Unlike previous studies, the current study did not see that glaucomatous reading speed was significantly associated with either binocular visual acuity, binocular contrast sensitivity, or 24–2 visual field MD. Although speculative, it may be related to any of the reasons that follow. First, as the current study employed predominantly early or moderate stage of glaucoma (82%), it might have had low statistical power to observe the significant effects of visual acuity, contrast sensitivity, and visual field MD on reading speed. For this reason, we acknowledge that a future study should consider a wide range of glaucoma stages including glaucoma suspects and more severe stages of glaucoma to better characterize the effects of glaucomatous damage on reading ability and the size of the visual span including other visual functions. On the other hand, our results suggest that the visual span may be a more sensitive and relevant assessment tool to capture glaucomatous reading impairments when compared to visual acuity, contrast sensitivity, stereoacuity, or visual field MD. Second, unlike previous studies, the current study assessed oral reading speed with short blocks of simple and standard text (flashcard method in which a line of text occupied the central 11 degrees of the visual field), which might have underestimated particular aspects of glaucomatous reading difficulties. For example, it has been shown that individuals with glaucoma appear to have more difficulties with long passage reading: Ramulu et al.
3 found that glaucoma patients exhibit more pronounced reading deficits in sustained silent reading than out-loud reading using short text; a study by Mathews et al.
13 further demonstrated that compared to glaucoma suspect controls, glaucoma patients take a longer time changing to the next line of text during reading. Thus, it is possible that the use of short text reading might have underestimated any effect of visual field defects (mean deviation) on reading speed because visual field defects (a 24–2 visual field test) were more pronounced in the far periphery. Reading is a complex task involving various sensory, cognitive, and linguistic components. Thus, different reading measures likely tap into different aspects of the reading process.
65 In this study, we focused on the role of bottom-up, visual sensory factors in glaucoma-related reading deficits while minimizing higher-level cognitive and linguistic influences. For this reason, we adopted the flashcard method as it has been proven to examine the role of vision in reading speed.
24,37,40,66,67 Particularly, the same flashcard reading speed was shown to be significantly correlated with developmental changes in the size of visual span.
37 Nonetheless, we acknowledge that a future study should consider using other reading speed measures (e.g., sustained silent reading) to see if the observed pattern of results could be generalized to other types of reading measures. Finally, the current study relied on visual field MD (i.e., a global measure of glaucoma severity) as an overall index of glaucomatous damage. However, vision loss in different parts of the visual field likely causes different degrees of impairment in a person's reading ability. Thus, a future study should also address how the location and spatial extent of glaucomatous field defects influence reading speed in glaucoma.