Our data show that the KG has a higher frequency of strabismus when compared to the general population
24–26 and to the CG. Besides binocular alignment, simultaneous perception is also a prerequisite for the third degree of binocularity (stereopsis). We observed that among 44 orthotropic subjects in the KG, suppression responses were observed in 4 (9%) subjects, contributing to impaired stereopsis in this group. This also happened in two subjects with high ametropia in the CG, suggesting that the suppression response may be triggered by the presence of high astigmatism.
The KG and CG were not different relative to the first and second degrees of binocular vision (simultaneous perception, fusion, and fusional convergence). As simultaneous perception is a prerequisite for fusion, the number of subjects evaluated for fusional convergence was 40, that is, the ones that demonstrated simultaneous perception with both striate Maddox and 6
Δ base-down prism tests. Although the KG and CG were not different relative to first and second degrees of binocularity, our data demonstrate that there was an association between good visual acuity and simultaneous perception, as subjects from KG whose BCVA in the worse eye was logMAR ≥0.7 were associated with a higher frequency of strabismus and a higher chance of having absence of simultaneous perception (
Tables 4,
5).
As in anisometropia with amblyopia, one of the factors associated with an increased frequency of strabismus or impaired binocular vision is the difference in visual acuity between the two eyes. Our data show decreased visual acuity in both eyes in the KG and a difference in visual acuity between the better and worse eye of logMAR 0.35 in the KG. Although careful refractometry improved at least two lines of vision in the better eye and almost three lines in the worse eye in the KG (
Table 3), relevant differences between BCVA from both eyes remained, and were associated with impaired binocular vision, as occurs in amblyopia associated with strabismus and anisometropia.
6,16 Optical correction with glasses of the refractive errors did not change the KG stereopsis performance, similar to that seen by Lee et al.
27 None of the GC or KG subjects were contact lens wearers. One possibility is that the adaptation of hard contact lenses, which improves the irregularities of the anterior corneal surface, reduces aniseikonia, and improves visual acuity, might improve their sensory status.
Regarding the third degree of binocular vision (stereopsis), the KG subjects without strabismus but with simultaneous perception still show a significantly impaired stereoscopic performance (
Fig. 3). Although studies of stereopsis and its prevalence in the general population have much bias (such as the choice of nonnäive observers, observers with clinical characteristics that could influence the results, or the use of nonstandardized stereopsis tests and/or nonstandardized methods for exclusion of participants),
11 Heron et al.
28 showed that in an adult population, about 85% of the observers had a stereoacuity equal to or better than 50 arcsec. In our data, however, only 10 (18%) subjects in the KG demonstrated stereopsis equal to 60 arcsec or better, while stereopsis was present in 22 (76%) of the CG subjects. Our study agrees with others that described impaired binocular vision in keratoconus subjects. In 2000, Brahma et al.
8 also found gross stereopsis in keratoconus subjects. After penetrating keratoplasty in one eye of keratoconus subjects, he found median stereopsis of 360 arcsec, although the logMAR visual acuity median of the operated eyes was 0.07. There were no data about the visual acuities of the nonoperated eyes. In 2001, Sherafat et al.
9 studied 20 subjects with keratoconus who used rigid contact lenses in the worse eye. He found only 1 patient with a stereopsis of 60 arcsec, and 13 out of 20 had stereopsis equal to 480 arcsec or worse. Their visual acuities ranged from logMAR 0 to 1.0. These data agree with the hypothesis that improvement of visual acuity, either with contact lenses or with corneal transplantation, was not sufficient to allow stereopsis to recover in these subjects to 60 arcsec or better.
Although low vision in one eye was associated with an increased frequency of strabismus and the absence of simultaneous perception, we did not find a correlation between BCVA of logMAR > 0.3 in at least in one eye and a higher frequency of impaired stereopsis, showing that some other factors may be contributing to impaired stereopsis.
We also examined whether anisometropia was associated with stereopsis performance of the keratoconus subjects, as other studies have already demonstrated reduced stereoacuity observed experimentally
29,30 or clinically.
31 Astigmatism occurs mostly in the surface of the anterior cornea, which is also true in keratoconus subjects. In the absence of keratoconus, prescription of the cylindrical degree is similar to the values found in corneal tomography. In keratoconus, however, this does not occur in the same way. Corneal tomography scans show fairly high irregular astigmatism values that are incompletely corrected by cylindrical lenses. Consequently, keratoconus subjects frequently do not get complete visual acuity improvement and usually cannot use all the cylinder correction. For this reason, anisometropia in keratoconus subjects was estimated both based on the manifest refractometry chosen subjectively by them (spherical equivalent anisometropia was calculated based on their subjective manifest refractometry) and also based on objective measurements such as Km (mean of the flat [K1] and the steep [K2] meridians) and frontal astigmatism of the anterior cornea surface, both obtained by corneal tomography.
Considering subjective manifest refractometry, although spherical equivalent anisometropia ≥ 1.0 spherical diopters equivalent was more frequent in the KG than in the CG (
P = 0.01;
Table 6), we did not find an association between spherical equivalent anisometropia <1.0 spherical diopters and a better stereopsis performance (
P = 0.06) in the KG. Lee et al.,
27 on the other hand, found worse stereopsis and a greater proportion subjects with gross stereopsis in the anisometropic group. However, he studied non-amblyopic children. The nonsignificance of our data might be explained by the fact that the CG was composed of approximately 40% (11 out of 29) of subjects with myopia and/or astigmatism equal to or greater than 3.5 cylinder diopters in both eyes, and 3 also had anisometropia between 1.0 and 2.25 spherical equivalent. These characteristics also increased the frequency of impaired stereopsis in the CG to a frequency of 24% (7 out of 29 subjects in the CG had gross stereopsis). It is important to remember that, as already mentioned, keratoconus was discarded in the CG by corneal tomography.
All refractometries were performed by a single experienced ophthalmologist. However, these values are still highly subjective in keratoconus subjects. For this reason, we also analyzed two other objective parameters, Km and frontal astigmatism. Although Km values were not different between eyes in the CG and KG, we found that frontal astigmatism was significantly different in the eyes of the CG (gross and fine stereopsis,
P = 0.01), as well as in the eyes of the KG (gross and fine stereopsis,
P = 0.03). In other words, no matter which subject group was examined (CG or KG), the amount of frontal astigmatism was significantly higher in the groups with gross stereopsis. The distortion caused by irregular high astigmatism on the frontal corneal surface may explain the disruptive effect on the ability to perform stereopsis tests, as observed by Asaria et al.
32 in subjects with epiretinal membranes.
Our data agree with other studies, in which experimentally induced
29,30 or clinically observed
31 astigmatism was associated with stereopsis reduction. Increasing the number of subjects with astigmatism in both the KG and CG might give us clues to determine the amount of astigmatism at which stereopsis becomes impaired. Looking back to the CG, 12 out of 29 subjects had astigmatism equal to or higher than 3.5 cylinder diopters and, curiously, 6 out of 7 subjects with gross stereopsis had astigmatism higher than 3.5 cylinder diopters. These data reinforce the need for more studies in order to improve our knowledge on the effect of astigmatism on stereoacuity. It is possible that one-meridian amblyopia in highly astigmatic subjects in both groups may explain this finding, depending on their age when they started using their optical correction.
33
Although the age reported as the onset of the impaired vision was 12 years old, it is difficult to know at which age the ectasia and its consequences on the binocular system began to develop, since keratoconus is a disease with asymmetric progression. This information, on the other hand, reinforces the importance of a closer follow-up of teenagers with a family history of keratoconus, strabismus, atopy, anisometropia, and poor eyesight.
In our study, no patient complained of diplopia, contrary to what Khan and Al-Shamsi described in 2008.
10 He described seven subjects with strabismus and keratoconus, who were referred for treatment of diplopia after a surgical procedure meant to improve the vision of the worse eye. This difference in findings may be explained by the fact that Khan series was obtained from subjects with diplopia who were referred to an outpatient strabismus clinic. In our series, the KG subjects were followed up after the diagnosis of keratoconus in a corneal service. They did not have diplopia, and some of them were not even aware of their ocular deviation.
There are some limitations to our study. First, this was a cross-sectional study, and new assessments could alter the results. The ability to perceive smaller angles of stereopsis using different approaches may be possible, since it is known that the contour-based stereoscopic tests overestimate stereopsis due to monocular cues.
34 Second, refractometry was not performed under cycloplegia due to peripheral aberrations in the KG. Although the examiner had significant experience with refractometry, this fact may have influenced the data collected. Third, axial lengths were not measured, so corneal refractive errors could not be separated from axial errors. Fourth, we do not know the exact time period when these subjects developed keratoconus and the exact period when they started using optical correction, nor do we know its influence on stereopsis over time, including in the CG. Fifth, impaired stereopsis may be associated with convergence insufficiency, and this issue was not investigated in our subjects. However, it is possible that convergence insufficiency could be a cause or consequence of impaired stereopsis.
In conclusion, keratoconus subjects using their best optical correction (glasses) had a higher frequency of strabismus and absence of simultaneous perception. This was associated with low vision at least in one of the eyes. Stereopsis performance was also significantly different in keratoconus subjects when compared to controls. Anisometropia did not appear to cause a deterioration of stereopsis in the studied groups. Frontal astigmatism was significantly higher in both CG and KG subjects with gross stereopsis. Improvement in the early diagnosis of keratoconus, through corneal tomography, as well as a better understanding of factors that help in the stereoscopic perception of space, may help us better understand the factors associated with stereoscopic impairment.