Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science Cover Image for Volume 59, Issue 12
October 2018
Volume 59, Issue 12
Open Access
Visual Psychophysics and Physiological Optics  |   October 2018
On Sensory Eye Dominance Revealed by Binocular Integrative and Binocular Competitive Stimuli
Author Affiliations & Notes
  • Chao Han
    College of Optometry, The Ohio State University, Columbus, Ohio, United States
  • Zijiang J. He
    Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, University of Louisville, Louisville, Kentucky, United States
  • Teng Leng Ooi
    College of Optometry, The Ohio State University, Columbus, Ohio, United States
  • Correspondence: Teng Leng Ooi, College of Optometry, The Ohio State University, 338 West 10th Avenue, Columbus, OH 43210, USA; [email protected]
  • Zijiang J. He, Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, University of Louisville, #317 Life Sciences Building, Louisville, KY 40292-0001, USA; [email protected]
Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science October 2018, Vol.59, 5140-5148. doi:https://doi.org/10.1167/iovs.18-24342
  • Views
  • PDF
  • Share
  • Tools
    • Alerts
      ×
      This feature is available to authenticated users only.
      Sign In or Create an Account ×
    • Get Citation

      Chao Han, Zijiang J. He, Teng Leng Ooi; On Sensory Eye Dominance Revealed by Binocular Integrative and Binocular Competitive Stimuli. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 2018;59(12):5140-5148. https://doi.org/10.1167/iovs.18-24342.

      Download citation file:


      © ARVO (1962-2015); The Authors (2016-present)

      ×
  • Supplements
Abstract

Purpose: Two core processes underlie 3-D binocular vision. The first, a binocular combination/summation process, integrates similar feature signals from the two eye channels to form a binocular representation. The second, a binocular inhibitory process, suppresses interocular conflicting signals or falsely matched binocular representations to establish single vision. Having an intrinsic interocular imbalance within one or both processes can cause sensory eye dominance (SED), related to imbalances of combination (SEDcombo) and/or inhibition (SEDinhibition). While much has recently been revealed about SEDcombo and SEDinhibition, the relationship between them is still unknown.

Methods: We measured observers' foveal SEDcombo and SEDinhibition, respectively, with a pair of dichoptic horizontal sine wave gratings with different phases and binocular rivalry stimulus with vertical and horizontal gratings. We then measured horizontal and vertical monocular contrast thresholds using sinusoidal grating stimuli, and stereo thresholds using random-dot stereograms.

Results: There exists a strong correlation between SEDcombo and SEDinhibition. An observer's interocular difference in contrast threshold was not always consistent with his/her SEDcombo and SEDinhihition, suggesting a partial binocular origin for the underlying imbalances. We also found stereo thresholds significantly increased with the magnitudes of SEDcombo, as well as with the magnitude of SEDinhibition.

Conclusions: Our findings suggest a common origin for interocular imbalance in the two different binocular processes and that both types of sensory eye dominance are significant factors in impeding stereopsis.

Conceptually, the visual system relies on two functionally distinct binocular processes to achieve single three-dimensional (3D) vision from the two slightly disparate retinal images.1 The binocular combination/summation process constructs a 3D binocular representation by integrating signals of similar visual features from the right and left eye's channels. In contrast, the interocular inhibitory process suppresses signals of the dissimilar features from one of the two eye channels to promote single binocular vision. These two binocular processes work in tandem to achieve optimal 3D binocular vision. It can further be argued that for these processes to work well, the two eyes need to be exposed to stimuli of roughly equal strengths within the binocular visual field. For example, it has been shown that 3D depth perception (stereopsis) of observers with clinically normal binocular vision is degraded when stimuli of unequal contrast values are presented to the two eyes.27 This observation also predicts that stereopsis will be compromised when there exists an intrinsic imbalance between the two eyes. Indeed, observers with large sensory eye dominance (SED) have poor stereo acuity.713 
The phenomenon of eye dominance has been documented in the early clinical literature1416 and have more recently been investigated with more quantitative psychophysical approaches.8,9,13,1720 For example, Ooi and He13 investigated sensory eye dominance related to the interocular inhibitory process (SEDinhibition) by using a binocular rivalry display. The study found that several of their observers who were clinically considered to have normal binocular vision had significant SEDinhibition. Furthermore, for a subset of these observers, their weak eye monocular contrast sensitivity and monocular perceived brightness of suprathreshold gratings were no worse than the strong eye. This suggests that their SEDinhibition were more likely caused by an imbalance in mutual inhibition between the two eyes rather than a difference between the two monocular pathways before binocular interaction.8,13 
Figures 1A and 1B illustrate two pairs of dichoptic orthogonal gratings for measuring SEDinhibition typically used in our laboratory.8 During testing, stimulus A is displayed for a brief interval and the observer reports the predominant orientation of the perceived grating disc. For the next test trial, and depending on the observer's report in the preceding trial, the contrast of the vertical grating in the left eye (LE) is appropriately adjusted with an adaptive procedure21 before stimulus A is presented again. This finetuning of the contrast is done after each trial until the observer experiences an equal percentage of seeing the two gratings (point of equality). Since the contrast of the horizontal grating in the right eye (RE) is kept constant, the contrast of the vertical grating obtained at the point of equality is referred to as the LE balance contrast. But as the LE balance contrast could also be caused by a difference in sensitivity to the grating orientation (e.g., better sensitivity to vertical orientation), it is necessary to measure the RE balance contrast. This is obtained by switching the vertical and horizontal gratings between the two eyes as shown in stimulus B, and adjusting the contrast of the vertical grating now in the RE until the point of equality is obtained. The difference between the LE and RE balance contrast values defines the SEDinhibition
Figure 1
 
(a, b) Binocular rivalry stimulus used for measuring SEDinhibition. (a) Test 1: The LE balance contrast is obtained by varying the vertical grating contrast while keeping the contrast of the horizontal grating seen by the RE constant. The balance contrast is reached when the two eyes obtain an equal percentage of perceiving the two gratings (point of equality). (b) Test 2: Switching the grating orientation between the two eyes permits measurement of RE balance contrast. (c) Binocular combination stimulus for measuring SEDcombo. The dichoptic horizontal gratings have a 90° interocular phase difference.
Figure 1
 
(a, b) Binocular rivalry stimulus used for measuring SEDinhibition. (a) Test 1: The LE balance contrast is obtained by varying the vertical grating contrast while keeping the contrast of the horizontal grating seen by the RE constant. The balance contrast is reached when the two eyes obtain an equal percentage of perceiving the two gratings (point of equality). (b) Test 2: Switching the grating orientation between the two eyes permits measurement of RE balance contrast. (c) Binocular combination stimulus for measuring SEDcombo. The dichoptic horizontal gratings have a 90° interocular phase difference.
To reveal SED associated with an imbalance in the binocular combination process (SEDcombo), one can employ a pair of dichoptic stimuli with similar features that can be integrated as a single image representation.2225 Figure 1C illustrates the stimulus used in this study, which is adapted from Ding and Sperling.25 The dichoptic display comprises two horizontal sinusoidal gratings with 90° spatial phase difference (−45° vs. 45°). A pair of reference horizontal lines is placed on each side of the grating. With fusion, the observer perceives an integrated (cyclopean) grating and judges its spatial phase relative to the horizontal reference lines. An observer with a balanced binocular combination process will report zero phase shift relative to the horizontal reference lines when the contrast values of the two dichoptic gratings are identical. Conversely, an observer with an unbalanced binocular combination (SEDcombo) will perceive a phase shift toward that of the grating in the strong eye, indicating that the grating signal in the strong eye biases the spatial phase of the combined binocular grating. To measure SEDcombo, one can add extra contrast to the grating seen by the weak eye until the observer perceives the integrated binocular grating to have zero phase shift. The extra contrast is a measure of SEDcombo
A number of studies have revealed that SEDinhibition degrades binocular depth perception.8,9,11,12,26 For example, Xu et al.8 found stereo threshold increased as SEDinhibition increased. However, SEDinhibition can be reduced after visual training using the Push-Pull perceptual learning protocol that mainly targets the putative interocular inhibitory neural network.9,10,12,26,27 During a training trial, an attention cue (monocular frame) was briefly presented to the weak eye, followed by a binocular rivalry stimulus (a pair of vertical and horizontal gratings). The brief cue attracts transient attention to the weak eye, resulting in the grating in the weak eye being perceived (push) while the grating with orthogonal orientation in the strong eye was suppressed (pull). The role of the cue was to deploy transient (involuntary, bottom-up) attention to the weak eye to cause its stimulus to be perceived in dominance during binocular rivalry. The push-pull training is based on the hypothesis that the suppression of the half-image in the strong eye during the push–pull training can effectively shift the balance of interocular inhibition between the two eyes.9 This is because with the push–pull protocol, repetitive stimulation of the strong eye while preventing its signals from reaching the higher level (thus failing to induce conscious perception) could effectively degrade the efficiency of the excitatory synaptic transmission within the strong eye's channel and also depress the inhibition of the strong eye on the weak eye's channel.9,28 Undergoing the protocol also resulted in a decrease in stereo threshold. Of significance, since the Push-Pull perceptual learning stimuli do not carry binocular disparity information, it suggests the improvement in binocular depth perception is likely a consequence of the reduced SEDinhibition. A similar learning effect was revealed in adult amblyopic patients who had much larger SEDinhibition due to the stronger interocular suppression experienced by the amblyopic eye.26 
In this paper, our first goal was to reveal whether SEDinhibition and SEDcombo are independent, given that they are associated with imbalances of the two functionally distinct binocular processes. To investigate this, we designed the stimuli and procedures for measuring both SEDinhibition and SEDcombo to be as similar as possible, the details of which are provided in the Supplementary Materials
The second goal of this paper was to investigate whether SEDcombo is similar to SEDinhibition in influencing (increasing) stereo threshold. The answer might not be straightforward. First, previous psychophysical studies and modeling works suggest there exists a mutual inhibition between the two eyes' channels prior to binocular combination/summation.29 This provides an explanation for the findings that a normal observer's stereo threshold increases with the interocular contrast difference in the stereo stimuli.3,4,6,29 Conceivably, having an interocular imbalance can further weaken the signals from the monocular channel that receives the lower contrast image, further resulting in poorer spatial resolution of the monocular representation as well as a poorer binocular representation. One could conceivably apply this explanation to SEDcombo, wherein the larger suppression from the strong eye onto the weak eye can cause a poor spatial resolution of the binocular representation. Alternatively, one could argue that this explanation is not applicable to SEDcombo. This is because SEDcombo is often measured with dichoptic horizontal gratings (Fig. 1C) that stimulate neurons with horizontal orientation selectivity, which has little impact on the processing of horizontal binocular disparity for stereopsis. Accordingly, stereo thresholds may not be correlated with SEDcombo measured with dichoptic horizontal gratings. 
Another consideration relates to the explanation that the measured SEDcombo can be partially contributed by an imbalance in the binocular contrast gain control mechanism, which plays a major role in determining brightness/contrast perception of binocular surfaces. Several recent computational models of binocular contrast gain control have hypothesized that in addition to binocular summation, there exists mutual inhibitions between the two monocular channels.2225,30,31 Accordingly, the measured SEDcombo could comprise of an imbalance of interocular summation and an imbalance of interocular inhibition. However, since SEDcombo is typically measured with horizontal orientation gratings in the two eyes, the imbalance of interocular inhibition that contributes to SEDcombo may not necessarily be the same as that which contributes to SEDinhibition. This is because that the latter is measured with a binocular rivalry stimulus that activates interocular inhibition between different feature (e.g., orientation) channels from the two eyes. 
Methods
Observers
Eighteen observers (ages 18–25) who were naïve to the purpose of the study participated in the experiments. All had normal or corrected-to-normal visual acuity (at least 20/20), clinically acceptable fixation disparity (≤8.6 arc min) and stereopsis (≤40 arc sec). During the experiments, they viewed the computer monitor through a haploscopic mirror system attached to a head-and-chin rest from a distance of 100 cm. 
We measured all observers' SEDinhibit, SEDcombo, and interocular difference in contrast threshold (IDCT). Among them, seven observers also participated in the stereo disparity threshold experiment. All, except one observer, were also tested for motor eye dominance (MED). 
The research conducted followed the tenets of the Declaration of Helsinki and was approved by the institutional review board (IRB). Informed consent was obtained from the observers after explanation of the nature and possible consequences of the study. 
Apparatus
Gamma-corrected stimuli were generated on a Mac Pro computer running MatLab with PsychToolBox,32,33 and presented on a 21-inch flat CRT monitor. The resolution of the monitor was set at 2048 × 1536 pixels @ 75 Hz refresh rate. 
Stimuli and Procedures
SEDinhibition
The stimulus comprised a pair of dichoptic vertical and horizontal sinusoidal grating discs (diameter = 1°, 3 cycle/deg, 35 cd/m2) on a gray background (8° × 8°, 35 cd/m2; e.g., Fig. 1A). The contrast of the horizontal grating was held constant (1.5 log unit) while the contrast of the vertical grating was variable (0.376-1.976 log unit). (Note: log contrast is defined as log10(C), where C is the Michelson contrast in percentage. Therefore, 1.5 log unit = 31.6% Michelson contrast). A trial began with the observer fixating on a white nonius target (0.45° × 0.45°, line width = 0.1°, 70 cd/m2). When accurate fixation was achieved, the observer pressed the start button on the keyboard to remove the nonius target; 146 ms after the removal, a pair of dichoptic orthogonal gratings were presented for 400 ms. This was followed by a 200-ms mask (8° × 8° random dots [50% black and 50% white], 35 cd/m2, dot size = 4.7 arcmin; contrast = 1.7 log unit) to end the trial. The observer responded to his/her percept by key presses. If a piecemeal pattern of vertical and horizontal orientation was seen, the observer would respond to the predominant orientation perceived. The vertical grating contrast was adjusted after each trial until equal predominance was achieved using the QUEST procedure (40 trials/block). When the vertical grating was presented to the LE (Fig. 1A) we refer to its contrast at equal predominance as the LE's balance contrast. To obtain the RE's balance contrast, the gratings were switched between the eyes (Fig. 1B). The difference between the LE and RE balance contrast values (i.e., log[CLE] − log[CRE]), is defined as SEDinhibition. The order of testing the two conditions was counterbalanced and each condition was repeated four times. 
SEDcomb With QUEST Procedure
The test stimulus was a pair of dichoptic horizontal grating squares (1° × 1°, 3 cycle/deg, 35 cd/m2) with a 90° phase difference between them (Fig. 1C). The average phase of the two gratings was always held at 0° (θL = 45° and θR = −45°, or θL = −45° and θR = 45°). The contrast of the grating in one half-image was fixed at 1.5 log unit, while the contrast of the other grating in the tested eye varied from 0.376 to 1.976 log unit. The contrast in the tested eye was adjusted using the QUEST procedure over 40 trials in an experimental block, as the observer responded to either seeing the central dark band of the perceived grating as above or below the horizontal reference lines. When the variable contrast grating was presented to the LE, we refer to the contrast at which the grating was perceived to be aligned with the reference line as the LE's balance contrast. To measure the RE's balance contrast, we swapped the gratings between the two eyes so that the LE now received the grating with the fixed contrast and the RE the grating with the variable contrast. We refer to the difference between the LE and RE's balance contrast as SEDcombo
To control for the possible effect of contrast and grating phase in each half-image causing a positional bias, we tested SEDcombo with two display types. In one display type, the variable contrast grating's phase in the tested eye was shifted upward relative to the fixed contrast grating's phase in the fellow eye. In the second display type, the variable grating's phase was shifted below the fellow eye's grating. For data analysis, SEDcombo of the two types were averaged. 
The observer prepared for a trial by maintaining accurate eye alignment on the fusion-lock (2° × 2°). Then to begin the trial, he/she would press a button on the keyboard. This was followed 146 ms later, with the presentation of the dichoptic grating stimulus (1° × 1°, 3 cycle/deg, 35 cd/m2) for 400 ms. A 200-ms mask was then presented to end the trial (8° × 8° random dots patch, 35 cd/m2, 1.7 log unit). The observer's task was to report by pressing one of two keys on the keyboard to indicate whether the grating band was perceived above or below the reference lines. The order of testing the balance contrast of the two eyes and the two display types was counterbalanced and each block was repeated four times. 
Monocular Contrast Detection Threshold
The tested eye was presented with either a vertical or horizontal sinusoidal grating disc (35 cd/m2, 3 cycle/deg, diameter = 1°), while a homogeneous gray (blank) field with the same mean luminance level was presented to the fellow eye. Each test trial was conducted using the 2IFC method, whose temporal sequence was: fixation, interval-1 (400 ms), blank (400 ms), interval-2 (400 ms), blank (200 ms), and mask (8° × 8° random dots, 35 cd/m2, 1.7 log unit contrast, 200 ms). The grating disc was presented at only one interval while the other interval had a blank field. The observer responded whether he/she saw the grating in either interval-1 or -2 by a key press. The grating contrast was adjusted after each trial using the QUEST method to obtain the threshold. This contrast threshold test was repeated four times in each eye for each orientation. Throughout the experiment, four reference dots (size 0.18° × 0.18°) were displayed in a square formation (size 1.25° × 1.25°) surrounding the foveal location. The observer was instructed to maintain fixation at the center of the four dots where the stimulus was presented. 
Stereopsis Threshold
An 8° × 8° random-dot stereogram (35 cd/m2, 1.4 log unit contrast) with a crossed- or uncrossed-disparity 1° disc target was used (Fig. 2). The dot size of the random-dot was either 1.344 arc min (for four observers) or 2.016 arc min (for three observers), with the larger dot size only being used if the observer was unable to reliably perceive the smaller dot size. Five repeats of crossed disparity thresholds were measured before another five repeats of uncrossed disparity thresholds. 
Figure 2
 
Random-dot stereogram for measuring stereopsis threshold. The disc target can be rendered in crossed (top) or uncrossed (bottom) binocular disparity.
Figure 2
 
Random-dot stereogram for measuring stereopsis threshold. The disc target can be rendered in crossed (top) or uncrossed (bottom) binocular disparity.
The standard 2IFC method in combination with the staircase procedure was employed to measure the stereo disparity threshold. The temporal sequence of the stimulus presentation was nonius fixation (0.45° × 0.45°, line width = 0.1°, 70 cd/m2), blank (147 ms), interval-1 (53 ms), blank (400 ms), interval-2 (53 ms), and random-dot mask (200 ms, 8° × 8°, 1.7 log unit contrast, 35 cd/m2). Both intervals comprised images with random-dot, but only the stimulus in one interval had some binocular disparity while the stimulus in the other interval had zero disparity. A block of trials comprised 10 reversals (step size = 0.672 arc min, total ∼40–60 trials), and the average of the last six reversals were taken as the stereo threshold. During the experiment, the observer indicated whether the stimulus with the disk in depth was seen in interval-1 or -2 by pressing a key on the keyboard. 
Motor Eye Dominance
A variation of the Ring sighting test was used.8,13,34 To perform the test, the observer brought both hands simultaneously to the front of his/her face and formed a ring (2–3 inches in diameter) by bringing together the index finger and thumb from each hand. He/she then sighted a target with both eyes opened through this “ring”, while carefully placing the sighted target in the center of the ring. After this, he/she closed each eye alternately to determine which eye saw the target as more centered in the ring. The eye that saw the target as more centered is defined as the motor-dominant eye. 
Results
Correlation Between SEDcombo vs. SEDinhibition Using the QUEST Procedure
Figure 3 plots the SEDcombo versus SEDinhibition data of all 18 observers. Clearly, all the data points (except for one observer who has very small SEDcombo and SEDinhibition), fall in quadrants I and III, indicating the sign of sensory eye dominance remains the same for both inhibitory and combination measures. This observation is corroborated by a correlation analysis revealing a large r2 (n = 18, P < 0.0001, r2 = 0.687) and a slope of the regression line being close to 1 (slope = 1.056). This means that despite the SEDcombo and SEDinhibition being measured with different stimuli and tasks, they result in the same eye being dominant by about the same magnitude. (Note: The observers' data in Figure 3 are plotted in different symbols to represent how their SED relates to luminance contrast thresholds, which will be elaborated in Fig. 5). 
Figure 3
 
Relating the two types of sensory eye dominance. The graph plots SEDcombo as a function of SEDinhibition. Each symbol represents the data of an observer. Clearly, all symbols, except one, fall in the first and third quadrants, indicated that SEDcombo and SEDinhibition are highly correlated. The data points are also plotted in different symbols, with all the open symbols regardless of their shapes representing the observers whose SED could not be explained by an interocular difference in contrast threshold (IDCT) for horizontal and/or vertical grating. The relationship between SED and IDCT will be further elaborated in Figure 5 where the differently shaped symbols will be defined.
Figure 3
 
Relating the two types of sensory eye dominance. The graph plots SEDcombo as a function of SEDinhibition. Each symbol represents the data of an observer. Clearly, all symbols, except one, fall in the first and third quadrants, indicated that SEDcombo and SEDinhibition are highly correlated. The data points are also plotted in different symbols, with all the open symbols regardless of their shapes representing the observers whose SED could not be explained by an interocular difference in contrast threshold (IDCT) for horizontal and/or vertical grating. The relationship between SED and IDCT will be further elaborated in Figure 5 where the differently shaped symbols will be defined.
Impact of SEDcombo and SEDinhibition on Stereo Threshold
We measured stereo thresholds for detecting front and back depth of a random dot stereogram display and plotted the data as a function of the observer's SEDcombo and SEDinhibition in Figures 4A through 4D (n = 7). Figures 4A and 4B, respectively, show the stereo thresholds increased with SEDcombo for the front (P = 0.019, r2 = 0.700) and back (P = 0.012, r2 = 0.750) depth threshold conditions. To the best of our knowledge, this finding is the first to reveal the relationship between SEDcombo and stereo threshold in clinically normal observers. This relationship is similar to those for SEDinhibition, as found in the current study (Figs. 4C, 4D; front: P = 0.059, r2 = 0.544; back: P = 0.024, r2 = 0.670) and in our earlier studies.8,9,13 
Figure 4
 
Relating sensory eye dominance to stereo threshold. (a) SEDcombo versus crossed/front depth threshold; (b) SEDcombo versus uncrossed/back depth threshold. (c) SEDinhibition versus crossed/front depth threshold; (d) SEDinhibition versus uncrossed/back depth threshold. Generally, a larger interocular imbalance results in a higher stereo threshold for both types of SED.
Figure 4
 
Relating sensory eye dominance to stereo threshold. (a) SEDcombo versus crossed/front depth threshold; (b) SEDcombo versus uncrossed/back depth threshold. (c) SEDinhibition versus crossed/front depth threshold; (d) SEDinhibition versus uncrossed/back depth threshold. Generally, a larger interocular imbalance results in a higher stereo threshold for both types of SED.
Figure 5
 
Relating sensory eye dominance to the interocular difference in contrast threshold (IDCT). (a) SEDcombo versus IDCT for horizontal grating. (b) SEDinhibition versus IDCT for vertical and horizontal gratings. Filled circles represent observers whose SEDcombo and SEDinhibition were consistent with IDCT. Open circles represent observers whose SEDcombo and SEDinhibition were both opposite from the prediction made based on IDCT. The triangle represents the observer whose SEDinhibition, but not SEDcombo, was consistent with IDCT. The squares represent observers whose SEDcombo, but not SEDinhibition, was consistent with IDCT. Taken together, the data suggest that both monocular and binocular channels can cause interocular imbalance.
Figure 5
 
Relating sensory eye dominance to the interocular difference in contrast threshold (IDCT). (a) SEDcombo versus IDCT for horizontal grating. (b) SEDinhibition versus IDCT for vertical and horizontal gratings. Filled circles represent observers whose SEDcombo and SEDinhibition were consistent with IDCT. Open circles represent observers whose SEDcombo and SEDinhibition were both opposite from the prediction made based on IDCT. The triangle represents the observer whose SEDinhibition, but not SEDcombo, was consistent with IDCT. The squares represent observers whose SEDcombo, but not SEDinhibition, was consistent with IDCT. Taken together, the data suggest that both monocular and binocular channels can cause interocular imbalance.
Monocular Contrast Thresholds Versus SEDcombo and SEDinhibition
Given that the test stimuli for SEDcombo are dichoptic horizontal gratings, we also explored how horizontal grating contrast threshold affected SEDcombo. Figure 5A shows the relationship between SEDcombo and interocular difference in contrast threshold (IDCT) of the horizontal grating. If an observer's SEDcombo could be accounted for by IDCT, the data point should fall in quadrants I and III (i.e., the dominant eye based on SEDcombo measurement is also the strong eye based on monocular contrast threshold measurement). Instead, we found 7 out of 18 data points (39%, the six open circles and the triangle) fall in quadrants II and IV, although SEDcombo and IDCT are significantly correlated (P = 0.031, r2 = 0.260). A similar trend is found in the SEDinhibition and IDCT plot in Figure 5B (the average contrast thresholds of horizontal and vertical grating stimuli since both orientations make up the SEDinhibition test stimulus). Here too, we found that 8 out of 18 observers' data points (44%, the six open circles and the two squares) fall in quadrant IV. There is also a weak but significant correlation (P = 0.044, r2 = 0.230) between SEDinhibition and IDCT, which is also consistent with the previous SEDinhibition results from our lab.8,13 In one of these previous studies, we also measured observers' monocular suprathreshold brightness perception and found a similar trend as in the monocular contrast threshold test.13 Namely, we found that there were a number of observers with dominant eyes that had a relatively weaker brightness perception than the nondominant eyes. Furthermore, there were also observers whose dominant eyes had a stronger brightness perception. However, the difference in brightness perception between the two eyes was much smaller than the magnitude of SEDinhibition.13 It will be interesting to learn if future investigations of other types of suprathreshold measurements, such as interocular contrast matching and contrast increment threshold discrimination measurements, would reveal a similar trend. 
To summarize, these observations are graphed in Figures 5A and 5B with different symbols. Filled circles represent observers whose SEDcombo and SEDinhibition were both consistent with IDCT. Open circles represent observers whose SEDcombo and SEDinhibition were both opposite from the prediction made based on IDCT. The triangle represents the observer whose SEDinhibition, but not SEDcombo, was consistent with IDCT. The squares represent observers whose SEDcombo, but not SEDinhibition, was consistent with IDCT. These same symbols are also used to plot Figure 3 above. 
Additionally, we notice there exists an overlap between the two groups of “inconsistent” observers: six observers (open circles in Fig. 5) from the SEDcombo-IDCT inconsistent group (six out of seven) also exhibited inconsistent SEDinhibition-IDCT (six out of eight). Overall, half of our observers (nine open symbols) had SEDs that are not accountable for by the monocular contrast threshold difference between the two eyes. The remaining half (filled circles in Fig. 5) had SED and IDCT that are consistent, suggesting that an imbalance in the binocular visual processes could be due in part to a difference in the monocular channels' sensitivity. In other words, both binocular and monocular channels together can cause SED in our observers with clinically normal binocular vision. We recognize that although the correlations between the SEDs and IDCT in Figure 5 are statistically significant, they are nevertheless small. Thus, they do not suggest a strong causal relationship between SED and IDCT, and in general, the contribution of monocular channels to SED in our sample of clinically normal observers was not substantial. One possible reason is that most observers with clinically normal binocular vision have small differences between their monocular channels. However, whether this conclusion could be generalized to the amblyopic population who often has a larger IDCT, requires further psychophysical and physiological investigations of both SEDinhibition and SEDcombo. Pertaining to SEDinhibition, a study with a small sample size26 as well as from other unreported observations in our laboratory suggest the weakness of the amblyopic eye can be attributed to poor monocular contrast sensitivity and strong interocular suppression onto the weak eye. 
Separately, modeling studies from other laboratories suggest that both monocular deficits and asymmetric interocular inhibition contribute to SEDcombo in amblyopic observers.23,24,30,35 Neurophysiological studies on macaque monkeys have also shown that deficits in a monocular channel (e.g., ocular dominant neurons in V1) are correlated with the amblyopic eyes' poor visual functions.3638 However, little is known about the neurophysiological substrates of interocular suppression in amblyopia.39 
Motor Eye Dominance Versus SEDcombo and SEDinhibition
Figures 6A and 6B, respectively, plot the SEDinhibition and SEDcombo of each observer and his/her motor eye dominance (MED; n = 17). Negative values along the y-axis of each graph represent the amount of LE sensory eye dominance. Filled bars represent observers with LE motor dominance. We performed analysis by averaging the SEDs of all observers with the same sign of eye dominance as shown in Figures 6C and 6D, respectively, for SEDinhibition and SEDcombo. For SEDinhibition, the mean SEDinhibition for observers with LE motor dominance was −0.1346 ± 0.0648 log unit (negative value denotes LE dominance), and the mean SEDinhibition was 0.0617 ± 0.1161 log unit (positive value denotes RE dominance) for observers with RE motor dominance. Although the mean SEDinhibition for each eye was consistent with the motor eye dominance, statistical analysis fails to show a significant effect (point-biserial correlation coefficient: rpb = 0.3658; 2-tailed unpaired t-test with equal variances for correlation coefficient: t[15] = −1.522, P = 0.1487]). Similarly for SEDcombo, the means of SEDcombo were −0.2501 ± 0.1031 log unit and 0.0299 ± 0.1246 log unit, respectively, for observers with LE and RE motor eye dominance, which is not significantly different (point-biserial correlation coefficient: rpb = 0.4109; 2-tailed unpaired t-test with equal variances for correlation coefficient: t[15]= −1.746, P = 0.1013). This observation is consistent with our previous findings using a binocular rivalry stimulus, that SEDinhibition and motor eye dominance do not necessarily reside in the same eye of an individual.8,12,13 Our current finding is also in agreement with the view that motor eye dominance may not always correspond with the asymmetry in visual functions.40 
Figure 6
 
Relating sensory eye dominance (SED) to motor eye dominance (MED). (a) SEDinhibition versus MED. (b) SEDcombo versus MED. Each bar along the x-axis represents the MED data of an individual observer. The data for observers with RE MED are plotted with open bars while the data for observers with LE MED are plotted with filled bars. The length of the bar is determined by his/her SED, whose magnitude is represented along the y-axis, with data for the observers with RE dominance being plotted as positive values. Therefore, should SED and MED be consistent, all filled bars should have negative values. Our data show that this is not the case. Graphs (c) and (d), respectively, plot the average SEDinhibition versus MED and average SEDcombo versus MED. Although each graph shows the mean SED was consistent with the mean MED, statistical analyses fail to show a significant relationship.
Figure 6
 
Relating sensory eye dominance (SED) to motor eye dominance (MED). (a) SEDinhibition versus MED. (b) SEDcombo versus MED. Each bar along the x-axis represents the MED data of an individual observer. The data for observers with RE MED are plotted with open bars while the data for observers with LE MED are plotted with filled bars. The length of the bar is determined by his/her SED, whose magnitude is represented along the y-axis, with data for the observers with RE dominance being plotted as positive values. Therefore, should SED and MED be consistent, all filled bars should have negative values. Our data show that this is not the case. Graphs (c) and (d), respectively, plot the average SEDinhibition versus MED and average SEDcombo versus MED. Although each graph shows the mean SED was consistent with the mean MED, statistical analyses fail to show a significant relationship.
Other laboratories11,41,42 have measured normal observers' SEDinhibition using binocular rivalry stimuli that were presented for a longer duration (1 minute). They defined the eye having the higher predominance of seeing its half-image as the SEDinhibition dominant eye. These studies also measured motor eye dominance. One study by Dieter et al.11 with a large sample size17 failed to show significant relationship between SEDinhibition and MED. Conversely, two of other studies reported a statistically significant relationship between sensory and motor eye dominance.41,42 However, we do not think the difference between these reports and our current findings can be attributed to the shorter stimulus duration used in our current study (400 ms). This is because a previous study from our laboratory revealed a significant correlation between SEDinhibition with a short duration presentation (500 ms) and SEDinhibition based on predominance over longer duration presentation (30 seconds).12 
Discussion
The current paper reveals a significant correlation between SEDcombo and SEDinhibition for observers with clinically normal vision. Our study also shows that stereo threshold increases with SEDcombo, a trend that is similar to that with SEDinhibition. Furthermore, by comparing monocular contrast thresholds between the two eyes, we found half of our observers' SEDcombo and SEDinhibition cannot be accounted for by an interocular difference in contrast threshold (IDCT). For these observers, their SED are likely caused by an imbalance within the binocular visual processes. For the remaining half of our observers, both difference between the monocular channels and imbalances within the binocular visual processes are likely to contribute to their SED. 
Presumably, the SEDcombo and SEDinhibition psychophysically measured here with two different types of binocular stimuli, respectively, reflect imbalances in the binocular combination and interocular inhibitory processes. If these two binocular processes were independent, a poor correlation would be found between an observer's SEDcombo and SEDinhibition. However, this prediction is not borne out by our current study. One possible explanation is that while the two processes are implemented by distinct distributed neural networks along the visual pathway from LGN to primary visual cortex (V1) and extrastriate visual cortical areas, they do interact with each other along the different stages of the visual pathway. For example, one could speculate that the LGN is one location where the interaction takes place. Neurophysiologic studies have shown that binocular suppression begins as early as dLGN.4346 From modeling work, Ding and Levi47 suggested that the interocular inhibition in the LGN, which lacks of orientation selectivity, could provide the neurophysiologic substrate for the interocular gain-control in binocular combination. This would predict that an imbalance of interocular inhibition at dLGN can affect SEDcombo. On the other hand, SEDinhibition is revealed with binocular rivalry stimuli (e.g., orthogonal orientation gratings) that activate the interocular inhibitory network at V1.4852 The interocular inhibitory activities at V1 during binocular rivalry can further affect dLGN neurons via feedback networks.53,54 
It is also reasonable to hypothesize that both binocular combination (compatible binocular stimulus) and interocular inhibitory (incompatible/rivalrous stimulus) processes belong to a larger integrated network that represents binocular surfaces. Consistent with this explanation, there are empirical findings showing the binocular combination and interocular inhibition can operate concurrently.5,55,56 Of course, this hypothesis needs to be further developed and tested. For example, it would be interesting to learn how modeling works that focuses on binocular combination (e.g., the gain-control theory of binocular vision by Ding and Sperling,25 Ding et al.,22,30 and Ding and Levi47,57) could account for binocular rivalry perception, which in turn could provide insights on our findings of significant correlation between SEDcombo and SEDinhibition
It is also possible that even if the two binocular visual processes have little interaction, the interocular imbalance is caused by the same factors during development. For example, when one eye receives weaker signals during development, it will affect both binocular visual processes in a similar manner, which can result in a similar ocular dominance. In particular, if the monocular deficits are located in the early visual pathway before binocular interaction occurs, the deficits can further affect the development of the binocular processes, causing permanent interocular imbalances. Further studies are needed to investigate these possibilities. 
Acknowledgments
Supported by National Institutes of Health grants EY023561 and EY023374. 
Disclosure: C. Han, None; Z.J. He, None; T.L. Ooi, None 
References
Howard I, Rogers B. Binocular Vision and Stereopsis. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 1995.
Smallman HS, McKee SP. A contrast ratio constraint on stereo matching. Proc Biol Sci. 1995; 260: 265–271.
Halpern DL, Blake RR. How contrast affects stereoacuity. Perception. 1988; 17: 483–495.
Schor C, Heckmann T. Interocular differences in contrast and spatial frequency: effects on stereopsis and fusion. Vision Res. 1989; 29: 837–847.
Wolfe JM. Stereopsis and binocular rivalry. Psychol Rev. 1986; 93: 269–282.
Legge GE, Gu YC. Stereopsis and contrast. Vision Res. 1989; 29: 989–1004.
Ooi TL, He ZJ. Unequal interocular suppression in stereopsis. 1996; 139.
Xu JP, He ZJ, Ooi TL. A binocular perimetry study of the causes and implications of sensory eye dominance. Vision Res. 2011; 51: 2386–2397.
Xu JP, He ZJ, Ooi TL. Effectively reducing sensory eye dominance with a push-pull perceptual learning protocol. Curr Biol. 2010; 20: 1864–1868.
Xu JP, He ZJ, Ooi TL. Perceptual learning to reduce sensory eye dominance beyond the focus of top-down visual attention. Vision Res. 2012; 61: 39–47.
Dieter KC, Sy JL, Blake R. Persistent biases in binocular rivalry dynamics within the visual field. Vision. 2017; 1: 18.
Xu JP, He ZJ, Ooi TL. Push-pull training reduces foveal sensory eye dominance within the early visual channels. Vision Res 2012; 61: 48–59.
Ooi TL, He ZJ. Sensory eye dominance. Optometry. 2001; 72: 168–178.
Sheard C. The dominant or sighting eye. Am J Physiol Optics. 1923; 1923; 4: 49–54.
Pascal J. The chromatic test for the dominant eye. Am J Ophthalmol. 1926; 9: 357–358.
Porac C, Coren S. The dominant eye. Psychol Bull. 1976; 83: 880–897.
Dieter KC, Sy JL, Blake R. Individual differences in sensory eye dominance reflected in the dynamics of binocular rivalry. Vision Res. 2017; 141: 40–50.
Leat SJ, Woodhouse JM. Rivalry with continuous and flashed stimuli as a measure of ocular dominance across the visual field. Perception. 1984; 13: 351–357.
Lunghi C, Burr DC, Morrone C. Brief periods of monocular deprivation disrupt ocular balance in human adult visual cortex. Curr Biol. 2011; 21: R538–R539.
Yang E, Blake R, McDonald JEII. A new interocular suppression technique for measuring sensory eye dominance. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2010; 51: 588–593.
Watson AB, Pelli DG. QUEST: a Bayesian adaptive psychometric method. Percept Psychophys. 1983; 33: 113–120.
Ding J, Klein SA, Levi DM. Binocular combination in abnormal binocular vision. J Vis. 2013; 13 (2): 14.
Huang CB, Zhou J, Lu ZL, Feng L, Zhou Y. Binocular combination in anisometropic amblyopia. J Vis. 2009; 9: 17 11–1716.
Huang CB, Zhou J, Lu ZL, Zhou Y. Deficient binocular combination reveals mechanisms of anisometropic amblyopia: signal attenuation and interocular inhibition. J Vis. 2011; 11 (6): 4.
Ding J, Sperling G. A gain-control theory of binocular combination. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2006; 103: 1141–1146.
Ooi TL, Su YR, Natale DM, He ZJ. A push-pull treatment for strengthening the ‘lazy eye' in amblyopia. Curr Biol. 2013; 23: R309–R310.
Xu JP, He ZJ, Ooi TL. Further support for the importance of the suppressive signal (pull) during the push-pull perceptual training. Vision Res. 2012; 61: 60–69.
Hebb D. The Organization of Behavior. New York, NY: Wiley; 1949.
Kontsevich LL, Tyler CW. Analysis of stereothresholds for stimuli below 2.5 c/deg. Vision Res. 1994; 34: 2317–2329.
Ding J, Klein SA, Levi DM. Binocular combination of phase and contrast explained by a gain-control and gain-enhancement model. J Vis. 2013; 13 (2): 13.
Meese TS, Georgeson MA, Baker DH. Binocular contrast vision at and above threshold. J Vis. 2006; 13: 1224–1243.
Brainard DH. The Psychophysics Toolbox. Spat Vis. 1997; 10: 433–436.
Pelli DG. The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. Spat Vis. 1997; 10: 437–442.
Borish I. Clinical Refraction. 3rd ed. New York, NY: Fairchild Publications.
Baker DH, Meese TS, Hess RF. Contrast masking in strabismic amblyopia: attenuation, noise, interocular suppression and binocular summation. Vision Res. 2008; 48: 1625–1640.
Hallum LE, Shooner C, Kumbhani RD, et al. Altered balance of receptive field excitation and suppression in visual cortex of amblyopic macaque monkeys. J Neurosci. 2017; 37: 8216–8226.
Shooner C, Hallum LE, Kumbhani RD, et al. Asymmetric dichoptic masking in visual cortex of amblyopic macaque monkeys. J Neurosci. 2017; 37: 8734–8741.
Shooner C, Hallum LE, Kumbhani RD, et al. Population representation of visual information in areas V1 and V2 of amblyopic macaques. Vision Res. 2015; 114: 56–67.
Kiorpes L, Daw N. Cortical correlates of amblyopia. Vis Neurosci. 2018; 35: E016.
Mapp AP, Ono H, Barbeito R. What does the dominant eye dominate? A brief and somewhat contentious review. Percept Psychophys. 2003; 65: 310–317.
Handa T, Mukuno K, Uozato H, et al. Effects of dominant and nondominant eyes in binocular rivalry. Optom Vis Sci. 2004; 81: 377–383.
Zheleznyak L, Alarcon A, Dieter KC, Tadin D, Yoon G. The role of sensory ocular dominance on through-focus visual performance in monovision presbyopia corrections. J Vis. 2015; 15 (6): 17.
Guido W, Tumosa N, Spear PD. Binocular interactions in the cat's dorsal lateral geniculate nucleus. I. Spatial-frequency analysis of responses of X, Y, and W cells to nondominant-eye stimulation. J Neurophysiol. 1989; 62: 526–543.
Sengpiel F, Blakemore C, Harrad R. Interocular suppression in the primary visual cortex: a possible neural basis of binocular rivalry. Vision Res. 1995; 35: 179–195.
Sengpiel F, Blakemore C. The neural basis of suppression and amblyopia in strabismus. Eye (Lond). 1996; 10 (pt 2): 250–258.
Tumosa N, McCall MA, Guido W, Spear PD. Responses of lateral geniculate neurons that survive long-term visual cortex damage in kittens and adult cats. J Neurosci. 1998; 9: 280–298.
Ding J, Levi DM. Binocular combination of luminance profiles. J Vis. 2017; 17 (13): 4.
Xu H, Han C, Chen M, et al. Rivalry-like neural activity in primary visual cortex in anesthetized monkeys. J Neurosci 2016; 36: 3231–3242.
Tong F, Engel SA. Interocular rivalry revealed in the human cortical blind-spot representation. Nature. 2001; 411: 195–199.
Polonsky A, Blake R, Braun J, Heeger DJ. Neuronal activity in human primary visual cortex correlates with perception during binocular rivalry. Nat Neurosci. 2000; 3: 1153–1159.
Leopold DA, Logothetis NK. Activity changes in early visual cortex reflect monkeys' percepts during binocular rivalry. Nature. 1996; 379: 549–553.
Blake R. Logothetis N. Visual competition. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2002; 3: 13–21.
Haynes JD, Deichmann R, Rees G. Eye-specific effects of binocular rivalry in the human lateral geniculate nucleus. Nature. 2005; 438: 496–499.
Wunderlich K, Schneider KA, Kastner S. Neural correlates of binocular rivalry in the human lateral geniculate nucleus. Nat Neurosci. 2005; 8: 1595–1602.
Treisman A. Binocular rivalry and stereoscopic depth perception. Q J Exp Psychol. 1962; 14: 23–37.
Su Y, He ZJ, Ooi TL. Coexistence of binocular integration and suppression determined by surface border information. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2009; 106: 15990–15995.
Ding J, Levi DM. Binocular contrast discrimination needs monocular multiplicative noise. J Vis. 2016; 16 (5): 12.
Figure 1
 
(a, b) Binocular rivalry stimulus used for measuring SEDinhibition. (a) Test 1: The LE balance contrast is obtained by varying the vertical grating contrast while keeping the contrast of the horizontal grating seen by the RE constant. The balance contrast is reached when the two eyes obtain an equal percentage of perceiving the two gratings (point of equality). (b) Test 2: Switching the grating orientation between the two eyes permits measurement of RE balance contrast. (c) Binocular combination stimulus for measuring SEDcombo. The dichoptic horizontal gratings have a 90° interocular phase difference.
Figure 1
 
(a, b) Binocular rivalry stimulus used for measuring SEDinhibition. (a) Test 1: The LE balance contrast is obtained by varying the vertical grating contrast while keeping the contrast of the horizontal grating seen by the RE constant. The balance contrast is reached when the two eyes obtain an equal percentage of perceiving the two gratings (point of equality). (b) Test 2: Switching the grating orientation between the two eyes permits measurement of RE balance contrast. (c) Binocular combination stimulus for measuring SEDcombo. The dichoptic horizontal gratings have a 90° interocular phase difference.
Figure 2
 
Random-dot stereogram for measuring stereopsis threshold. The disc target can be rendered in crossed (top) or uncrossed (bottom) binocular disparity.
Figure 2
 
Random-dot stereogram for measuring stereopsis threshold. The disc target can be rendered in crossed (top) or uncrossed (bottom) binocular disparity.
Figure 3
 
Relating the two types of sensory eye dominance. The graph plots SEDcombo as a function of SEDinhibition. Each symbol represents the data of an observer. Clearly, all symbols, except one, fall in the first and third quadrants, indicated that SEDcombo and SEDinhibition are highly correlated. The data points are also plotted in different symbols, with all the open symbols regardless of their shapes representing the observers whose SED could not be explained by an interocular difference in contrast threshold (IDCT) for horizontal and/or vertical grating. The relationship between SED and IDCT will be further elaborated in Figure 5 where the differently shaped symbols will be defined.
Figure 3
 
Relating the two types of sensory eye dominance. The graph plots SEDcombo as a function of SEDinhibition. Each symbol represents the data of an observer. Clearly, all symbols, except one, fall in the first and third quadrants, indicated that SEDcombo and SEDinhibition are highly correlated. The data points are also plotted in different symbols, with all the open symbols regardless of their shapes representing the observers whose SED could not be explained by an interocular difference in contrast threshold (IDCT) for horizontal and/or vertical grating. The relationship between SED and IDCT will be further elaborated in Figure 5 where the differently shaped symbols will be defined.
Figure 4
 
Relating sensory eye dominance to stereo threshold. (a) SEDcombo versus crossed/front depth threshold; (b) SEDcombo versus uncrossed/back depth threshold. (c) SEDinhibition versus crossed/front depth threshold; (d) SEDinhibition versus uncrossed/back depth threshold. Generally, a larger interocular imbalance results in a higher stereo threshold for both types of SED.
Figure 4
 
Relating sensory eye dominance to stereo threshold. (a) SEDcombo versus crossed/front depth threshold; (b) SEDcombo versus uncrossed/back depth threshold. (c) SEDinhibition versus crossed/front depth threshold; (d) SEDinhibition versus uncrossed/back depth threshold. Generally, a larger interocular imbalance results in a higher stereo threshold for both types of SED.
Figure 5
 
Relating sensory eye dominance to the interocular difference in contrast threshold (IDCT). (a) SEDcombo versus IDCT for horizontal grating. (b) SEDinhibition versus IDCT for vertical and horizontal gratings. Filled circles represent observers whose SEDcombo and SEDinhibition were consistent with IDCT. Open circles represent observers whose SEDcombo and SEDinhibition were both opposite from the prediction made based on IDCT. The triangle represents the observer whose SEDinhibition, but not SEDcombo, was consistent with IDCT. The squares represent observers whose SEDcombo, but not SEDinhibition, was consistent with IDCT. Taken together, the data suggest that both monocular and binocular channels can cause interocular imbalance.
Figure 5
 
Relating sensory eye dominance to the interocular difference in contrast threshold (IDCT). (a) SEDcombo versus IDCT for horizontal grating. (b) SEDinhibition versus IDCT for vertical and horizontal gratings. Filled circles represent observers whose SEDcombo and SEDinhibition were consistent with IDCT. Open circles represent observers whose SEDcombo and SEDinhibition were both opposite from the prediction made based on IDCT. The triangle represents the observer whose SEDinhibition, but not SEDcombo, was consistent with IDCT. The squares represent observers whose SEDcombo, but not SEDinhibition, was consistent with IDCT. Taken together, the data suggest that both monocular and binocular channels can cause interocular imbalance.
Figure 6
 
Relating sensory eye dominance (SED) to motor eye dominance (MED). (a) SEDinhibition versus MED. (b) SEDcombo versus MED. Each bar along the x-axis represents the MED data of an individual observer. The data for observers with RE MED are plotted with open bars while the data for observers with LE MED are plotted with filled bars. The length of the bar is determined by his/her SED, whose magnitude is represented along the y-axis, with data for the observers with RE dominance being plotted as positive values. Therefore, should SED and MED be consistent, all filled bars should have negative values. Our data show that this is not the case. Graphs (c) and (d), respectively, plot the average SEDinhibition versus MED and average SEDcombo versus MED. Although each graph shows the mean SED was consistent with the mean MED, statistical analyses fail to show a significant relationship.
Figure 6
 
Relating sensory eye dominance (SED) to motor eye dominance (MED). (a) SEDinhibition versus MED. (b) SEDcombo versus MED. Each bar along the x-axis represents the MED data of an individual observer. The data for observers with RE MED are plotted with open bars while the data for observers with LE MED are plotted with filled bars. The length of the bar is determined by his/her SED, whose magnitude is represented along the y-axis, with data for the observers with RE dominance being plotted as positive values. Therefore, should SED and MED be consistent, all filled bars should have negative values. Our data show that this is not the case. Graphs (c) and (d), respectively, plot the average SEDinhibition versus MED and average SEDcombo versus MED. Although each graph shows the mean SED was consistent with the mean MED, statistical analyses fail to show a significant relationship.
Supplement 1
×
×

This PDF is available to Subscribers Only

Sign in or purchase a subscription to access this content. ×

You must be signed into an individual account to use this feature.

×