Consistent with the above-mentioned two reports, we also showed a clear binocular imbalance in amblyopes. Nevertheless, there are several differences between our study and these two previous reports. First, there are different numbers of subjects. There were four amblyopes in the study of Huang et al.,
8 two amblyopes in the study of Ding et al.,
26 and eight amblyopes in the current study. The interocular interactions could be different in different patients, as is illustrated in
Figure 3 (also see Ref.
27). For example, for amblyopic subjects S3 and S7, the two interocular suppression magnitudes (good to weak eye, weak to good eye) were similar and small. On the other hand, amblyopic subjects S1 and S8 showed relatively larger suppression from the good to weak eye, even more so than all the normal controls. Nevertheless, we didn't find meaningful differences between the amblyopes with and without strabismus. Larger samples in future studies would be necessary to clarify the differences between different types of amblyopia. Second, different paradigms are used. In the studies of Huang et al.
8 and Ding et al.,
26 suprathreshold binocular interactions were investigated when the contrasts in both eyes were well above the contrast thresholds. In contrast, when we tested the suppression from the mask (in the untested eye) to the grating (in the tested eye), only the mask was suprathreshold (five times the contrast threshold), while the grating was at threshold. The advantage of using this dichoptic-masking approach is that the suppression from the tested eye (seeing the threshold grating) to the untested eye (seeing the suprathreshold mask) is quite minimal, thus enabling one to directly quantify the suppression from the eye seeing the mask in terms of the threshold of the other eye. Third, different contrast levels are used. In both the studies of Huang et al.
8 and Ding et al.,
26 the contrast in the amblyopic eye was fixed at one particular level (i.e., base contrast), and the binocular perceived phase and contrast was measured by varying the contrast in the fellow eye to find the interocular contrast ratio when the two eyes were balanced in binocular combination. We would argue that a fixed visibility criterion (i.e., five times the contrast threshold) is better than a fixed absolute contrast criterion (as used in the above-mentioned two studies) in the current study. It enables a better comparison across individuals or spatial frequencies where contrast thresholds under these conditions could be very different. Also, previous studies have shown that different base contrast could produce different extents of binocular imbalance, even in the same observer.
8,26 This difference might account for the different spatial-frequency dependency of the binocular imbalance in different studies: for example, Ding et al.
26 found more binocular imbalance at higher spatial frequencies (0.68 vs. 1.36 cyc/deg), while we didn't find this phenomenon within the range of 0.67 to 1.31 cyc/deg.