Besle J, Webb BS, Barrett BT, McGraw PV. Estimation of cortical magnification from positional error in normally sighted and amblyopic subjects. J Vis. 2015; 15 (2): 25.
Levi DM, Klein SA, Chen I. What limits performance in the amblyopic visual system: seeing signals in noise with an amblyopic brain. J Vis. 2008; 8 (4): 1.
Dumoulin SO, Wandell BA. Population receptive field estimates in human visual cortex. Neuroimage. 2008; 39: 647–660.
Harvey BM, Dumoulin SO. The relationship between cortical magnification factor and population receptive field size in human visual cortex: constancies in cortical architecture. J Neurosci. 2011; 31: 13604–13612.
Gilbert CD, Wiesel TN. Receptive field dynamics in adult primary visual ortex. Nature. 1992; 356: 150–152.
Figure 1
 
(A) Treatment system: a personal computer, 3D-capable monitor, response-keypad, infrared emitter, and goggle-mounted shutter-glasses. (B) (Top) The child's view through AEs and FEs. The system applied sufficient blur to the FE to match acuity with the AE. (Bottom) Although movies are 2D, a shift in the relative position of each eye's image modulates the perceived depth of the movie. The zero-disparity textured background is also visible, which provides a vergence lock. Movie image © copyright 2008, Blender Foundation, www.bigbuckbunny.org, available under the Creative Commons Attribution License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/3.0/).
Figure 1
 
(A) Treatment system: a personal computer, 3D-capable monitor, response-keypad, infrared emitter, and goggle-mounted shutter-glasses. (B) (Top) The child's view through AEs and FEs. The system applied sufficient blur to the FE to match acuity with the AE. (Bottom) Although movies are 2D, a shift in the relative position of each eye's image modulates the perceived depth of the movie. The zero-disparity textured background is also visible, which provides a vergence lock. Movie image © copyright 2008, Blender Foundation, www.bigbuckbunny.org, available under the Creative Commons Attribution License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/3.0/).
Figure 2
 
The psychophysical tasks used (A, B) to establish blur level for the FE and (C) to quantify suppression during treatment. (A) Crowded “VacMan” stimulus used to measure acuity in the AE. (B) Stimuli used to estimate the blur level required to match the performance of the FE to the AE. (C) Suppression/compliance task. VacMan is flanked by two ghosts either positioned on the left and the right (as shown) or above and below (dashed outlines). Each ghost was a mixture of one dark and one light ghost presented to different eyes on each side (illustrated within the white circles). We quantify suppression as the mixture of luminance (L)-increments and -decrements required for the child to be equally likely to report either ghost as “whiter.” (D) Sample staircases for two observers. Gray text and horizontal line indicate the estimated balance point.
Figure 2
 
The psychophysical tasks used (A, B) to establish blur level for the FE and (C) to quantify suppression during treatment. (A) Crowded “VacMan” stimulus used to measure acuity in the AE. (B) Stimuli used to estimate the blur level required to match the performance of the FE to the AE. (C) Suppression/compliance task. VacMan is flanked by two ghosts either positioned on the left and the right (as shown) or above and below (dashed outlines). Each ghost was a mixture of one dark and one light ghost presented to different eyes on each side (illustrated within the white circles). We quantify suppression as the mixture of luminance (L)-increments and -decrements required for the child to be equally likely to report either ghost as “whiter.” (D) Sample staircases for two observers. Gray text and horizontal line indicate the estimated balance point.
Figure 3
 
(A, B) Acuity difference in the AE compared with baseline (BL) during treatment; changes below or above the dashed line, respectively, represent improvements or deteriorations in vision. Participants' amblyopia type was pure anisometropic (n = 7; [A]: squares), pure strabismic (n = 6; [A]: triangles), or combined (n = 9; [B]: circles). Thick lines show the mean change in acuity difference for each type of amblyopia (aniso. and strab. in [A]; combined amb. in [B]). Symbol-color codes the age of participants (age indicated in parentheses, in legends of parts [A, B]; from blue-younger to red-older children). Identity codes (1–24) are given next to individual lines and in the legends of (A) and (B) and label individual data point in (C). Note that group 1 (A) is anisometropic (i.e., treated for a maximum of 8 weeks), and group 2 (A, B) is strabismic and combined (treated for a maximum of 24 weeks; line length gives length of treatment). (C) Comparison of pre- and posttreatment acuity for each child. Points below the diagonal line are improvement, with the shaded region indicating gains less than 0.15 logMAR (considered critical of test-retest reliability).44 The dashed vertical line (at 0.6 logMAR) represents the cutoff between mild-to-moderate and severe amblyopia.
Figure 3
 
(A, B) Acuity difference in the AE compared with baseline (BL) during treatment; changes below or above the dashed line, respectively, represent improvements or deteriorations in vision. Participants' amblyopia type was pure anisometropic (n = 7; [A]: squares), pure strabismic (n = 6; [A]: triangles), or combined (n = 9; [B]: circles). Thick lines show the mean change in acuity difference for each type of amblyopia (aniso. and strab. in [A]; combined amb. in [B]). Symbol-color codes the age of participants (age indicated in parentheses, in legends of parts [A, B]; from blue-younger to red-older children). Identity codes (1–24) are given next to individual lines and in the legends of (A) and (B) and label individual data point in (C). Note that group 1 (A) is anisometropic (i.e., treated for a maximum of 8 weeks), and group 2 (A, B) is strabismic and combined (treated for a maximum of 24 weeks; line length gives length of treatment). (C) Comparison of pre- and posttreatment acuity for each child. Points below the diagonal line are improvement, with the shaded region indicating gains less than 0.15 logMAR (considered critical of test-retest reliability).44 The dashed vertical line (at 0.6 logMAR) represents the cutoff between mild-to-moderate and severe amblyopia.
Figure 4
 
Stereoacuity (when measurable) for children who completed treatment. (A) Before, during, and after treatment (at 0, 8, and 16 weeks respectively), (B) Pre- versus posttreatment. All children for whom data are shown (n = 7) had purely anisometropic amblyopia (group 1). Symbols are colored to reflect the relative age of each child compared with their peers (red = oldest). Boxed legend in (A) shows individual gain in stereoacuity (arcsec). For six participants, stereoacuity gains exceeded the test-retest variability threshold of 0.3 log arcsec and a step of one octave (shaded area).
Figure 4
 
Stereoacuity (when measurable) for children who completed treatment. (A) Before, during, and after treatment (at 0, 8, and 16 weeks respectively), (B) Pre- versus posttreatment. All children for whom data are shown (n = 7) had purely anisometropic amblyopia (group 1). Symbols are colored to reflect the relative age of each child compared with their peers (red = oldest). Boxed legend in (A) shows individual gain in stereoacuity (arcsec). For six participants, stereoacuity gains exceeded the test-retest variability threshold of 0.3 log arcsec and a step of one octave (shaded area).
Figure 5
 
Day-by-day estimates of suppression (R: % reliance on FE, see Treatment Regimen) for participants with anisometropia (upper row; group 1) and/or with strabismus (group 2). Participants' ID numbers are indicated at the bottom left of each subplot. Here, 50% means “balanced vision,” 100% indicates complete reliance on the FE (i.e., complete suppression of the AE), and 0% indicates complete suppression of the FE. Green symbols pool data within three periods (beginning, middle, end; binned around the individual duration of BBV; note ID4 was not compliant for a period, hence the middle bin is missing). We derived black trend lines from linear regression analysis of daily estimates. An asterisk after the child's ID indicates a significant reduction in suppression for that child (P < 0.05).
Figure 5
 
Day-by-day estimates of suppression (R: % reliance on FE, see Treatment Regimen) for participants with anisometropia (upper row; group 1) and/or with strabismus (group 2). Participants' ID numbers are indicated at the bottom left of each subplot. Here, 50% means “balanced vision,” 100% indicates complete reliance on the FE (i.e., complete suppression of the AE), and 0% indicates complete suppression of the FE. Green symbols pool data within three periods (beginning, middle, end; binned around the individual duration of BBV; note ID4 was not compliant for a period, hence the middle bin is missing). We derived black trend lines from linear regression analysis of daily estimates. An asterisk after the child's ID indicates a significant reduction in suppression for that child (P < 0.05).
Figure 6
 
Estimates of suppression for 21 children (ID4 excluded; IDs numbers to label the correspondent data points; markers colored from blue-younger to red-older children across all recruited children) (A) Suppression (R) is similar at entry compared with exit from treatment. (B) There is only a modest correlation of gain in visual acuity (VA) with change in suppression (r = 0.193), which does not reach statistical significance (P = 0.402). The horizontal dashed line represents the level of “balanced vision.” VA gains outside of the shaded region are considered clinically significant (≥ 0.2 logMAR). Error bars indicate 95% confidence intervals on acuity (horizontal) and suppression (vertical) estimates.
Figure 6
 
Estimates of suppression for 21 children (ID4 excluded; IDs numbers to label the correspondent data points; markers colored from blue-younger to red-older children across all recruited children) (A) Suppression (R) is similar at entry compared with exit from treatment. (B) There is only a modest correlation of gain in visual acuity (VA) with change in suppression (r = 0.193), which does not reach statistical significance (P = 0.402). The horizontal dashed line represents the level of “balanced vision.” VA gains outside of the shaded region are considered clinically significant (≥ 0.2 logMAR). Error bars indicate 95% confidence intervals on acuity (horizontal) and suppression (vertical) estimates.
Figure 7
 
Compliance and attention. (A) Daily dose in minutes is plotted against acuity (VA) gains in logMAR for the AE. The trend line shows how a higher daily dose (individual mean number of minutes per day spent watching movies) was associated with greater improvement in VA. (B) Percentage of lapse trials on the whitest-ghost task (“invalid responses,” i.e., where the child either indicated a location where a ghost was not present or did not respond at all) as a function of age. Younger children are more prone to lapsing, possibly indicating poorer attention to the task.
Figure 7
 
Compliance and attention. (A) Daily dose in minutes is plotted against acuity (VA) gains in logMAR for the AE. The trend line shows how a higher daily dose (individual mean number of minutes per day spent watching movies) was associated with greater improvement in VA. (B) Percentage of lapse trials on the whitest-ghost task (“invalid responses,” i.e., where the child either indicated a location where a ghost was not present or did not respond at all) as a function of age. Younger children are more prone to lapsing, possibly indicating poorer attention to the task.
Table 1
 
Baseline Details of Participants (n = 24)
Table 1
 
Baseline Details of Participants (n = 24)
Table 2
 
Current and Proposed Treatments for Amblyopia
Table 2
 
Current and Proposed Treatments for Amblyopia
Supplement 1
×
×

This PDF is available to Subscribers Only

Sign in or purchase a subscription to access this content. ×

You must be signed into an individual account to use this feature.

×