September 2018
Volume 59, Issue 11
Open Access
Eye Movements, Strabismus, Amblyopia and Neuro-ophthalmology  |   September 2018
Dichoptic Attentive Motion Tracking is Biased Toward the Nonamblyopic Eye in Strabismic Amblyopia
Author Affiliations & Notes
  • Amy Chow
    Optometry and Vision Science, University of Waterloo, Waterloo, Ontario, Canada
  • Deborah Giaschi
    Department of Ophthalmology and Visual Sciences, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada
  • Benjamin Thompson
    Optometry and Vision Science, University of Waterloo, Waterloo, Ontario, Canada
    Optometry and Vision Science, University of Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand
  • Correspondence: Amy Chow, Department of Optometry and Vision Science, University of Waterloo, 200 Columbia Street West Waterloo, ON N2L 3G1, Canada; [email protected]
Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science September 2018, Vol.59, 4572-4580. doi:https://doi.org/10.1167/iovs.18-25236
  • Views
  • PDF
  • Share
  • Tools
    • Alerts
      ×
      This feature is available to authenticated users only.
      Sign In or Create an Account ×
    • Get Citation

      Amy Chow, Deborah Giaschi, Benjamin Thompson; Dichoptic Attentive Motion Tracking is Biased Toward the Nonamblyopic Eye in Strabismic Amblyopia. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 2018;59(11):4572-4580. https://doi.org/10.1167/iovs.18-25236.

      Download citation file:


      © ARVO (1962-2015); The Authors (2016-present)

      ×
  • Supplements
Abstract

Purpose: To determine whether attention is biased toward the nonamblyopic eye under binocular viewing conditions in adults with anisometropic or strabismic amblyopia. We first determined whether attention could be allocated preferentially to one eye in visually normal observers performing a dichoptic attentive motion tracking task. We then assessed dichoptic attentive motion tracking in amblyopia.

Methods: Participants performed a multiple-object tracking task under the following three viewing conditions: target dots to the dominant eye and distractor dots to the nondominant eye (DE condition), vice versa (NDE condition), or all dots to both eyes (binocular condition). Interocular attentional asymmetry scores were computed as the difference in accuracy between DE and NDE conditions. An interocular contrast difference favoring the amblyopic eye was used for all conditions to neutralize amblyopic eye suppression. To test for confounding effects of suppression, participants completed a separate dot enumeration task under dichoptic presentation conditions to obtain an interocular enumeration asymmetry score.

Results: Participants with normal vision demonstrated similar accuracy between the DE and NDE conditions and exhibited slightly impaired performance under dichoptic compared with binocular viewing conditions. Participants with strabismic/mixed amblyopia had significantly higher interocular attentional asymmetry than participants with normal vision or with anisometropic amblyopia, whereby attention was biased toward the nonamblyopic eye. The latter two groups did not exhibit a bias in interocular attention. No interocular asymmetries for the enumeration task were observed for any group.

Conclusions: A nonamblyopic eye bias in the interocular allocation of attention may contribute to the binocular vision impairments caused by strabismic amblyopia.

Abnormal binocular visual experience during early childhood, typically due to a large difference in refractive error between the eyes (anisometropia) or a deviated eye (strabismus), can disrupt visual development and cause amblyopia. Amblyopia is characterized clinically by decreased visual acuity in the affected eye that cannot be entirely accounted for by refractive error or pathology.1,2 Beyond the reduction in visual acuity, amblyopia also causes a broad range of visual deficits ranging from impairments in contrast sensitivity37 to the global processing of motion and form.817 A number of these deficits also extend to the nonamblyopic fellow eye.8,12,14,1821 
Beyond the loss of monocular visual function, amblyopia also impairs binocular vision, in part because visual information from the amblyopic eye is suppressed from conscious awareness when both eyes are viewing.2227 Suppression can affect large areas of the amblyopic eye visual field28,29 and may play an important role in the visual deficits experienced by patients with amblyopia.22,25,30 Stronger suppression is associated with poorer visual acuity in the amblyopic eye25,31,32 and treatments that aim to reduce suppression improve amblyopic eye visual acuity to some extent,3340 although randomized clinical trial evidence for this approach is mixed.4143 
The abnormal patterns of interocular excitation and inhibition that may contribute to suppression are evident within early visual areas of amblyopic primates.4449 However, it has recently been suggested that abnormal allocation of visual attention between the two eyes may also play a role in suppression.50 Using electroencephalography, Hou and colleagues50 observed a reduced effect of attentional modulation in areas V1, V4, and V5 when participants with strabismic amblyopia viewed monocularly with their amblyopic eye. Importantly, they also found a strong positive correlation between the extent of the attentional modulation deficit in V1 and the strength of amblyopic eye suppression. Reduced attentional modulation was also observed for fellow eye viewing in V4 and V5, but not V1. Similarly, another study identified differences in event-related potential waveforms during processing of the Stroop task for children with amblyopia relative to controls.51 Other studies have used psychophysics to investigate attentive processing in amblyopia under monocular viewing conditions, with mixed results. Impairments have been reported in multiple-object tracking,52,53 conjunction visual search,54 and attentional blink tasks,55 suggesting an attention deficit associated with not just the amblyopic but also the fellow eye, that affects both spatial and temporal components of visual attention. On the other hand, studies using attentive cueing paradigms in humans56,57 and primates58 have reported that attentional modulation is comparable to a normal visual system. Thus, the extent of any attentional deficit in amblyopia remains unclear. This may be due to previous studies employing monocular presentation of visual stimuli. Presumably, if attentional allocation is abnormal between the eyes, it would be most evident under dichoptic viewing conditions. 
In normal vision, attention can be allocated independently to each eye and modulate binocular combination of monocular information. Monocular cues presented under dichoptic viewing conditions can attract visual attention59,60 and can affect perceptual dominance in binocular rivalry paradigms, even though they cannot be discriminated from binocular cues.6164 Therefore, it is conceivable that a biased allocation of attention between the eyes may contribute to interocular suppression and the loss of binocular vision in amblyopia. 
We used a dichoptic multiple-object tracking task to directly address the question of whether interocular suppression in amblyopia involves an attentional bias in favor of the fellow eye.50 By presenting target elements to only one eye and distractors to the other eye, we determined whether attention was biased toward one eye or allocated equally across both eyes. In Experiment 1, we tested whether dichoptic viewing affected multiple-object tracking in binocularly normal controls. In Experiment 2, we measured dichoptic multiple-object tracking performance in controls and participants with anisometropic and/or strabismic amblyopia. We considered the two amblyopia groups separately because a previous study of attentional modulation within the visual cortex only involved strabismic amblyopia.50 Interocular contrast balancing32,6570 and an enumeration control task were used to ensure that the stimulus elements presented to each eye were continuously visible in the amblyopia group. If a bias in the interocular balance of attention does play a role in suppression of the amblyopic eye, we would expect participants with amblyopia to have worse multiple-object tracking task performance when the target dots are presented to the less attended (amblyopic) eye versus the more attended (fellow) eye. We hypothesized that if such an attentional imbalance was a fundamental component of amblyopia, it would still be present after interocular contrast balancing was used to ensure that dots were equally visible to each eye. 
Methods
Participants
Experiment 1 involved 12 participants with normal vision (8 female; mean age 25 ± 2.8 years), and Experiment 2 involved 17 participants with amblyopia (9 anisometropic amblyopia, 8 strabismic/mixed amblyopia, mean age 37 ± 13.5 years) as well as 15 controls with normal vision (14 female; mean age 23 ± 1.5 years; only 1 of whom also participated in Experiment 1). Participants with normal vision had best-corrected visual acuity better than 0.1 logMAR (20/25), with no greater than 1 logMAR line difference between the eyes, and no history of binocular vision disorders. Inclusion in the amblyopia group required the following: (1) at least a 2 logMAR line difference in best-corrected visual acuity between the eyes (all participants had AE acuity worse than 0.2 logMAR with exception of A12, who had received successful treatment), (2) either anisometropia (>1 diopter difference in spherical equivalent between the eyes or >1.5 diopters of cylinder in one eye) and/or strabismus (including history of strabismus surgery), (3) normal ocular and general health. All participants, except for author AC, were naïve to the experimental hypothesis and were reimbursed with $15 for their time. Participants provided written informed consent to take part in the study, and the study protocol was approved by the institutional ethics committee, in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. 
Apparatus
Stimuli were presented on an ASUS 27” VG278 3D monitor (1280 × 720 resolution, 120-Hz refresh rate; Taipei, Taiwan), which was synchronized to the alternation rate of a pair of NVIDIA 3D VISION LCD shutter glasses (Santa Clara, CA, USA). Participants viewed the screen (subtending 48° × 27°) through the shutter glasses at a distance of 67 cm. Stimuli were presented using MATLAB (The MathWorks, Natick, MA, USA) and Psychtoolbox.71,72 
Procedure
All participants were screened at the School of Optometry and Vision Science, University of Waterloo by author AC, who assessed visual acuity (electronic Early Treatment Diabetic Retinopathy Study chart), binocular status (distance and near cover test), and stereoacuity (Randot Stereotest; Stereo Optical Co. Inc., Chicago, IL, USA). Refraction was conducted if an eye exam had not been completed within the last 2 years. Participants wore their optimal refractive correction either through their habitual corrective lenses or a trial frame. Clinical details for individuals with amblyopia are summarized in the Table. Sensory eye dominance was determined for all participants using an established dichoptic motion coherence paradigm, assigning the eye with a lower dichoptic motion coherence threshold as the dominant eye.65,66 This dichoptic global motion task was then used to identify the balance point contrast required for normal binocular combination. Each participant viewed the experimental stimuli at 100% contrast with their nondominant or amblyopic eye, and at their balance point contrast for the dominant or fellow eye. Any ocular misalignments were accounted for subjectively with the alignment of a central Nonius cross prior to the start of each task. 
Table
 
Clinical Details for Amblyopic Participants
Table
 
Clinical Details for Amblyopic Participants
Participants performed a multiple-object tracking task (similar to that employed by Giaschi et al.73 and illustrated in Figs. 1A and 1B) that involved tracking four target dots among six identical distractor dots (Experiment 1) or tracking three target dots among seven distractor dots (Experiment 2). This change was made because participants with amblyopia had difficulty tracking four dots, so we opted for a task that allowed for near normal overall performance. Previous work has noted this tracking deficit, whereby participants with amblyopia demonstrated 75% accuracy tracking a mean of four dots with the fellow eye and 3.7 dots with the amblyopic eye, as compared with 5.16 dots in control participants.52 At the start of each trial, 10 stationary dots (1° in diameter, presented within a Gaussian envelope) were presented and the target dots were highlighted in green for 2 seconds. The dots then moved at 10°/s within a 14° × 14° field along random but nonoverlapping trajectories. Participants tracked the dots for 5 seconds while fixating a central cross. Using a partial report procedure, participants indicated whether a highlighted dot was a target dot at the end of each trial. Audio feedback was given after each trial. 
Figure 1
 
(A) The multiple-object tracking task, consisting of four (Experiment 1) or three (Experiment 2) target dots to be tracked (highlighted in green) among distractor dots. (B) The two-alterative force choice (2-AFC) partial report screen. Participants reported whether the highlighted dot was a target dot. (C) The monocular awareness task. Participants reported the eye that was presented with the moving dot. All dots were presented within a Gaussian envelope.
Figure 1
 
(A) The multiple-object tracking task, consisting of four (Experiment 1) or three (Experiment 2) target dots to be tracked (highlighted in green) among distractor dots. (B) The two-alterative force choice (2-AFC) partial report screen. Participants reported whether the highlighted dot was a target dot. (C) The monocular awareness task. Participants reported the eye that was presented with the moving dot. All dots were presented within a Gaussian envelope.
In Experiment 1, a monocular awareness task (illustrated in Fig. 1C) was used to assess whether participants were aware of which eye a stimulus of interest was being presented to. In a 14° × 14° field, a single dot moving at 10°/s was presented to only one eye among four static dots displayed to the other eye. Participants were asked to identify which eye was viewing the moving dot using a key press. No feedback was provided, and participants were monitored to ensure they were viewing the display with both eyes open. The moving dot was presented to either eye with equal probability, and each participant completed 48 trials. 
In Experiment 2, an enumeration task was also performed at each participants' contrast balance point to ensure that participants with amblyopia were not suppressing the dots presented to the amblyopic eye. Dot parameters were the same as the multiple-object tracking task (dots moving at 10°/s in a 14° × 14° field), except the target dots were presented in red and remained red throughout the entire 5-second viewing period. Participants were asked to report the number of red dots (either 3 or 5) that were present with a key press on a number pad. Target dots were split between the dominant and nondominant eyes in either a 2:1 or 3:2 ratio, or all targets and distractors were presented to both eyes as catch trials. Splitting the target dots between the eyes allowed us to determine by numeric report whether amblyopic eye dots were being suppressed. Participants also provided a subjective yes/no report of whether any of the dots disappeared during the trial. This provided an indirect measure of transient suppression. Participants performed 60 trials in this self-paced task. 
The multiple-object tracking and enumeration tasks were performed across three conditions in the following randomized order: (1) binocular viewing, (2) dichoptic viewing with target dots presented to the dominant eye (DE condition) and distractor dots to the other eye, and (3) dichoptic viewing with target dots presented to the nondominant eye (NDE condition) and distractor dots to the other eye. In both experiments, participants were given 12 practice trials prior to completing 120 test trials, with an opportunity to take a break every 60 trials. 
Statistical Methods
The one-sample Kolmogorov-Smirnov test was used to test for normality. Data that were not normally distributed were analyzed with nonparametric statistics. For the multiple-object tracking task in Experiment 1, ANOVA was used to assess the effect of viewing condition. Pairwise t-tests were used for post-hoc analysis. Multiple comparison corrections were not applied due to the limited sample size. For the monocular awareness task in Experiment 1, a one-sample t-test was used to test whether task performance differed from chance. 
For Experiment 2, three analyses were conducted on the multiple-object tracking accuracy data. First, the three different viewing conditions were compared within each group using Wilcoxon signed ranks tests. Second, the binocular condition results were compared between the three groups to assess whether any group exhibited a general multiple-object tracking deficit. Because the binocular condition accuracy data were normally distributed, a univariate ANOVA with a factor of group (control versus anisometropia versus strabismic) was conducted. Third, an interocular asymmetry score was computed for each group by subtracting NDE from DE accuracy to enable between-group comparisons in the allocation of attention between the two eyes. Asymmetry scores were analyzed using the independent samples Kruskal-Wallis test and post-hoc testing was conducted using Mann-Whitney U tests. Asymmetry scores were also calculated for the enumeration task and analyzed in the same way. Spearman's rho correlation coefficients were used to investigate the association between NDE enumeration task accuracy and multiple-object tracking asymmetry scores. 
Results
Experiment 1
Multiple-object tracking performance (percent accuracy, mean ± SE) was 86 ± 3% for binocular viewing, 79 ± 2% for the DE condition, and 81 ± 3% for the NDE condition (see Fig. 2). There was a significant main effect of viewing condition (F2,11 = 4.2, P = 0.048). Pairwise t-tests revealed a significant difference between performance under binocular viewing compared with the DE condition (t11 = −2.96, P = 0.013) and no significant difference between binocular viewing compared with the NDE condition (t11 = −2.00, P = 0.07). Performance between the DE and NDE conditions did not differ significantly (t11 = −0.776, P = 0.45). In the conscious monocular awareness task, participants were unable to consciously report which eye the signal was being presented to and did not perform significantly better than chance (t11 = 1.03, P = 0.33). 
Figure 2
 
Mean percent accuracy on the multiple-object tracking task in Experiment 1. Participants with normal vision showed slightly improved tracking performance under binocular viewing conditions than dichoptic viewing conditions. Error bars denote ±1 SEM.
Figure 2
 
Mean percent accuracy on the multiple-object tracking task in Experiment 1. Participants with normal vision showed slightly improved tracking performance under binocular viewing conditions than dichoptic viewing conditions. Error bars denote ±1 SEM.
These results demonstrate that in participants with normal vision, dichoptic viewing with target dots presented to one eye, and distractor dots presented to the other eye did not benefit multiple-object tracking. In fact, dichoptic presentation slightly impaired task performance relative to binocular presentation. Furthermore, participants were not consciously aware of which eye was receiving target information and were unable to use this information to their advantage. As a whole, the results indicate that attention was allocated equally between the two eyes in participants with normal vision, even when task performance would have benefited from preferential allocation of attention to the eye that saw the target dots. 
Experiment 2
Multiple-object tracking performance (Fig. 3A) was quantified as percent accuracy (mean ± SE) for the normal group (DE 83 ± 3%, NDE 80 ± 4%, binocular 92 ± 2%), anisometropia group (DE 81 ± 6%, NDE 86 ± 3%, binocular 78 ± 6%), and strabismic/mixed group (DE 88 ± 1%, NDE 65 ± 5%, binocular 84 ± 5%). Within each group, the multiple-object tracking accuracy scores for each viewing condition were compared with determine whether there was an effect of viewing condition. For the normal vision group, no significant difference was found between the DE and NDE conditions (W = 24.5; P = 0.3), but both differed significantly from the binocular condition (DE versus binocular: W = 117.5, P = 0.001; NDE versus binocular: W = 117.5, P = 0.001). In the anisometropic amblyopia group, none of the pairwise comparisons were significant (DE versus NDE: W = 25, P > 0.05; DE versus binocular: W = 8.5, P > 0.05; NDE versus binocular: W = 17, P > 0.05). In the strabismic/mixed amblyopia group, there was no significant difference between the DE and binocular conditions (W = 7.5, P > 0.05). However, the NDE condition was worse than both the DE (W = 3, P = 0.04) and binocular (W = 26, P = 0.04) conditions. Each participant's multiple-object tracking task accuracy under the two dichoptic conditions can be seen in Figure 4
Figure 3
 
(A) Mean percent accuracy on the multiple-object tracking task for each group in Experiment 2. (B) Multiple-object tracking asymmetry scores calculated from the difference between FE and AE accuracy in the amblyopia groups and DE and NDE accuracy in controls). Interocular asymmetry scores were significantly higher in the strabismic/mixed amblyopia as compared with the anisometropic amblyopia or control groups. Error bars denote ±1 SEM. FE, fellow eye; AE, amblyopic eye.
Figure 3
 
(A) Mean percent accuracy on the multiple-object tracking task for each group in Experiment 2. (B) Multiple-object tracking asymmetry scores calculated from the difference between FE and AE accuracy in the amblyopia groups and DE and NDE accuracy in controls). Interocular asymmetry scores were significantly higher in the strabismic/mixed amblyopia as compared with the anisometropic amblyopia or control groups. Error bars denote ±1 SEM. FE, fellow eye; AE, amblyopic eye.
Figure 4
 
Individual participant multiple-object tracking task performance accuracy under each dichoptic condition for each group ([A] controls with normal vision; [B] anisometropic amblyopia; [C] strabismic/mixed amblyopia). Participants with anisometropic amblyopia and binocularly normal had similar patterns of attentional allocation. Participants with strabismic/mixed amblyopia showed a significant deficit when target dots were presented to the amblyopic eye. Data points from the same participant across the two separate viewing conditions are connected with a line for ease of interpretation. The dashed line in (C) denotes participant A16, who reported intermittent diplopia sporadically during the multiple-object tracking task.
Figure 4
 
Individual participant multiple-object tracking task performance accuracy under each dichoptic condition for each group ([A] controls with normal vision; [B] anisometropic amblyopia; [C] strabismic/mixed amblyopia). Participants with anisometropic amblyopia and binocularly normal had similar patterns of attentional allocation. Participants with strabismic/mixed amblyopia showed a significant deficit when target dots were presented to the amblyopic eye. Data points from the same participant across the two separate viewing conditions are connected with a line for ease of interpretation. The dashed line in (C) denotes participant A16, who reported intermittent diplopia sporadically during the multiple-object tracking task.
Binocular multiple-object tracking accuracy was compared between groups in a separate analysis to evaluate whether the strabismic/mixed group had a general multiple-object tracking deficit compared with the other groups. There was a significant main effect of group (F2,29 = 3.89, P = 0.03). Pairwise t-tests showed that performance in the anisometropic amblyopia group was significantly worse than controls (t9.49 = −2.34, P = 0.043). There was no significant difference between the strabismic/mixed amblyopia and control groups (t8.86 = −1.59, P = 0.15), or between the anisometropic and strabismic/mixed amblyopia groups (t15 = −0.77, P = 0.45). 
Asymmetry scores (normal 3 ± 2%; anisometropia −5 ± 4%; strabismic/mixed 24 ± 6%) varied significantly across groups as seen in Figure 3B (H2 = 8.4, P = 0.02). Pairwise comparisons revealed a significant difference between the normal and strabismic/mixed groups (U = 24, P = 0.02), and between the anisometropia and strabismic/mixed groups (U = 60.5, P = 0.02). Asymmetry scores between the normal and anisometropia groups did not significantly differ (U = 92, P > 0.05). 
To explore whether the differences in multiple-object tracking accuracy could be explained by suppression, we analyzed the contrast balance points for our participants with amblyopia. There was no significant difference in contrast balance points between the anisometropic amblyopia and strabismic/mixed amblyopia groups (t12.9 = −0.11, P = 0.91). Similarly, when the enumeration task was performed at each participants' contrast balance point, participants exhibited the following high accuracy (mean ± SE) across all three groups: anisometropia DE 97 ± 2%, NDE 96 ± 2%, and binocular 99 ± 1%; strabismic/mixed DE 89 ± 3%, NDE 86 ± 3%, and binocular 87 ± 12%; and normal DE 99 ± 0%, NDE 99 ± 1%, and binocular 99 ± 1%. Enumeration performance differed across the groups for both DE and NDE conditions (H2 = 10.5, P = 0.005 and H2 = 13, P = 0.001, respectively), but not for the binocular condition (H2 = 0.3, P = 0.86). Pairwise comparisons showed a significant difference between the normal and strabismic/mixed groups (DE: U = 19, P = 0.007; NDE: U = 11.5, P = 0.001) and between the anisometropia and strabismic/mixed groups (DE: U = 17, P = 0.05; NDE U = 14, P = 0.03). No significant difference between the anisometropia and normal groups were found in the dichoptic conditions (DE: U = 53, P > 0.05; NDE: U = 42, P > 0.05). Analysis of the enumeration task interocular asymmetry scores revealed that the groups did not differ in interocular difference in enumeration task performance (H2 = 0.7, P = 0.72). 
Additionally, the multiple-object tracking task asymmetry scores did not correlate significantly with enumeration performance in the NDE condition (ρ16 = −0.395, P = 0.12), contrast balance point (ρ16 = −0.08, P = 0.77), or visual acuity difference between the eyes (ρ16 = 0.131, P = 0.62). 
Participant A16 subjectively reported intermittent diplopia during the tracking task. When this participant was removed from the analysis, the asymmetry scores still varied significantly across groups (H2 = 7.06, P = 0.02) and a significant difference between the binocularly normal and strabismic/mixed groups persisted (although the P value was very close to 0.05: U = 26.5, P = 0.047) as well as between the anisometropia and strabismic/mixed groups (U = 51.5, P = 0.03). 
The results show that participants with anisometropic amblyopia had an equal distribution of attention between the two eyes, similar to those with normal vision. However, those with strabismic amblyopia displayed a significant deficit when targets were presented to their amblyopic eye and distractors their fellow eye, despite the dots being adequately visible to both eyes. This asymmetry cannot be attributed to suppression of dots presented to the amblyopic eye, as seen by the poor correlation between enumeration performance in the NDE condition and the asymmetry scores seen on the multiple-object tracking task. Similarly, a general motion tracking deficit cannot explain this effect, as task performance in the strabismic/mixed group under binocular viewing conditions did not differ from controls. Instead, these results suggest an imbalance in the allocation of attention to each eye for the multiple-object tracking task. 
Discussion
The aims of this study were to determine whether interocular attention is biased in visually normal participants (Experiment 1), and whether interocular attention is biased in favor of the fellow eye in anisometropic and strabismic/mixed amblyopia (Experiment 2) when both eyes are viewing. Experiment 1 revealed that the normal visual system has an equal distribution of attention between the two eyes when performing the multiple-object tracking task, and that dichoptic presentation of target dots to only one eye did not benefit task performance. In fact, splitting targets and distractors between the two eyes impaired performance, as we found an advantage of binocular over dichoptic stimulus presentation for multiple-object tracking accuracy. This is the opposite of what we would expect if attention could be biased in favor of the eye viewing only target elements during dichoptic viewing. Therefore, despite previous evidence that attentive processing of monocular information can be used in an advantageous manner to expedite stimulus detection in dichoptic tasks,59,60 we found that it does not benefit performance within a motion tracking paradigm. 
Experiment 2 revealed an equal allocation of attention between the two eyes in anisometropic amblyopia and an attentional bias in favor of the fellow eye in strabismic amblyopia. These findings are consistent with relatively reduced attentional modulation for amblyopic eye viewing in strabismic amblyopia.50 Our results are also broadly consistent with previous neuroimaging studies that have reported abnormalities within the motion processing and attentional networks that contribute to multiple-object tracking task performance in amblyopia.7482 However, other studies have observed normal performance on attentional tasks in amblyopia. For example, Roberts and colleagues57 found normal accuracy and reaction times for a group of participants with amblyopia who performed involuntary and voluntary attentive cueing tasks under monocular viewing conditions. Furthermore, they found that the extent of attentional modulation did not correlate with amblyopia severity. However, when only considering the participants in their study who had at least a 2 logMAR line difference in acuity, those who remain (7/19 participants) predominantly had anisometropic amblyopia. The results of the current study are consistent with this previous study in finding no attentional deficit in anisometropic amblyopia. 
An inspection of Figure 4 reveals variability in the control and strabismic/mixed groups with some participants exhibiting a bias toward the fellow/dominant eye and others showing no bias. This indicates that an attentional bias was not present in all participants with strabismic amblyopia and that a bias can also exist in controls. A study with a larger sample size is required understand this variability and to enable stronger conclusions to be drawn regarding the allocation of interocular attention in strabismic/mixed amblyopia. 
Unlike controls, participants with amblyopia did not exhibit an advantage of binocular over dichoptic presentation. This effect may be related to previous work demonstrating that binocular summation may be impaired in amblyopia.5,8385 However, the reason for a multiple-object tracking task advantage for binocular versus dichoptic presentation of the multiple-object tracking task remains to be explained. Similarly, the reason for an interocular attention asymmetry for the strabismic/mixed group and not the anisometropic group is unclear. Stronger dichoptic masking in strabismic relative to anisometropic amblyopia has been reported.86 However, other studies have not observed differences in interocular suppression strength between strabismic/mixed and anisometropic amblyopia.25,28 In agreement with these latter studies, we did not find a difference in interocular suppression strength between the two amblyopia groups in terms of balance point contrast. In addition, once interocular suppression was neutralized with an appropriate interocular contrast balance, interocular asymmetries in enumeration task performance did not differ between groups, although the strabismic/mixed group did exhibit worse overall performance on this task than the other groups. If we assume that an interocular bias in attention contributes to interocular suppression in amblyopia,50 then our measurements reveal a residual attentional bias in strabismic/mixed amblyopia that is maintained when suppression is minimized. 
To rule out the potential confound of a general motion tracking deficit in amblyopia, we examined multiple-object tracking performance under binocular viewing conditions to provide an index of multiple-object tracking accuracy without any interocular manipulations. Both amblyopia groups exhibited numerically poorer multiple-object tracking accuracy than controls in agreement with previous studies of monocular multiple-object tracking in amblyopia52,53,76; however, the difference from controls only reached significance for the anisometropia group, perhaps due to limited sample size. Critically, the two amblyopia groups did not differ significantly from one another, confirming that the interocular attentional bias in the strabismic/mixed group could not be explained by a general tracking deficit. Both amblyopia groups had greater mean ages than the control group; however, the mean age of the two amblyopia groups did not differ. As all participants were adults with mature visual systems and healthy eyes, we do not anticipate age to have affected our results. 
It has been proposed that the spatial attention deficits may be involved in the multiple-object tracking impairments in amblyopia,76 whereby the spatial resolution of attention is coarser in the amblyopic eye.56 This may cause crowding of dots presented to an amblyopic eye.8789 Although we cannot eliminate crowding as a confounding factor in our study, contour interactions were minimized by the use of Gaussian blur on dot edges. It is also important to note that our target dots were generally perceived to be separate dots in the enumeration task. Although the strabismic/mixed group displayed poorer overall enumeration accuracy in both dichoptic conditions, no interocular differences were present and binocular performance was similar to the anisometropic and visually normal groups. 
Overall, our results provide new evidence that an interocular imbalance in attention occurs in strabismic/mixed amblyopia. Interocular attention may be important to consider within the rapidly developing field of binocular amblyopia treatments. 
Acknowledgments
Supported by a NSERC CGS-M Grant (AC) and a NSERC Discovery Grant (BT; Ottawa, ON, Canada). 
Disclosure: A. Chow, None; D. Giaschi, None; B. Thompson, None 
References
Wallace DK, Repka MX, Lee KA, et al. Amblyopia Preferred Practice Pattern®. Ophthalmology. 2018; 125: P105–P142.
Holmes JM, Clarke MP. Amblyopia. Lancet. 2006; 367: 1343–1351.
Mullen KT, Sankeralli MJ, Hess RF. Color and luminance vision in human amblyopia: Shifts in isoluminance, contrast sensitivity losses, and positional deficits. Vision Res. 1996; 36: 645–653.
Pardhan S, Gilchrist J. Binocular contrast summation and inhibition in amblyopia. The influence of the interocular difference on binocular contrast sensitivity. Doc Ophthalmol. 1992; 82: 239–248.
Baker DH, Meese TS, Hess RF. Contrast masking in strabismic amblyopia: attenuation, noise, interocular suppression and binocular summation. Vision Res. 2008; 48: 1625–1640.
Hess RF, Howell ER. The threshold contrast sensitivity function in strabismic amblyopia: evidence for a two type classification. Vision Res. 1977; 17: 1049–1055.
Levi DM, Harwerth RS. Spatio-temporal interactions in anisometropic and strabismic amblyopia. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1977; 16: 90–95.
Giaschi DE, Regan D, Kraft SP, Hong XH. Defective processing of motion-defined form in the fellow eye of patients with unilateral amblyopia. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1992; 33: 2483–2489.
Hayward J, Truong G, Partanen M, Giaschi DE. Effects of speed, age, and amblyopia on the perception of motion-defined form. Vision Res. 2011; 51: 2216–2223.
Wang J, Ho CS, Giaschi DE. Deficient motion-defined and texture-defined figure-ground segregation in amblyopic children. J Pediatr Ophthalmol Strabismus. 2007; 44: 363–371.
Husk JS, Hess RF. Global processing of orientation in amblyopia. Vision Res. 2013; 82: 22–30.
Hamm LM, Black J, Dai S, Thompson B. Global processing in amblyopia: a review. Front Psychol. 2014; 5: 1–21.
Atkinson J. The Davida Teller Award Lecture, 2016: Visual Brain Development: a review of “Dorsal Stream Vulnerability”-motion, mathematics, amblyopia, actions, and attention. J Vis. 2017; 17 (3): 26.
Ho CS, Giaschi DE, Boden C, Dougherty R, Cline R, Lyons C. Deficient motion perception in the fellow eye of amblyopic children. Vision Res. 2005; 45: 1615–1627.
Aaen-Stockdale C, Hess RF. The amblyopic deficit for global motion is spatial scale invariant. Vision Res. 2008; 48: 1965–1971.
Simmers AJJ, Ledgeway T, Mansouri B, Hutchinson CV, Hess RFF. The extent of the dorsal extra-striate deficit in amblyopia. Vision Res. 2006; 46: 2571–2580.
Mansouri B, Hess RF. The global processing deficit in amblyopia involves noise segregation. Vision Res. 2006; 46: 4104–4117.
Meier K, Sum B, Giaschi DE. Global motion perception in children with amblyopia as a function of spatial and temporal stimulus parameters. Vision Res. 2016; 127: 18–27.
Meier K, Giaschi DE. Unilateral amblyopia affects two eyes: fellow eye deficits in amblyopia. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2017; 58: 1779–1800.
Aaen-Stockdale C, Ledgeway T, Hess RF. Second-order optic flow deficits in amblyopia. Invest Opthalmology Vis Sci. 2007; 48: 5532–5538.
Simmers AJ, Ledgeway T, Hess RF, McGraw PV. Deficits to global motion processing in human amblyopia. Vision Res. 2003; 43: 729–738.
Maehara G, Thompson B, Mansouri B, Farivar R, Hess RF. The perceptual consequences of interocular suppression in amblyopia. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2011; 52: 9011–9017.
Mansouri B, Thompson B, Hess RF. Measurement of suprathreshold binocular interactions in amblyopia. Vision Res. 2008; 48: 2775–2784.
Farivar R, Thompson B, Mansouri B, Hess RF. Interocular suppression in strabismic amblyopia results in an attenuated and delayed hemodynamic response function in early visual cortex. J Vis. 2011; 11 (14): 16.
Li J, Thompson B, Lam CSYY, et al. The role of suppression in amblyopia. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2011; 52: 4169–4176.
Sireteanu R, Fronius M. Naso-temporal asymmetries in human amblyopia: consequence of long-term interocular suppression. Vision Res. 1981; 21: 1055–1063.
Hamm L, Chen Z, Li J, et al. Interocular suppression in children with deprivation amblyopia. Vision Res. 2017; 133: 112–120.
Babu RJ, Clavagnier SR, Bobier W, Thompson B, Hess RF. The regional extent of suppression: strabismics versus nonstrabismics. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2013; 54: 6585–6593.
Babu RJ, Clavagnier S, Bobier WR, Thompson B, Hess RF. Regional extent of peripheral suppression in amblyopia. Invest Opthalmology Vis Sci. 2017; 58: 2329–2340.
Hess RF, Thompson B. Amblyopia and the binocular approach to its therapy. Vision Res. 2015; 114: 4–16.
Birch EE, Morale SE, Jost RM, et al. Assessing suppression in amblyopic children with a dichoptic eye chart. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2016; 57: 5649–5654.
Narasimhan S, Harrison ER, Giaschi DE. Quantitative measurement of interocular suppression in children with amblyopia. Vision Res. 2012; 66: 1–10.
Hess RF, Babu RJ, Clavagnier S, Black J, Bobier W, Thompson B. The iPod binocular home-based treatment for amblyopia in adults: Efficacy and compliance. Clin Exp Optom. 2014; 97: 389–398.
Spiegel DP, Li J, Hess RF, et al. Transcranial direct current stimulation enhances recovery of stereopsis in adults with amblyopia. Neurotherapeutics. 2013; 10: 831–839.
Li J, Thompson B, Deng D, Chan LYLL, Yu M, Hess RF. Dichoptic training enables the adult amblyopic brain to learn. Curr Biol. 2013; 23: R308–R309.
Hess RF, Mansouri B, Thompson B. A new binocular approach to the treatment of amblyopia in adults well beyond the critical period of visual development. Restor Neurol Neurosci. 2010; 28: 793–802.
Hess RF, Thompson B, Black JM, et al. An iPod treatment of amblyopia: an updated binocular approach. Optometry. 2012; 83: 87–94.
Li SL, Reynaud A, Hess RF, et al. Dichoptic movie viewing treats childhood amblyopia. J AAPOS. 2015; 19: 401–405.
Vedamurthy I, Knill DC, Huang SJ, et al. Recovering stereo vision by squashing virtual bugs in a virtual reality environment. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci. 2016; 371: 20150264.
Vedamurthy I, Nahum M, Huang SJ, et al. A dichoptic custom-made action video game as a treatment for adult amblyopia. Vision Res. 2015; 114: 173–187.
Gao TY, Guo CX, Babu RJ, et al. Effectiveness of a binocular video game vs placebo video game for improving visual functions in older children, teenagers, and adults with amblyopia. JAMA Ophthalmol. 2018; 136: 172–181.
Holmes JM, Manh VM, Lazar EL, et al. Effect of a binocular iPad game vs part-time patching in children aged 5 to 12 years with amblyopia. JAMA Ophthalmol. 2016; 134: 1391–1400.
Kelly KR, Jost RM, Dao L, Beauchamp CL, Leffler JN, Birch EE. Binocular iPad game vs patching for treatment of amblyopia in children. JAMA Ophthalmol. 2016; 134: 1402–1408.
Bi H, Zhang B, Tao X, et al. Neuronal responses in visual area V2 (V2) of macaque monkeys with strabismic amblyopia. Cereb Cortex. 2011; 21: 2033–2045.
Sengpiel F, Jirmann K-U, Vorobyov V, Eysel UT. Strabismic suppression is mediated by inhibitory interactions in the primary visual cortex. Cereb Cortex. 2005; 16: 1750–1758.
Sengpiel F, Blakemore C. The neural basis of suppression and amblyopia in strabismus. Eye. 1996; 10: 250–258.
Hallum LE, Shooner C, Kumbhani RD, et al. Altered balance of receptive field excitation and suppression in visual cortex of amblyopic macaque monkeys. J Neurosci. 2017; 37: 8216–8226.
Tao X, Zhang B, Shen G, et al. Early monocular defocus disrupts the normal development of receptive-field structure in V2 neurons of macaque monkeys. J Neurosci. 2014; 34: 13840–13854.
Shooner C, Hallum LE, Kumbhani RD, et al. Asymmetric dichoptic masking in visual cortex of amblyopic macaque monkeys. J Neurosci. 2017; 37: 8734–8741.
Hou C, Kim Y, Lai XJ, Verghese P. Degraded attentional modulation of cortical neural populations in strabismic amblyopia. J Vis. 2016; 16 (3): 16.
Zhou A, Jiang Y, Chen J, et al. Neural mechanisms of selective attention in children with amblyopia. PLoS One. 2015; 10: 1–17.
Ho CS, Paul PS, Asirvatham A, Cavanagh P, Cline R, Giaschi DE. Abnormal spatial selection and tracking in children with amblyopia. Vision Res. 2006; 46: 3274–3283.
Tripathy SP, Levi DM. On the effective number of tracked trajectories in amblyopic human vision. 2008; 8: 1–22.
Tsirlin I, Colpa L, Goltz HC, Wong AMF. Visual search deficits in amblyopia. J Vis. 2018; 18 (4): 17.
Popple A V, Levi DM. The attentional blink in amblyopia. J Vis. 2008; 8 (13): 12.
Sharma V, Levi DM, Klein SA. Undercounting features and missing features: evidence for a high-level deficit in strabismic amblyopia. Nat Neurosci. 2000; 3: 496–501.
Roberts M, Cymerman R, Smith RT, Kiorpes L, Carrasco M. Covert spatial attention is functionally intact in amblyopic human adults. J Vis. 2016; 16 (15): 30.
Pham A, Carrasco M, Kiorpes L. Endogenous attention improves perception in amblyopic macaques. J Vis. 2018; 18 (3): 11.
Zhaoping L. Attention capture by eye of origin singletons even without awareness–a hallmark of a bottom-up saliency map in the primary visual cortex. J Vis. 2008; 8 (5): 1
Zhaoping L. Ocularity feature contrast attracts attention exogenously. Vision. 2018; 2: 12.
Paffen CLE, Van der Stigchel S. Shifting spatial attention makes you flip: exogenous visual attention triggers perceptual alternations during binocular rivalry. Atten Percept Psychophys. 2010; 72: 1237–1243.
Ooi TL, He ZJ. Binocular rivalry and visual awareness: the role of attention. Perception. 1999; 28: 551–574.
Paffen CLE, Alais D. Attentional modulation of binocular rivalry. Front Hum Neurosci. 2011; 5: 105.
Zhang P, Jiang Y, He S. Voluntary attention modulates processing of eye-specific visual information. Psychol Sci. 2012; 23: 254–260.
Li J, Lam CSYY, Yu M, et al. Quantifying sensory eye dominance in the normal visual system: a new technique and insights into variation across traditional tests. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2010; 51: 6875–6881.
Li J, Hess RF, Chan LYLL, et al. Quantitative measurement of interocular suppression in anisometropic amblyopia: a case-control study. Ophthalmology. 2013; 120: 1672–1680.
Black JM, Thompson B, Maehara G, Hess RF. A compact clinical instrument for quantifying suppression. Optom Vis Sci. 2011; 88: 334–343.
Baker DH, Meese TS, Mansouri B, Hess RF. Binocular summation of contrast remains intact in strabismic amblyopia. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2007; 48: 5332–5338.
Kwon M, Wiecek E, Dakin SC, Bex PJ. Spatial-frequency dependent binocular imbalance in amblyopia. Sci Rep. 2015; 5: 17181.
Ding J, Levi DM. Rebalancing binocular vision in amblyopia. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt. 2014; 34: 199–213.
Brainard DH. The Psychophysics Toolbox. Spat Vis. 1997; 10: 433–436.
Kleiner M, Brainard D, Pelli D, Ingling A, Murray R, Broussard C. What's new in Psychtoolbox-3. Perception. 2007; 36: 1.
Giaschi DE, Chapman C, Meier K, Narasimhan S, Regan D. The effect of occlusion therapy on motion perception deficits in amblyopia. Vision Res. 2015; 114: 122–134.
Bonhomme GR, Liu GT, Miki A, et al. Decreased cortical activation in response to a motion stimulus in anisometropic amblyopic eyes using functional magnetic resonance imaging. J AAPOS. 2006; 10: 540–546.
Ho CS, Giaschi DE. Low- and high-level motion perception deficits in anisometropic and strabismic amblyopia: evidence from fMRI. Vision Res. 2009; 49: 2891–2901.
Secen J, Culham J, Ho C, Giaschi DE. Neural correlates of the multiple-object tracking deficit in amblyopia. Vision Res. 2011; 51: 2517–2527.
Wang H, Crewther SG, Liang M, et al. Impaired activation of visual attention network for motion salience is accompanied by reduced functional connectivity between frontal eye fields and visual cortex in strabismic amblyopia. Front Hum Neurosci. 2017; 11: 1–13.
Culham JC, Brandt SA, Cavanagh P, Kanwisher NG, Dale AM, Tootell RB. Cortical fMRI activation produced by attentive tracking of moving targets. J Neurophysiol. 1998; 80: 2657–2670.
Culham JC, Cavanagh P, Kanwisher NG. Attention response functions: characterizing brain areas using fMRI activation during parametric variations of attentional load. Neuron. 2001; 32: 737–745.
Howe PD, Horowitz TS, Morocz IA, Wolfe J, Livingstone MS. Using fMRI to distinguish components of the multiple object tracking task. J Vis. 2009; 9 (4): 10.
Jovicich J, Peters RJ, Koch C, Braun J, Chang L, Ernst T. Brain areas specific for attentional load in a motion-tracking task. J Cogn Neurosci. 2001; 13: 1048–1058.
Thompson B, Villeneuve MY, Casanova C, Hess RF. Abnormal cortical processing of pattern motion in amblyopia: evidence from fMRI. Neuroimage. 2012; 60: 1307–1315.
Thompson B, Richard A, Churan J, Hess RF, Aaen-Stockdale C, Pack CC. Impaired spatial and binocular summation for motion direction discrimination in strabismic amblyopia. Vision Res. 2011; 51: 577–584.
Barrett BT, Panesar GK, Scally AJ, Pacey IE. Binocular summation and other forms of non-dominant eye contribution in individuals with strabismic amblyopia during habitual viewing. PLoS One. 2013; 8: e77871.
Huang C-B, Zhou J, Lu Z-L, Feng L, Zhou Y. Binocular combination in anisometropic amblyopia. J Vis. 2009; 9 (3): 17.
Harrad RA, Hess RF. Binocular integration of contrast information in amblyopia. Vision Res. 1992; 32: 2135–2150.
Flom MC, Weymouth FW, Kahneman D. Visual resolution and contour interaction. J Opt Soc Am. 1963; 53: 1026–1032.
Hariharan S, Levi DM, Klein SA. “Crowding” in normal and amblyopic vision assessed with Gaussian and Gabor C's. Vision Res. 2005; 45: 617–633.
Levi DM, Hariharan S, Klein SA. Suppressive and facilitatory spatial interactions in amblyopic vision. Vision Res. 2002; 42: 1379–1394.
Figure 1
 
(A) The multiple-object tracking task, consisting of four (Experiment 1) or three (Experiment 2) target dots to be tracked (highlighted in green) among distractor dots. (B) The two-alterative force choice (2-AFC) partial report screen. Participants reported whether the highlighted dot was a target dot. (C) The monocular awareness task. Participants reported the eye that was presented with the moving dot. All dots were presented within a Gaussian envelope.
Figure 1
 
(A) The multiple-object tracking task, consisting of four (Experiment 1) or three (Experiment 2) target dots to be tracked (highlighted in green) among distractor dots. (B) The two-alterative force choice (2-AFC) partial report screen. Participants reported whether the highlighted dot was a target dot. (C) The monocular awareness task. Participants reported the eye that was presented with the moving dot. All dots were presented within a Gaussian envelope.
Figure 2
 
Mean percent accuracy on the multiple-object tracking task in Experiment 1. Participants with normal vision showed slightly improved tracking performance under binocular viewing conditions than dichoptic viewing conditions. Error bars denote ±1 SEM.
Figure 2
 
Mean percent accuracy on the multiple-object tracking task in Experiment 1. Participants with normal vision showed slightly improved tracking performance under binocular viewing conditions than dichoptic viewing conditions. Error bars denote ±1 SEM.
Figure 3
 
(A) Mean percent accuracy on the multiple-object tracking task for each group in Experiment 2. (B) Multiple-object tracking asymmetry scores calculated from the difference between FE and AE accuracy in the amblyopia groups and DE and NDE accuracy in controls). Interocular asymmetry scores were significantly higher in the strabismic/mixed amblyopia as compared with the anisometropic amblyopia or control groups. Error bars denote ±1 SEM. FE, fellow eye; AE, amblyopic eye.
Figure 3
 
(A) Mean percent accuracy on the multiple-object tracking task for each group in Experiment 2. (B) Multiple-object tracking asymmetry scores calculated from the difference between FE and AE accuracy in the amblyopia groups and DE and NDE accuracy in controls). Interocular asymmetry scores were significantly higher in the strabismic/mixed amblyopia as compared with the anisometropic amblyopia or control groups. Error bars denote ±1 SEM. FE, fellow eye; AE, amblyopic eye.
Figure 4
 
Individual participant multiple-object tracking task performance accuracy under each dichoptic condition for each group ([A] controls with normal vision; [B] anisometropic amblyopia; [C] strabismic/mixed amblyopia). Participants with anisometropic amblyopia and binocularly normal had similar patterns of attentional allocation. Participants with strabismic/mixed amblyopia showed a significant deficit when target dots were presented to the amblyopic eye. Data points from the same participant across the two separate viewing conditions are connected with a line for ease of interpretation. The dashed line in (C) denotes participant A16, who reported intermittent diplopia sporadically during the multiple-object tracking task.
Figure 4
 
Individual participant multiple-object tracking task performance accuracy under each dichoptic condition for each group ([A] controls with normal vision; [B] anisometropic amblyopia; [C] strabismic/mixed amblyopia). Participants with anisometropic amblyopia and binocularly normal had similar patterns of attentional allocation. Participants with strabismic/mixed amblyopia showed a significant deficit when target dots were presented to the amblyopic eye. Data points from the same participant across the two separate viewing conditions are connected with a line for ease of interpretation. The dashed line in (C) denotes participant A16, who reported intermittent diplopia sporadically during the multiple-object tracking task.
Table
 
Clinical Details for Amblyopic Participants
Table
 
Clinical Details for Amblyopic Participants
×
×

This PDF is available to Subscribers Only

Sign in or purchase a subscription to access this content. ×

You must be signed into an individual account to use this feature.

×