The mean cone intensity was significantly different between healthy and NPDR eyes, but only if the average between the retinal locations was considered (
Tables 1,
2). In addition, if all data at different locations were taken into account, there was a dependence on time, and this was the only parameter for which this was the case. This dependence on retinal location, together with the lack of significant differences in standard deviations and skewness between groups, indicated that the histograms of cone intensities are not good candidates to investigate differences in cone reflectance caused by diabetes mellitus.
Under the assumption that the intensity of the pixels inside the segmentations is primarily directional light backscattered from the outer/inner segment (IS/OS) junction and pixel intensity outside the cone segmentations corresponds to nondirectional backscattered light,
54–56 in healthy subjects there was a significantly higher fraction of directional backscattered light than in those with NPDR, given by the higher ratio of cone/intercone intensity. A significant difference in cone intensity in areas where the cones still were detectable, although with abnormal packing density arrangement, has been found previously.
14–16,19 A possible explanation could be the alteration of the wave-guiding properties due to the pathology of the individual cones.
16 The significance of the retinal location for this factor could suggest that this variation of cone intensity, even if important enough at all locations so that its average still is significantly different between groups, might not be constant across the central retina (e.g., caused by variable effect of intraretinal scattering, and so forth). Another possible explanation could be given by the rods, which are not resolved in these images but may be present sporadically in between the cones at these eccentricities. In this case, the difference in the ratio would indicate a change in the rod reflectance, or a combination of changes in rods and cones. Even if significant, the difference between the ratio in the two groups had a small numerical value (
Table 3), as in the case of the mean intensity, which could make the use of this parameter for clinical applications hard to achieve. Further investigation of the ratio of cone/intercone intensity at different illumination angles could improve the understanding of how this phenomenon is correlated with cone function and if it eventually also is related to cone spatial properties, such as spatial density or packing arrangement.
We chose to use a sample area of 200 μm side, which was bigger than what has been used in similar studies,
14,15 to have an area big enough to evaluate the reflectance property of several clusters of cones in all subjects. From the analysis of the spatial distribution of cone reflectance through the variograms, we were able to infer a significant difference between the two groups and this result was consistent between the average of the locations and for all locations considered. The NPDR curves showed a shallower slope at short distances (<20 μm), reflecting the fact that the cones appear to be clustered in bright and dark patches more than in healthy subjects. In addition, the average variogram curves in NPDR eyes showed peaks at long distances (>50 μm), which were not found in healthy subjects. These peaks corresponded to the distance at which there was a greater difference in the observed cone intensity. In NPDR eyes, it is likely that the position of the peaks reveals the size of the bright and dark intensity patches present in the images (see
Supplementary Materials). The intersubject variability in the shapes would make challenging the definition of a characteristic “standard” variogram shape in an adult population at this moment. For this reason, we analyzed the initial slope of the variograms, which showed potential as a straightforward method to quantify the degree of spatial dependence of cone intensities over a short range. The clustering of cone reflectances did not show a significant dependence on the retinal location, implying that difference in these two aspects of cone reflectance between healthy and NPDR eyes might have different causes. This dependence can be caused by the physiology/pathophysiology of the photoreceptor layer itself or of other layers. Further work is needed to investigate suitable models to fit experimental data and develop a more complete procedure to compare curves from different subjects or retinal locations. In addition, more work could be done to understand how this approach can be extended to other retinal diseases. Strictly related to the multiple humped shape of the variogram found in NPDR eyes, clustering of the cone reflectivity has been qualitatively observed in DR
14,19 and inherited retinal diseases. For example, in albinism, clustering has been related to the melanin distribution in the retina;
30 in choroidermia, hyperreflective clusters of cones have been related to alteration in the RPE.
33 Degeneration of the RPE also has been indicated as a cause of disruption of cone reflectivity in age-related macular degeneration, possibly before the formation of drusen.
34
The inner retina has been shown to influence the low frequency pattern of cone intensity in AO flood illumination images of the cone mosaic (
Fig. 9). The presence of subclinical or clinically visible abnormalities located in the inner layers of the retina (e.g., microaneurysms, microvascular abnormalities, subtle retinal edema) might further explain the greater clumping found in NPDR eyes than controls.
16 A thickening of the inner retinal layers, which could not be considered as macular edema, has been found previously in diabetic eyes.
14,19 Further work is ensured to correlate the changes in the cone reflectance properties with the abnormalities in spectral domain-optical coherence tomography (SD-OCT) cross-section images of the same subject. Another possible explanation could be a high degree of alignment between neighboring cones, with clusters of cones pointing towards the pupil center appearing here brighter than clusters of cones pointing away from the center. This hypothesis could be investigated again, as in the case of nondirectional backscattered light, with observations at different illumination angles.
27,54
In any case, even if the source of the modifications in cone reflectance was the result of shadowing artifacts and not of abnormalities in the cones, their quantification still can be used as an indicator of the disease.
We also analyzed the texture of the parafoveal cone mosaic with two metrics originally used for the assessment of image quality. The reason behind the choice of only these two metrics, sharpness and entropy, from the cited study
50 is that these investigators found other quality metrics, such as variance, contrast, and kurtosis, to be highly correlated with sharpness, while entropy was not. In this way, we were able to explore different aspects on the images, such as the definition of bright features over the background (sharpness) and the textural properties of the image (entropy). Entropy was significantly higher in the AO images of the cone mosaic in NPDR eyes than controls, while sharpness was very similar between the two groups. A possible explanation could be that sharpness measures the definition of the cone apertures as bright dots over a darker background. In this sense, this characteristic seems not to depend on the condition of the retina. Entropy, on the other hand, relies on the histogram distribution (
Fig. 6). The fact that entropy involves taking the logarithm of the histogram makes it more sensitive to extreme intensity values, and so it is able to detect a difference in images with apparently similar histograms (in a previous study, sharpness was highly correlated with the image variance, while entropy was not).
50
We did not observe the characteristics of cone reflectance or mosaic texture of the same areas in AO flood illuminated retinal images to change significantly with time. In a previous study,
40 we found that although the cones individually change reflectance over time in a random way, the distribution of the cone intensities maintains the same shape over time, and this result was confirmed here. This also was true for the retinas affected by NPDR, as we also observed no significant interaction between time and condition. A possible explanation for the lack of change, especially for the NPDR group, is that the time range considered was too short compared to the development time of the disease. Furthermore, the retinal location proved not to be a significant factor influencing the cone reflectance in AO flood retinal images, meaning that the cone mosaic has similar intensity and textural characteristics at different locations at the same distance from the fovea. This result confirmed the validity of using the average of the values of the four locations as one global parameter with potential to be translated to clinical studies.
An important aspect of this work is the automation of the analysis. We used our previously described method
40 for the analysis of large retinal patches (≥2 × 2° or ≥0.61 × 0.61 mm), showing how performing the detection on time averaged images further improved the performance of the automated cone detection algorithm, which is 97% of cones correctly detected using only one image. The improvement in the automated detection performance using more images taken at different times also can be justified for the NPDR images, which can present a decrease in cone density but no major deterioration that would require manual supervision. On similar images, the investigators in a different study
14 have used an automated algorithm with poorer performance
57 and the percentage of cones that had to be corrected manually was not greater than 9% also for NPDR cases, which is compatible with the performance of the same algorithm on healthy retinas.
57
Limitations of this study included the small sample size (especially the NPDR cases), though it had enough power to verify the hypothesis of the study, and the incomplete number of observations with time in all cases. Future work could include a more consistent number of subjects at different stages of the disease and a longer time range, which could lead to observation of change on the same retina with time. Another improvement could be achieved using complementary observation modalities, such as AO-scanning laser ophthalmoscopy (AO-SLO) and/or AO-OCT, as well as using different illumination angles, which could provide a better resolution closer to the fovea (<2°) and insights into the causes of differences in cone reflectance. Finally, analysis of the function of the retina in the selected regions also could be performed, to determine the clinical significance of the differences between healthy and NPDR eyes.
In conclusion, we observed significant differences in cone mosaic reflectance properties between healthy eyes and eyes with mild NPDR, in its spatial organization and in its intensity, especially between directional and nondirectional backscattering. We did not observe significant changes of the parameters with time in any group. We performed a largely automated analysis of cone reflectance and introduced a novel method for the study of the spatial distribution of intensity, the variogram, which was able to quantify differences of the spatial dependence of intensity values of the cone layer at a short range between NPDR and control eyes and a tendency of cones in NPDR to appear clustered in clumps of similar intensities.