How does MNTA relate to the development of EIE?
Figure 2, from an article by Behrens and associates,
17,18 shows the neuroanatomy of MNTA. The subcortical motion pathways (demarcated by a red oval) are normally operative in the early months of human infancy.
20,21 In investigating EIE, we have put our magnifying glass on secondary aberrations within the motion pathways of the visual cortex (demarcated by a green oval); however, the innervational changes that actually cause the eyes to turn in are generated within the subcortical motion pathways. In
Figure 2, the left eye is receiving nasalward optokinetic input, which crosses through the chiasm to the right NOT and DTN-AOS. These structures respond to rightward optokinetic input (nasalward for the left eye), and project their output signal to the dorsal cap of the inferior olive, which acts as a comparator of motor commands from the cerebral cortex and brainstem nuclei and feedback from receptors via the spinal cord, visual system, and vestibular organs. The inferior olive relays gated information to the cerebellar flocculus, which processes visuo-vestibular input.
15–18 The cerebellar flocculus then sends its output signal to the vestibular nucleus, which integrates visual motion input from the eyes with head motion input from the labyrinths. The vestibular nucleus provides a prenuclear signal to the ocular motor nuclei, which innervate the appropriate extraocular muscles to minimize retinal slip.
Beginning at 2 to 3 months of age, binocular cells from the visual cortex, which are bidirectionally sensitive to horizontal optokinetic motion, begin to establish connections to the NOT-DTN, allowing foveal pursuit to override the monocular subcortical optokinetic bias. By 6 months of age, monocular horizontal responses to optokinetic stimuli become symmetrical. According to the Hoffmann hypothesis,
22–24 when the development of cortical binocular vision is preempted, only the crossed nasal fibers from the left eye have the necessary rightward visual motion sensitivity to project through the right visual cortex (middle temporal/medial superior temporal [MT/MST]) and connect to the right NOT-DTN to form one unified system and run visual motion responses monocularly. Hebbian mechanisms
25 (popularly paraphrased as “neurons that fire together wire together”) require the monocular horizontal motion sensitivity of foveal pursuit (a cortical function) to be paradoxically dictated by the preexisting subcortical substrate of the NOT-DTN. Thus, uncrossed monocular fibers from the right temporal retina, which are selectively sensitive to leftward motion, cannot establish connections to the right NOT-DTN.
Because cortical motion pathways must feed into this preexisting subcortical directional bias,
24 you end up with a secondary cortical pursuit motion asymmetry and foveal pursuit asymmetry to optokinetic stimuli that we see every day in clinic when we spin the optokinetic drum nasally and temporally.
6 Once monocular cortical pursuit connections become established, the subcortical motion pathways can be driven by nasalward foveal pursuit pathways within the visual cortex (mainly MT but also MST),
24 allowing cortical suppression of one eye to signal binocular visual imbalance and thereby trigger subcortical visual reflexes.
7,26
Within the comparative biology literature, it is striking that each of the unique eye movements described in infantile esotropia, be it latent nystagmus, dissociated vertical deviation, or oblique muscle overaction, have precise analogs in lower lateral-eyed animals, and the subcortical neuroanatomical pathways for all of these movements are well-established.
27 For example, MNTA underlies the clinical phenomenon of latent nystagmus in humans.
6–8,28 The expression of these atavisms suggests that this subcortical visual motion circuitry is integral to the pathogenesis of EIE.
6 In fact, a defining feature of EIE is the persistence of subcortical visual reflexes with signature torsional rotations of the eyes. In EIE, the older subcortical visual system runs the show, which explains why its neuroanatomy is so rarely included in neuro-ophthalmology textbooks. To understand it, we must shine our flashlight into the basement of the brain where old subcortical visual reflexes that are put to sleep in early infancy get reactivated.