Open Access
Visual Neuroscience  |   January 2019
Retinal Deficits Precede Cognitive and Motor Deficits in a Rat Model of Type II Diabetes
Author Affiliations & Notes
  • Rachael S. Allen
    Atlanta VA Center for Visual and Neurocognitive Rehabilitation, Atlanta VA Health Care System, Decatur, Georgia, United States
    Department of Biomedical Engineering, Georgia Institute of Technology, Atlanta, Georgia, United States
  • Andrew Feola
    Atlanta VA Center for Visual and Neurocognitive Rehabilitation, Atlanta VA Health Care System, Decatur, Georgia, United States
    Department of Biomedical Engineering, Georgia Institute of Technology, Atlanta, Georgia, United States
  • Cara T. Motz
    Atlanta VA Center for Visual and Neurocognitive Rehabilitation, Atlanta VA Health Care System, Decatur, Georgia, United States
  • Amy L. Ottensmeyer
    Atlanta VA Center for Visual and Neurocognitive Rehabilitation, Atlanta VA Health Care System, Decatur, Georgia, United States
    Department of Ophthalmology, Emory University, Atlanta, Georgia, United States
  • Kyle C. Chesler
    Atlanta VA Center for Visual and Neurocognitive Rehabilitation, Atlanta VA Health Care System, Decatur, Georgia, United States
    Department of Biomedical Engineering, Georgia Institute of Technology, Atlanta, Georgia, United States
  • Ryan Dunn
    Atlanta VA Center for Visual and Neurocognitive Rehabilitation, Atlanta VA Health Care System, Decatur, Georgia, United States
    Department of Biomedical Engineering, Georgia Institute of Technology, Atlanta, Georgia, United States
  • Peter M. Thulé
    Section Endocrinology & Metabolism, Atlanta VA Health Care System & Emory University School of Medicine, Decatur, Georgia, United States
  • Machelle T. Pardue
    Atlanta VA Center for Visual and Neurocognitive Rehabilitation, Atlanta VA Health Care System, Decatur, Georgia, United States
    Department of Biomedical Engineering, Georgia Institute of Technology, Atlanta, Georgia, United States
  • Correspondence: Rachael S. Allen, Atlanta VA Center for Visual and Neurocognitive Rehabilitation, Research Service (151 Oph), 1670 Clairmont Road, Decatur, GA 30033, USA; restewa@emory.edu
Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science January 2019, Vol.60, 123-133. doi:https://doi.org/10.1167/iovs.18-25110
  • Views
  • PDF
  • Share
  • Tools
    • Alerts
      ×
      This feature is available to authenticated users only.
      Sign In or Create an Account ×
    • Get Citation

      Rachael S. Allen, Andrew Feola, Cara T. Motz, Amy L. Ottensmeyer, Kyle C. Chesler, Ryan Dunn, Peter M. Thulé, Machelle T. Pardue; Retinal Deficits Precede Cognitive and Motor Deficits in a Rat Model of Type II Diabetes. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 2019;60(1):123-133. https://doi.org/10.1167/iovs.18-25110.

      Download citation file:


      © ARVO (1962-2015); The Authors (2016-present)

      ×
  • Supplements
Abstract

Purpose: To investigate the temporal appearance of retinal, cognitive, and motor deficits in Goto-Kakizaki (GK) rats, a spontaneously occurring, polygenic model of type II diabetes. GK rats develop impaired insulin secretion at 2 weeks and fasting hyperglycemia at 4 weeks.

Methods: In male and female GK rats and Wistar controls, glucose tolerance test (hyperglycemia) and electroretinogram (ERG, retinal function) were performed at 4 and 8 weeks of age. Spectral domain-optical coherence tomography (retinal structure) was assessed at 6 weeks. Spatial alternation (cognitive function) and number of entries (exploratory behavior) were assessed via Y-maze at 4, 5, 6, 7, and 8 weeks. Rotarod (motor function) was performed at 4, 6, and 8 weeks.

Results: By 4 weeks, the GK rats exhibited significant glucose intolerance (P < 0.001) and retinal deficits, including delays in ERG implicit times (flicker, P < 0.01; oscillatory potentials, P < 0.001). In addition, the GK rats showed greater ERG amplitudes (P < 0.001) and thinner retinas (P < 0.001). At 7 weeks, the GK rats showed deficits in cognitive function (P < 0.001) and exploratory behavior (P < 0.01). However, no motor function deficits were observed by 8 weeks. Interestingly, the male GK rats showed greater hyperglycemia (P < 0.05), but the female rats showed greater ERG delays (P < 0.001).

Conclusions: In GK rats, retinal function deficits developed prior to cognitive or motor deficits. Future studies will investigate common mechanistic links, long-term functional and vascular changes, and whether early retinal deficits can predict cognitive dysfunction or late-stage retinal disease.

In the United States, more than 30 million people have diabetes,1 a number predicted to increase 35% by 2025.2 Chronic hyperglycemia and accompanying inflammation with diabetes cause complications such as diabetic retinopathy (DR), a leading cause of blindness in working-age adults.3 In the clinic, DR is characterized by retinal ischemia, macular edema, and abnormal neovascularization. However, signs of retinal dysfunction and damage have been observed before the detection of vascular pathology in the clinic.4,5 These early changes in the retinas of diabetic patients and diabetic animals include impairments in night vision,6 contrast sensitivity,6,7 visual acuity (Aung MH, et al. IOVS 2011;52:ARVO E-Abstract 5960),7 and color vision,6 as well as electroretinogram (ERG) deficits such as delayed 30 Hz flicker implicit time8,9 and delayed oscillatory potentials (OPs).1016 Retinal cells affected include amacrine, Müller, photoreceptor, and ganglion cells.4,17 Because these neural changes in the diabetic retina occur prior to the clinically recognized vascular pathology, they may allow for earlier detection and treatment for DR. 
Oxidative stress, hypoxia, lipid abnormalities, and inflammation occur in both the brain and retina with diabetes, suggesting that the disease is damaging both tissues via similar pathways.1823 Deficits in both cognitive and motor function have been shown to occur with diabetes,24,25 and the presence of DR is associated with cognitive deficits and greater amounts of ischemia in the brain.26 Early or nonproliferative DR (which may indicate chronic hyperglycemia) is associated with structural changes in brain as well as deficits in attention, concentration, and information processing.27 In addition, patients with proliferative DR show increased cortical atrophy as measured by magnetic resonance imaging,28 and severity of vascular DR may be associated with accelerated cognitive decline with age, particularly in men.29 These studies show links between cerebral and retinal vascular complications with diabetes, but it is not known whether early retinal neuronal deficits can be used as an early marker for other complications. Starting interventions at the earliest signs of diabetes-induced retinal dysfunction could prevent the development of cognitive and motor deficits and worsening of retinal deficits, but first, the time course of these deficits must be identified. 
Because 90% to 95% of diabetic patients have type II diabetes,1 we chose an animal model of type II diabetes for this research. The GK rat is a non-insulin-dependent, polygenic, nonobese model that develops impaired glucose-induced insulin secretion by 2 weeks and fasting hyperglycemia by 4 weeks.30,31 Retinal32,33 and cognitive deficits3436 and reduced motor nerve conduction37 have been reported in the GK rat. However, the temporal appearance of these deficits and the relationship between them has not been described. We hypothesized that GK rats will exhibit retinal dysfunction prior to cognitive and motor dysfunction. 
Materials and Methods
Animals, Diabetes Confirmation, and Experimental Design
Male and female GK (diabetic) and Wistar (nondiabetic control; Charles River, Wilmington, MA, USA, and in-house breeding; n = 78) rats were housed in shoebox-style cages with chow and water provided ad libitum on a 12:12 light:dark cycle (light onset at 6:00 AM). All procedures were approved by the Atlanta Veterans Affairs Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee and conformed to the ARVO Statement for the Use of Animals in Ophthalmic and Vision Research and the National Institutes of Health guide for the care and use of laboratory animals.38 One GK rat died during the course of the study and was excluded from our analysis. 
The GK rat is a spontaneously occurring, nonobese model of type II diabetes that was developed by selectively interbreeding Wistar rats that showed the highest blood glucose levels during a glucose tolerance test (GTT). As a consquence, GTT responses exhibit the most striking blood glucose differences between GK and wildtype animals.30,39 Thus, GTTs were used to confirm hyperglycemia prior to structural and functional assessments (4 and 8 weeks). Body weight was monitored weekly. Unlike rats with streptozotocin (STZ)-induced diabetes,40 GK rats do not lose weight with diabetes, and thus insulin treatment was not needed. 
ERG recordings were performed at the 4- and 8-week timepoints to assess retinal function, and Y-maze testing for spontaneous alternation was performed at 5, 6, 7, and 8 weeks to assess cognitive function and exploratory behavior (see Fig. 1 for experimental timeline). An additional group of GK (n = 7) and Wistar (n = 10) rats were tested on the Y-maze beginning at 4 weeks so that comparisons to 4-week ERG results would be more meaningful. Previously, we had not begun Y-maze testing until 5 weeks because younger animals seem to show a fear response in the maze and exhibit little movement. By handling the rats for 5 minutes prior to starting the test and by starting them at the choice point (as opposed to the back of the first arm), we obtained 4-week Y-maze data in ∼85% of the animals. Rats that did not perform on Y-maze at 4 weeks were excluded from the experiment. 
Figure 1
 
Experimental timeline and appearance of functional changes. The first observable changes in the GK rat after hyperglycemia are retinal function deficits (4 weeks) with deficits in exploratory behavior, cognitive function, and retinal structure occurring at 6 weeks, and motor coordination deficits and vascular pathology not occurring during the 8-week testing period.
Figure 1
 
Experimental timeline and appearance of functional changes. The first observable changes in the GK rat after hyperglycemia are retinal function deficits (4 weeks) with deficits in exploratory behavior, cognitive function, and retinal structure occurring at 6 weeks, and motor coordination deficits and vascular pathology not occurring during the 8-week testing period.
In a subset of rats, rotarod testing was performed at 4, 6, and 8 weeks to assess motor function, and spectral domain-optical coherence tomography (SD-OCT) was performed at 6 weeks to assess in vivo retinal structure. In a second subset of rats, GTT and ERG were performed at 3 weeks to determine whether these changes occurred prior to 4 weeks. Optomotor response was not assessed as both GK and Wistar rats were unresponsive to this test, which is not surprising given that some albino rodents are incapable of tracking41 and others show greatly reduced tracking (Prusky G, personal communication, 2014). 
Assessment of Hyperglycemia: GTT
GTTs were used to measure hyperglycemia. Rats were fasted for 6 hours prior to intraperitoneal injections of glucose (2 mg/kg body weight) in dH2O. Blood glucose levels (mg/dL) were monitored at 0, 15, 30, 60, and 120 minutes using a handheld blood glucose meter (FreeStyle Lite; Abbott Diabetes Care, Alameda, CA, USA) and test strips with blood obtained via tail prick. When comparing glucose tolerance in GK rats across time, blood glucose levels were normalized to those of Wistar controls. 
Assessment of Retinal Function: ERG
After overnight dark adaptation, retinal responses to light were measured in rats via ERG as previously described.42,43 Briefly, under dim red light, the rats were anesthetized with ketamine (60 mg/kg) and xylazine (7.5 mg/kg), the corneal surface was anesthetized (0.5% tetracaine), and pupils were dilated (1% tropicamide). Platinum needle ground and reference electrodes were placed in the tail and each cheek, respectively. A gold loop recording electrode was placed on each cornea. Flash stimuli were presented, and electrical responses were recorded using a signal-averaging system (UTAS BigShot; LKC Technologies, Gaithersburg, MD, USA). ERG stimuli consisted of a 6-step protocol of flash stimuli presented in order of increasing luminance. Five dark-adapted responses were recorded to isolate rod-dominated and mixed rod and cone responses (scotopic: −3.0 to 2.1 log cd s/m2). The rats were then light-adapted (30 cd/m2) for 10 minutes to saturate the rod photoreceptors after which responses were recorded to flicker stimuli (2.0 log cd s/m2 at 6 Hz) in the presence of the background light to isolate cone pathway function. Afterward, the animals were given yohimbine (2.1 mg/kg) to reverse the effects of xylazine and prevent corneal ulcers.44 
ERG data were analyzed offline. The right and left eye responses were averaged for each animal. Amplitudes and implicit times were measured for a-waves, b-waves, and OPs and flicker response for ERG waveforms. Using the ERG system software, OPs were filtered digitally offline (75–500 Hz; EM Version 8.1.2, 2008; LKC Technologies). OPs were analyzed for two flash stimuli, which we previously showed to distinguish early dysfunction in diabetes7,16: −1.9 log cd s/m2 (rod pathway function) and 0.7 log cd s/m2 (rod-cone pathway). 
Assessment of Retinal Structure: SD-OCT
To assess retinal structure, SD-OCT was used as described previously45 to capture in vivo retinal images at 6 weeks. After general anesthesia (using the same methods described in the ERG section), the corneas were anesthetized (0.5% tetracaine), pupils were dilated (1% tropicamide), and the rats were placed on an adjustable stage. An OCT camera (Bioptigen 4300; Bioptigen, Inc., Morrisville, NC, USA) was used to record a 3-mm radial scan centered at the optic nerve for the right eye only. 
Using a customized MATLAB program (MathWorks, Natick, MA, USA), images were marked manually by a trained technician. Layers assessed included the retinal nerve fiber layer, inner plexiform layer, inner nuclear layer, outer plexiform layer, outer nuclear layer, external limiting membrane, inner segments/outer segments, and retinal pigment epithelium (RPE). Individual layer thicknesses were measured by the difference in distance between markers placed on the outer edges of each layer, and total retinal thickness was computed as the distance between the top marker of the retinal nerve fiber layer to the bottom marker of the RPE. Measurements were taken for the first (superior–inferior) and third (nasal–temporal) B-scans. Within each orientation, measurements were taken at 0.5 and 1.2 mm to the left and right of the optic nerve head, resulting in two distances each for the superior, inferior, nasal, and temporal quadrants of the eye. 
Analysis of Cognitive and Exploratory Behavior: Y-Maze
To assess short-term spatial memory and exploratory behavior, the Y-maze (San Diego Instruments, San Diego, CA, USA) was used to record spontaneous alternation behavior based on the methods outlined by Maurice and colleagues.46 Briefly, the rats were positioned in one arm of the Y-maze and allowed to explore freely for 8 minutes. The series of arm entries was monitored and recorded. Entering all three arms consecutively was defined as a successful alternation. The percentage of correct spontaneous alternations was calculated as follows: number of correct alternations / (total number of arm entries − 2) × 100. This number was used to represent cognitive function. The total number of entries during the course of 8 minutes was used to represent exploratory behavior. 
Analysis of Motor Function: Rotarod
The rotarod (San Diego Instruments) was used to measure balance and motor coordination, which have been shown to be impaired in diabetic animals.25,47 The rats were placed on a rotating bar, and the latency to fall was recorded. Prior to testing, the animals were allowed 1 minute to acclimate to the stationary rotarod. During testing, the rod began rotating slowly, with the speed gradually accelerating to a maximum of 50 rpm during a 5-minute period. If the rat fell off within the first 15 seconds of rotation, the trial was repeated. The rats underwent four trials per testing day with 5 minutes of rest in between. The three best trials were averaged for each rat. 
Statistical Analysis
The results are expressed as mean ± standard error of the mean (SEM). Weight, GTT, ERG (a-wave, b-wave, OPs, 4- and 8-week comparisons), OCT, Y-maze, and rotarod results were analyzed using a 2-way repeated measures (RM) ANOVA followed by Holms-Sidak tests for individual comparisons. Flicker ERG results were analyzed using a t-test. Average glucose tolerance results over time and comparisons between male and female GK rats were analyzed using a 1-way ANOVA followed by Holms-Sidak tests for individual comparisons. 
Results
GK Rats Showed Reduced Body Weight and Increased Blood Glucose
Although GK rats continued to gain weight over time and did not exhibit weight loss in response to their hyperglycemia, GK rats were ∼30% smaller than Wistar controls, as demonstrated by significantly reduced body weights at all timepoints measured from 4 to 8 weeks of age (RM ANOVA main effect of strain, F1,164 = 26.938, P < 0.001; Fig. 2A). GK rats showed significant increases in fasting blood glucose (the 0-minute timepoint of the GTT, 121.5 mg/dL vs. 85.4 mg/dL, P < 0.05) and progressively worsening glucose tolerance (26%–117%) when compared with Wistar controls (P < 0.001 at 15, 30, and 60 minutes post–glucose administration; RM ANOVA interaction effect, strain × timepoint, F1,106 = 15.558, P < 0.001; Fig. 2B). Impaired glucose tolerance in GK rats was observed as early as 3 weeks (the earliest point measured, 38% higher, P < 0.001) and worsened significantly over time (78% at 4 weeks and 96% at 8 weeks; RM ANOVA interaction effect, strain × age, F1,87 = 21.9, P < 0.001; Fig. 2C). 
Figure 2
 
GK rats showed reduced glucose tolerance and body weight when compared with nondiabetic controls. (A) Weight (g) at 4 to 8 weeks of age. (B) Average blood glucose (mg/dL) at 0, 15, 30, 60, and 120 minutes post–glucose injection for GTT performed at 4 weeks of age. (C) Average blood glucose for all 5 timepoints (0, 15, 30, 60, 120 minutes) of GTTs for 3, 4, and 8 weeks of age. GK values were normalized to Wistar values at each age. Black asterisks indicate comparisons between GK and Wistar groups. Red asterisks indicate comparisons between GK groups at different ages. *P < 0.05, ***P < 0.001. Results expressed as means ± SEM.
Figure 2
 
GK rats showed reduced glucose tolerance and body weight when compared with nondiabetic controls. (A) Weight (g) at 4 to 8 weeks of age. (B) Average blood glucose (mg/dL) at 0, 15, 30, 60, and 120 minutes post–glucose injection for GTT performed at 4 weeks of age. (C) Average blood glucose for all 5 timepoints (0, 15, 30, 60, 120 minutes) of GTTs for 3, 4, and 8 weeks of age. GK values were normalized to Wistar values at each age. Black asterisks indicate comparisons between GK and Wistar groups. Red asterisks indicate comparisons between GK groups at different ages. *P < 0.05, ***P < 0.001. Results expressed as means ± SEM.
GK Rats Showed Increased ERG Amplitudes and Delayed Implicit Times
At 4 weeks of age, GK rats showed increased a-wave and b-wave amplitudes, as shown in representative waveforms (Fig. 3A). When amplitudes were quantified, a-waves were 95% larger and b-waves were 72% larger (RM ANOVA interaction effect; strain × flash intensity; a-wave, F1,159 = 83.828, P < 0.001; b-wave, F1,199 = 31.314, P < 0.001; Figs. 3B, 3C). GK rats also showed increased amplitudes for OPs and flicker ERGs as well as delays in OP and flicker ERG implicit times (Figs. 4A, 4B). When quantified, OP2 amplitudes were 130% larger and flicker amplitudes were 32% larger in 4-week-old GK rats when compared with Wistar controls (OP, RM ANOVA interaction effect; strain × OP, F1,159 = 59.138, P < 0.001; flicker, t-test; t38 = 2.652, P < 0.05; Figs. 4C, 4D). When implicit times were quantified, GK rats exhibited significant delays in OP and flicker ERG implicit times (OP, RM ANOVA interaction effect; strain × OP, F1,159 = 55.868, P < 0.001; flicker, t-test; t38 = 3.505, P < 0.01; Figs. 4E, 4F). In a subset of animals, ERGs were also performed at 3 weeks of age with similar results (data not shown). 
Figure 3
 
GK rats showed increased a-wave and b-wave amplitudes that approached control levels over time. (A) Representative waveforms for dark-adapted ERG (0.7 log cd s/m2). Quantification of dark-adapted a-wave (B) and b-wave amplitudes (C) at 4 weeks of age. Average summed ERG amplitudes normalized to controls for 4 and 8 weeks of age for a-wave (D) and b-wave (E). Black asterisks indicate comparisons between GK and Wistar groups. Red asterisks indicate comparisons between GK groups at different ages. *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001. Results expressed as means ± SEM.
Figure 3
 
GK rats showed increased a-wave and b-wave amplitudes that approached control levels over time. (A) Representative waveforms for dark-adapted ERG (0.7 log cd s/m2). Quantification of dark-adapted a-wave (B) and b-wave amplitudes (C) at 4 weeks of age. Average summed ERG amplitudes normalized to controls for 4 and 8 weeks of age for a-wave (D) and b-wave (E). Black asterisks indicate comparisons between GK and Wistar groups. Red asterisks indicate comparisons between GK groups at different ages. *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001. Results expressed as means ± SEM.
Figure 4
 
GK rats showed increased OP and flicker amplitudes that decrease over time and delayed OP and flicker implicit times that persist to 8 weeks. Representative waveforms for dark-adapted OPs (0.7 log cd s/m2) (A) and light-adapted flicker ERG (B). Quantification of OP (C) and flicker (D) amplitudes and OP (E) and flicker (F) implicit times at 4 weeks of age. Average ERG values normalized to controls for 4 and 8 weeks of age for OP amplitudes (0.7 log cd s/m2) (G) and flicker implicit times (H). Black asterisks indicate comparisons between GK and Wistar groups. Red asterisks indicate comparisons between GK groups at different ages. *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001. Results expressed as means ± SEM.
Figure 4
 
GK rats showed increased OP and flicker amplitudes that decrease over time and delayed OP and flicker implicit times that persist to 8 weeks. Representative waveforms for dark-adapted OPs (0.7 log cd s/m2) (A) and light-adapted flicker ERG (B). Quantification of OP (C) and flicker (D) amplitudes and OP (E) and flicker (F) implicit times at 4 weeks of age. Average ERG values normalized to controls for 4 and 8 weeks of age for OP amplitudes (0.7 log cd s/m2) (G) and flicker implicit times (H). Black asterisks indicate comparisons between GK and Wistar groups. Red asterisks indicate comparisons between GK groups at different ages. *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001. Results expressed as means ± SEM.
From 4 to 8 weeks of age, GK rat ERG amplitudes approached control levels while delays in implicit times persisted. Specifically, in rats that underwent both 4- and 8-week ERGs, a-wave amplitude declined from 86% to 54% (RM ANOVA main effect of timepoint, F1,41 = 4.374, P < 0.05; Fig. 3D), b-wave amplitude declined from 81% to 33% (RM ANOVA interaction effect; strain × timepoint, F1,41 = 7.725, P < 0.05; Fig. 3E), OP amplitude declined from 121% to 27% (RM ANOVA interaction effect; strain × timepoint, F1,41 = 15.721, P < 0.001; Fig. 4G), and flicker amplitude declined from 46% to 28% (data not shown). Meanwhile, the implicit time delay did not show significant change between 4 and 8 weeks for either OPs (data not shown) or flicker (RM ANOVA main effect of strain, F1,41 = 24.861, P < 0.001; Fig. 4H]). 
GK Rats Had Thinner Retinas Than Wistar Controls
Representative SD-OCT images reveal thinner retinas in GK rats (Figs. 5A, 5B). At 6 weeks of age, GK rats had thinner retinal nerve fiber layers in the peripheral, but not the central, retina when compared with Wistar controls (RM ANOVA main effect of strain, F1,81 = 8.388, P < 0.01; Fig. 5C). Total retinal thickness was reduced in GK versus Wistar rats as well, both centrally (P < 0.05, data not shown) and peripherally, with the greatest differences being observed in the inferior and temporal quadrants of the retina (RM ANOVA interaction effect, strain × quadrant, F1,81 = 6.784, P < 0.001; Fig. 5D). Thinner retinal layers for GK rats were also observed in the central and peripheral RPE (P < 0.01 for both), central inner plexiform layer (P < 0.001), and central inner nuclear layer (P < 0.05; data not shown). 
Figure 5
 
GK rats had thinner retinal nerve fiber layers and total retinal thicknesses. Representative SD-OCT images from Wistar controls (A) and GK rats (B) at 6 weeks of age. Total retinal thickness was measured from the bottom of the RPE to the top of the RNFL. (C, D) Mean thicknesses for RNFL (C) and total thickness (D) for GK and Wistar rats for all four quadrants (Sup, Superior; Temp, Temporal; Inf, Inferior; Nas, Nasal) of the peripheral retina. **P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001. Results expressed as means ± SEM. RNFL, retinal nerve fiber layer; IPL, inner plexiform layer; INL, inner nuclear layer; OPL, outer plexiform layer; ONL, outer nuclear layer; ELM, external limiting membrane; IS/OS, inner segment/outer segment layer.
Figure 5
 
GK rats had thinner retinal nerve fiber layers and total retinal thicknesses. Representative SD-OCT images from Wistar controls (A) and GK rats (B) at 6 weeks of age. Total retinal thickness was measured from the bottom of the RPE to the top of the RNFL. (C, D) Mean thicknesses for RNFL (C) and total thickness (D) for GK and Wistar rats for all four quadrants (Sup, Superior; Temp, Temporal; Inf, Inferior; Nas, Nasal) of the peripheral retina. **P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001. Results expressed as means ± SEM. RNFL, retinal nerve fiber layer; IPL, inner plexiform layer; INL, inner nuclear layer; OPL, outer plexiform layer; ONL, outer nuclear layer; ELM, external limiting membrane; IS/OS, inner segment/outer segment layer.
GK Rats Showed Deficits in Cognitive and Exploratory Behavior, But Not Motor Function
The GK rats showed significant deficits in cognitive behavior as measured by spontaneous alternation on the Y-maze, beginning at 7 weeks (RM ANOVA interaction effect, strain × age, F1,261 = 2.986, P < 0.05; Fig. 6A). GK rats were indistinguishable from Wistar rats at 4 weeks (P = 0.694), a time when ERG deficits were already apparent. The power of the performed test for strain differences = 0.976. Significant deficits in exploratory behavior as measured by number of entries on the Y-maze also occurred at 7 weeks (RM ANOVA interaction effect, strain × age, F1,261 = 10.789, P < 0.001; Fig. 6B). No deficits in motor coordination as measured by rotarod were observed even by 8 weeks (Fig. 6C). 
Figure 6
 
Cognitive and exploratory behavior, but not motor, deficits in GK rats. (A) Cognitive function at 4 to 8 weeks of age as measured by spontaneous alternation on Y-maze. (B) Exploratory behavior at 4 to 8 weeks of age as measured by number of entries on Y-maze. (C) Motor coordination as measured by rotarod latency. *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001. Results expressed as means ± SEM.
Figure 6
 
Cognitive and exploratory behavior, but not motor, deficits in GK rats. (A) Cognitive function at 4 to 8 weeks of age as measured by spontaneous alternation on Y-maze. (B) Exploratory behavior at 4 to 8 weeks of age as measured by number of entries on Y-maze. (C) Motor coordination as measured by rotarod latency. *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001. Results expressed as means ± SEM.
Greater Hyperglycemia in Male GK Rats; Greater ERG Delays in Female GK Rats
Male GK rats showed higher levels of hyperglycemia (P < 0.05), but female rats showed greater ERG delays for both flicker (P < 0.001; Fig. 7) and OPs (P < 0.05; data not shown). Meanwhile, male GK rats showed greater reductions in exploratory behavior (P < 0.05). No significant sex differences were observed for cognitive behavior or rotarod (Fig. 7). 
Figure 7
 
Sex differences in GK rats. A comparison of performance on behavioral and functional tests for female and male GK rats versus Wistar controls (sexes combined). GK values were normalized to control values for each test. Flicker, flicker implicit time; Exp, exploratory. *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001. Results expressed as means ± SEM.
Figure 7
 
Sex differences in GK rats. A comparison of performance on behavioral and functional tests for female and male GK rats versus Wistar controls (sexes combined). GK values were normalized to control values for each test. Flicker, flicker implicit time; Exp, exploratory. *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001. Results expressed as means ± SEM.
Discussion
Retinal Changes Appear Prior to Cognitive and Motor Changes in the GK Rat Model of Type II Diabetes
In GK rats, retinal deficits were the first to appear after hyperglycemia, with significant delays in OP and flicker implicit times being observed at 4 weeks of age. Deficits in spatial cognition and exploratory behavior did not appear until 7 weeks of age, and motor deficits were not observed even at 8 weeks (Fig. 1). In previous studies, our group identified retinal function deficits that preceded retinal vascular pathology in both a rodent model of type I diabetes7 and in human diabetic patients.16 If retinal deficits are the earliest functional change after hyperglycemia, then starting treatment at the first sign of retinal dysfunction could prevent or delay cognitive and motor deficits and worsening of retinal deficits. These rodent experiments suggest a similar window in people with diabetes, the discovery of which might result in accelerated treatment of diabetic complications. 
It is important to note that we used one test of cognitive function (spontaneous alternation on Y-maze) and one test of motor function (rotarod). Other assays may be more sensitive and could detect changes at earlier timepoints. However, previous studies in GK rats only revealed deficits at later times, including deficits in novel object recognition observed at 24 weeks of age,48 deficits in memory retention observed at 10 weeks,49 and deficits in olfactory memory at 6 to 10 months.35 
Supernormal ERG Amplitudes in the GK Rat
In addition to delays in ERG implicit time that are well documented in diabetic retinopathy,7,16 we observed ERG amplitudes that were much larger than those of nondiabetic controls. Although supernormal ERG amplitudes are occasionally reported in patients with early-stage diabetic retinopathy,50 the magnitude of the increases recorded here is unusual. It is possible that because hyperglycemia and impaired insulin secretion begin at 2 weeks in this model, a time when the retina is still developing, the hyperglycemia may alter the development of excitatory and inhibitory retinal circuitry such that ERG amplitudes are increased. Indeed, early changes in insulin receptor signaling in the hippocampus were shown to inhibit synaptic maturation and potentially cause cognitive deficits in the GK rat.49 In addition, decreased inhibition from amacrine cells has been proposed to increase rod bipolar cell signaling in the diabetic retina,51 suggesting that amacrine cells could be investigated in the GK rat as a target for synaptic change during hyperglycemia in retinal development. Contrary to our results, other groups have reported reduced ERG amplitudes in the GK rat.32,33 However, after the GK rat was developed by Goto and Kakizaki in 1975 in Japan,38 one colony remained in Japan and eventually became the colony sold by Charles River Laboratories, whereas other colonies were started in Europe in the 1990s using stock from the original colony (e.g., the colony that eventually became the animals sold by Taconic Europe, Isaszeg, Hungary). Differences in islet metabolism, islet morphology, and insulin content have been observed among the colonies,30 and use of rats from different colonies may explain why we observe different ERG phenotypes. 
Retinal, Cognitive, and Metabolic Changes in the GK Rat When Compared With Other Diabetes Models
When examining the large ERG amplitude increases observed in GK rats over Wistar rats, it is important to consider whether these increases are caused by larger signal in the GK rats or smaller signal in the Wistar rats. In Figure 6A, we compare the b-wave amplitude data from GK and Wistar rats presented here with b-wave amplitude data from a type I model of diabetes, the STZ rat. In this model, we induced hyperglycemia in 9-week-old Long Evans rats using STZ (100 mg/kg), a toxin that ablates pancreatic beta cells. These data have been presented previously,40 but have been repurposed to make comparisons with the GK rat. Here, we compare ERGs from 4-week-old GK and Wistar rats with STZ rats that have been hyperglycemic for 4 weeks. We show that b-wave amplitudes are very similar for Long Evans controls and Wistar controls, indicating that the amplitude increases observed in the GK rat are not the result of abnormalities in the Wistar rat. Although significant amplitude increases are observed in the STZ rat as well, the magnitude of increase is larger in the GK rat (71% vs. 31%, on average), and the GK rat amplitudes are significantly larger than the STZ rat amplitudes. 
For ERG implicit times, the oscillatory potential delays observed in the GK rats (12%) were similar to the delays observed in the STZ rats (17%; Fig. 6B). Delays in oscillatory potential and flicker ERG implicit times, similar to those found in the GK rats here, are typical of the retinal dysfunction observed in early DR in a type I diabetes model.7,16,40 Interestingly, in the STZ rats, delays were observed in OP1, OP2, OP3, and OP4, whereas in the GK rats, delays were observed in only OP3 and OP4. Meanwhile, OP delays were observed with both bright (0.7 log cd s/m2) and dim (−1.9 log cd s/m2) flashes in the GK rats versus dim flash only in the STZ rats. 
The GK and STZ rats also showed similar deficits for cognitive and exploratory behavior, with the GK rats showing a 19% decline in spontaneous alternation versus 12% in the STZ rats and the GK rats showing a 19% decline in exploratory behavior versus 20% in the STZ rats (Figs. 8C, 8D). The differences in cognitive and exploratory behavior observed between Wistar controls and Long Evans controls may be the result of age differences. The data presented are from 8-week-old GK and Wistar rats and 17-week-old STZ rats and Long Evans controls (8 weeks posthyperglycemia). 
Figure 8
 
A comparison of type II GK rats versus type I STZ-induced rats. GK rats and Wistar controls when compared with STZ-treated rats and nondiabetic Long Evans controls for (A) ERG b-wave amplitude, (B) oscillatory potential (OP3) implicit times at flash intensity −1.9 log cd s/m2, (C) spontaneous alternation on Y-maze, and (D) exploratory behavior on Y-maze. STZ model results are repurposed from data presented in Allen et al.40 *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001. Results expressed as means ± SEM. For (A), black asterisks indicate comparisons between GK and all other groups; gray asterisks indicate comparisons between diabetic and both control groups.
Figure 8
 
A comparison of type II GK rats versus type I STZ-induced rats. GK rats and Wistar controls when compared with STZ-treated rats and nondiabetic Long Evans controls for (A) ERG b-wave amplitude, (B) oscillatory potential (OP3) implicit times at flash intensity −1.9 log cd s/m2, (C) spontaneous alternation on Y-maze, and (D) exploratory behavior on Y-maze. STZ model results are repurposed from data presented in Allen et al.40 *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001. Results expressed as means ± SEM. For (A), black asterisks indicate comparisons between GK and all other groups; gray asterisks indicate comparisons between diabetic and both control groups.
In terms of metabolic phenotype, the GK rats exhibit large impairments in glucose tolerance and a fasting blood glucose of 121.5 mg/dL. For comparison, 250 mg/dL is our fed blood glucose cutoff for inclusion as a type I animal in our STZ experiments. As for other type II models, Zucker diabetic fatty rats show fed blood glucose levels of 86 mg/dL at 6 weeks and 200 to 289 mg/dL at 12 to 40 weeks.52 Otsuka Long-Evans Tokushima Fatty (OLETF) rats show fed blood glucose levels of 143 to 168 mg/dL between 10 and 30 week,53 and high-fat diet + low dose STZ rats show fasting blood glucose levels of 140+ mg/dL.54 Unlike the STZ rat, the GK rats are not insulin dependent. Unlike other type II models, the GK rats weigh less than their Wistar counterparts and are not obese. However, despite showing reduced weights, the GK rats show changes in lipid profiles, including increased cholesterol levels from 7 weeks though 26 weeks, increased high-density lipoproteins (HDL) levels at 26 but not 18 weeks, increased triglycerides at 12 weeks through 30 weeks, and increased free fatty acids at 18 weeks through 30 weeks.5558 Unfortunately, we did not measure lipid profiles in this experiment, and the studies that investigated lipid profiles did not use timepoints as early as 4 and 8 weeks, the timepoints at which we measured retinal and cognitive function. It is possible that hyperlipidemia, as well as hyperglycemia, contributes to the retinal and cognitive deficits observed here. 
Greater Hyperglycemia in Male GK Rats; Greater ERG Delays in Female GK Rats
Male GK rats exhibited significantly higher levels of hyperglycemia than female GK rats. This finding is not surprising given that female rats often require more intensive measures (i.e., a higher percentage of high-fat diet) to induce hyperglycemia.59 Female animals appear to have a track record for being more resistant to the development of diabetes. Interestingly, despite having reduced hyperglycemia, the female GK rats exhibited greater delays in flicker ERG and OP implicit times. This finding highlights the importance of testing both male and female animals in models of diabetes and in other disease and injury models. 
Conclusions
Retinal function deficits developed prior to cognitive or motor deficits in the GK rat model of type II diabetes. Future studies will investigate mechanistic links between retinal and cognitive deficits; assess long-term changes in retinal, cognitive, and motor function and retinal vasculature; and determine whether early retinal deficits can predict cognitive dysfunction and late-stage retinal disease. Future research will also investigate whether starting interventions at the earliest signs of diabetes-induced changes in the retina could prevent or delay other complications, including late-stage retinal vascular changes. 
Acknowledgments
Supported by the Department of Veterans Affairs (Rehab R&D Service Merit Award I01 RX00951 and I01 RX002615) and a Research Career Scientist Award (C9257) to MTP and Career Development Award (RX002111) to RSA. 
Disclosure: R.S. Allen, None; A. Feola, None; C.T. Motz, None; A.L. Ottensmeyer, None; K.C. Chesler, None; R. Dunn, None; P.M. Thulé, None; M.T. Pardue, None 
References
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. National Diabetes Statistics Report, 2017. Available at: https://www.cdc.gov/features/diabetes-statistic-report/index.html.
King H, Aubert RE, Herman WH. Global burden of diabetes, 1995-2025: prevalence, numerical estimates, and projections. Diabetes Care. 1998; 21: 1414–1431.
Klein BE. Overview of epidemiologic studies of diabetic retinopathy. Ophthalmic Epidemiol. 2007; 14: 179–183.
Antonetti DA, Barber AJ, Bronson SK, et al. Diabetic retinopathy: seeing beyond glucose-induced microvascular disease. Diabetes. 2006; 55: 2401–2411.
Barber AJ. A new view of diabetic retinopathy: a neurodegenerative disease of the eye. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2003; 27: 283–290.
Ghirlanda G, Di Leo MA, Caputo S, Cercone S, Greco AV. From functional to microvascular abnormalities in early diabetic retinopathy. Diabetes Metab Rev. 1997; 13: 15–35.
Aung MH, Kim MK, Olson DE, Thule PM, Pardue MT. Early visual deficits in streptozotocin-induced diabetic Long Evans rats. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2013; 54: 1370–1377.
Tyrberg M, Lindblad U, Melander A, Lovestam-Adrian M, Ponjavic V, Andreasson S. Electrophysiological studies in newly onset type 2 diabetes without visible vascular retinopathy. Doc Ophthalmol. 2011; 123: 193–198.
Holopigian K, Greenstein VC, Seiple W, Hood DC, Carr RE. Evidence for photoreceptor changes in patients with diabetic retinopathy. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1997; 38: 2355–2365.
Shirao Y, Kawasaki K. Electrical responses from diabetic retina. Prog Retin Eye Res. 1998; 17: 59–76.
Holopigian K, Seiple W, Lorenzo M, Carr R. A comparison of photopic and scotopic electroretinographic changes in early diabetic retinopathy. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1992; 33: 2773–2780.
Lecleire-Collet A, Audo I, Aout M, et al. Evaluation of retinal function and flicker light-induced retinal vascular response in normotensive patients with diabetes without retinopathy. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2011; 52: 2861–2867.
Kizawa J, Machida S, Kobayashi T, Gotoh Y, Kurosaka D. Changes of oscillatory potentials and photopic negative response in patients with early diabetic retinopathy. Jpn J Ophthalmol. 2006; 50: 367–373.
Simonsen SE. The value of the oscillatory potential in selecting juvenile diabetics at risk of developing proliferative retinopathy. Acta Ophthalmol (Copenh). 1980; 58: 865–878.
Severns ML, Johnson MA, Bresnick GH. Methodologic dependence of electroretinogram oscillatory potential amplitudes. Doc Ophthalmol. 1994; 86: 23–31.
Pardue MT, Barnes CS, Kim MK, et al. Rodent hyperglycemia-induced inner retinal deficits are mirrored in human diabetes. Trans Vis Sci Tech. 2014; 3 (3): 6.
Fletcher EL, Phipps JA, Ward MM, Puthussery T, Wilkinson-Berka JL. Neuronal and glial cell abnormality as predictors of progression of diabetic retinopathy. Curr Pharm Des. 2007; 13: 2699–2712.
Kowluru RA, Chan PS. Oxidative stress and diabetic retinopathy. Exp Diabetes Res. 2007; 2007: 43603.
Raza H, John A, Howarth FC. Increased oxidative stress and mitochondrial dysfunction in zucker diabetic rat liver and brain. Cell Physiol Biochem. 2015; 35: 1241–1251.
Ly A, Yee P, Vessey KA, Phipps JA, Jobling AI, Fletcher EL. Early inner retinal astrocyte dysfunction during diabetes and development of hypoxia, retinal stress, and neuronal functional loss. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2011; 52: 9316–9326.
Fletcher EL, Downie LE, Ly A, et al. A review of the role of glial cells in understanding retinal disease. Clin Exp Optom. 2008; 91: 67–77.
Muriach M, Flores-Bellver M, Romero FJ, Barcia JM. Diabetes and the brain: oxidative stress, inflammation, and autophagy. Oxid Med Cell Longev. 2014; 2014: 102158.
Lutty GA. Effects of diabetes on the eye. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2013; 54: ORSF81–ORSF87.
van Duinkerken E, Klein M, Schoonenboom NS, et al. Functional brain connectivity and neurocognitive functioning in patients with long-standing type 1 diabetes with and without microvascular complications: a magnetoencephalography study. Diabetes. 2009; 58: 2335–2343.
Sherin A, Peeyush KT, Jayanarayanan S, Amee KK, Paulose CS. Decreased cholinergic receptor expression in the striatum: motor function deficit in hypoglycemic and diabetic rats. Cell Mol Neurobiol. 2012; 32: 83–93.
Haan M, Espeland MA, Klein BE, et al. Cognitive function and retinal and ischemic brain changes: the Women's Health Initiative. Neurology. 2012; 78: 942–949.
Ferguson SC, Blane A, Perros P, et al. Cognitive ability and brain structure in type 1 diabetes: relation to microangiopathy and preceding severe hypoglycemia. Diabetes. 2003; 52: 149–156.
Wessels AM, Rombouts SA, Simsek S, et al. Microvascular disease in type 1 diabetes alters brain activation: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Diabetes. 2006; 55: 334–340.
Ding J, Strachan MW, Reynolds RM, et al. Diabetic retinopathy and cognitive decline in older people with type 2 diabetes: the Edinburgh Type 2 Diabetes Study. Diabetes. 2010; 59: 2883–2889.
Ostenson CG. The Goto-Kakizaki rat. In: Shafrir E, ed. Animal Models of Diabetes: Frontiers in Research. Boca Raton, FL: Taylor & Francis Group; 2007: 119–137.
Portha B, Serradas P, Bailbe D, Suzuki K, Goto Y, Giroix MH. Beta-cell insensitivity to glucose in the GK rat, a spontaneous nonobese model for type II diabetes. Diabetes. 1991; 40: 486–491.
Matsubara H, Kuze M, Sasoh M, Ma N, Furuta M, Uji Y. Time-dependent course of electroretinograms in the spontaneous diabetic Goto-Kakizaki rat. Jpn J Ophthalmol. 2006; 50: 211–216.
Fan Y, Liu K, Wang Q, Ruan Y, Zhang Y, Ye W. Exendin-4 protects retinal cells from early diabetes in Goto-Kakizaki rats by increasing the Bcl-2/Bax and Bcl-xL/Bax ratios and reducing reactive gliosis. Mol Vis. 2014; 20: 1557–1568.
Li XH, Xin X, Wang Y, et al. Pentamethylquercetin protects against diabetes-related cognitive deficits in diabetic Goto-Kakizaki rats. J Alzheimers Dis. 2013; 34: 755–767.
Lietzau G, Davidsson W, Ostenson CG, et al. Type 2 diabetes impairs odour detection, olfactory memory and olfactory neuroplasticity; effects partly reversed by the DPP-4 inhibitor Linagliptin. Acta Neuropathol Commun. 2018; 6: 14.
Moreira T, Malec E, Ostenson CG, Efendic S, Liljequist S. Diabetic type II Goto-Kakizaki rats show progressively decreasing exploratory activity and learning impairments in fixed and progressive ratios of a lever-press task. Behav Brain Res. 2007; 180: 28–41.
Suzuki K, Saito N, Sakata Y, Toyota T, Goto Y. A new prostaglandin E1 analogue (TFC-612) improves the reduction in motor nerve conduction velocity in spontaneously diabetic GK (Goto-Kakizaki) rats. Prostaglandins. 1990; 40: 463–471.
National Research Council (US) Committee for the Update of the Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals. Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals. 8th ed. Washington DC: National Academies Press (US); 2011.
Goto Y, Kakizaki M, Masaki N. Production of spontaneous diabetic rats by repetition of selective breeding. Tohoku J Exp Med. 1976; 119: 85–90.
Allen RS, Hanif AM, Gogniat MA, et al. TrkB signalling pathway mediates the protective effects of exercise in the diabetic rat retina. Eur J Neurosci. 2018; 47: 1254–1265.
Precht W, Cazin L. Functional deficits in the optokinetic system of albino rats. Exp Brain Res. 1979; 37: 183–186.
Allen RS, Olsen TW, Sayeed I, et al. Progesterone treatment in two rat models of ocular ischemia. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2015; 56: 2880–2891.
Aung MH, Park HN, Han MK, et al. Dopamine deficiency contributes to early visual dysfunction in a rodent model of type 1 diabetes. J Neurosci. 2014; 34: 726–736.
Turner PV, Albassam MA. Susceptibility of rats to corneal lesions after injectable anesthesia. Comp Med. 2005; 55: 175–182.
Allen R, Motz CT, Feola A, et al. Long-term functional and structural consequences of primary blast overpressure to the eye. J Neurotrauma. 2018 35: 2104–2116.
Maurice T, Bayle J, Privat A. Learning impairment following acute administration of the calcium channel antagonist nimodipine in mice. Behav Pharmacol. 1995; 6: 167–175.
Nascimento PS, Lovatel GA, Barbosa S, et al. Treadmill training improves motor skills and increases tyrosine hydroxylase immunoreactivity in the substantia nigra pars compacta in diabetic rats. Brain Res. 2011; 1382: 173–180.
Hardigan T, Yasir A, Abdelsaid M, et al. Linagliptin treatment improves cerebrovascular function and remodeling and restores reduced cerebral perfusion in Type 2 diabetes. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol. 2016; 311: R466–R477.
Matsunaga Y, Negishi T, Hatakeyama A, Kawagoe Y, Sawano E, Tashiro T. Impairment of synaptic development in the hippocampus of diabetic Goto-Kakizaki rats. Int J Dev Neurosci. 2016; 53: 58–67.
Jenkins TC, Cartwright JP. The electroretinogram in minimal diabetic retinopathy. The Br J Ophthalmol. 1990; 74: 681–684.
Moore-Dotson JM, Beckman JJ, Mazade RE, et al. Early retinal neuronal dysfunction in diabetic mice: reduced light-evoked inhibition increases rod pathway signaling. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2016; 57: 1418–1430.
Kowluru RA, Mishra M, Kowluru A, Kumar B. Hyperlipidemia and the development of diabetic retinopathy: comparison between type 1 and type 2 animal models. Metabolism. 2016; 65: 1570–1581.
Kawano K. OLETF rats: model for the metabolic syndrome and diabetic nephropathy in humans. In: Shafrir E, ed. Animal Models of Diabetes. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press; 2007: 209–221.
Zhang M, Lv XY, Li J, Xu ZG, Chen L. The characterization of high-fat diet and multiple low-dose streptozotocin induced type 2 diabetes rat model. Exp Diabetes Res. 2008; 2008: 704045.
Al Kury L, Smail M, Qureshi MA, et al. Calcium signaling in the ventricular myocardium of the Goto-Kakizaki type 2 diabetic rat. J Diabetes Res. 2018; 2018: 2974304.
Sarkozy M, Szucs G, Fekete V, et al. Transcriptomic alterations in the heart of non-obese type 2 diabetic Goto-Kakizaki rats. Cardiovasc Diabetol. 2016; 15: 110.
Liepinsh E, Vilskersts R, Zvejniece L, et al. Protective effects of mildronate in an experimental model of type 2 diabetes in Goto-Kakizaki rats. Br J Pharmacol. 2009; 157: 1549–1556.
Picatoste B, Ramirez E, Caro-Vadillo A, et al. Sitagliptin reduces cardiac apoptosis, hypertrophy and fibrosis primarily by insulin-dependent mechanisms in experimental type-II diabetes. Potential roles of GLP-1 isoforms. PLoS One. 2013; 8: e78330.
Charles River. Zucker Diabetic Fatty Rat. Available at: https://www.criver.com/products-services/find-model/zdf-rat?region=3611.
Figure 1
 
Experimental timeline and appearance of functional changes. The first observable changes in the GK rat after hyperglycemia are retinal function deficits (4 weeks) with deficits in exploratory behavior, cognitive function, and retinal structure occurring at 6 weeks, and motor coordination deficits and vascular pathology not occurring during the 8-week testing period.
Figure 1
 
Experimental timeline and appearance of functional changes. The first observable changes in the GK rat after hyperglycemia are retinal function deficits (4 weeks) with deficits in exploratory behavior, cognitive function, and retinal structure occurring at 6 weeks, and motor coordination deficits and vascular pathology not occurring during the 8-week testing period.
Figure 2
 
GK rats showed reduced glucose tolerance and body weight when compared with nondiabetic controls. (A) Weight (g) at 4 to 8 weeks of age. (B) Average blood glucose (mg/dL) at 0, 15, 30, 60, and 120 minutes post–glucose injection for GTT performed at 4 weeks of age. (C) Average blood glucose for all 5 timepoints (0, 15, 30, 60, 120 minutes) of GTTs for 3, 4, and 8 weeks of age. GK values were normalized to Wistar values at each age. Black asterisks indicate comparisons between GK and Wistar groups. Red asterisks indicate comparisons between GK groups at different ages. *P < 0.05, ***P < 0.001. Results expressed as means ± SEM.
Figure 2
 
GK rats showed reduced glucose tolerance and body weight when compared with nondiabetic controls. (A) Weight (g) at 4 to 8 weeks of age. (B) Average blood glucose (mg/dL) at 0, 15, 30, 60, and 120 minutes post–glucose injection for GTT performed at 4 weeks of age. (C) Average blood glucose for all 5 timepoints (0, 15, 30, 60, 120 minutes) of GTTs for 3, 4, and 8 weeks of age. GK values were normalized to Wistar values at each age. Black asterisks indicate comparisons between GK and Wistar groups. Red asterisks indicate comparisons between GK groups at different ages. *P < 0.05, ***P < 0.001. Results expressed as means ± SEM.
Figure 3
 
GK rats showed increased a-wave and b-wave amplitudes that approached control levels over time. (A) Representative waveforms for dark-adapted ERG (0.7 log cd s/m2). Quantification of dark-adapted a-wave (B) and b-wave amplitudes (C) at 4 weeks of age. Average summed ERG amplitudes normalized to controls for 4 and 8 weeks of age for a-wave (D) and b-wave (E). Black asterisks indicate comparisons between GK and Wistar groups. Red asterisks indicate comparisons between GK groups at different ages. *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001. Results expressed as means ± SEM.
Figure 3
 
GK rats showed increased a-wave and b-wave amplitudes that approached control levels over time. (A) Representative waveforms for dark-adapted ERG (0.7 log cd s/m2). Quantification of dark-adapted a-wave (B) and b-wave amplitudes (C) at 4 weeks of age. Average summed ERG amplitudes normalized to controls for 4 and 8 weeks of age for a-wave (D) and b-wave (E). Black asterisks indicate comparisons between GK and Wistar groups. Red asterisks indicate comparisons between GK groups at different ages. *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001. Results expressed as means ± SEM.
Figure 4
 
GK rats showed increased OP and flicker amplitudes that decrease over time and delayed OP and flicker implicit times that persist to 8 weeks. Representative waveforms for dark-adapted OPs (0.7 log cd s/m2) (A) and light-adapted flicker ERG (B). Quantification of OP (C) and flicker (D) amplitudes and OP (E) and flicker (F) implicit times at 4 weeks of age. Average ERG values normalized to controls for 4 and 8 weeks of age for OP amplitudes (0.7 log cd s/m2) (G) and flicker implicit times (H). Black asterisks indicate comparisons between GK and Wistar groups. Red asterisks indicate comparisons between GK groups at different ages. *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001. Results expressed as means ± SEM.
Figure 4
 
GK rats showed increased OP and flicker amplitudes that decrease over time and delayed OP and flicker implicit times that persist to 8 weeks. Representative waveforms for dark-adapted OPs (0.7 log cd s/m2) (A) and light-adapted flicker ERG (B). Quantification of OP (C) and flicker (D) amplitudes and OP (E) and flicker (F) implicit times at 4 weeks of age. Average ERG values normalized to controls for 4 and 8 weeks of age for OP amplitudes (0.7 log cd s/m2) (G) and flicker implicit times (H). Black asterisks indicate comparisons between GK and Wistar groups. Red asterisks indicate comparisons between GK groups at different ages. *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001. Results expressed as means ± SEM.
Figure 5
 
GK rats had thinner retinal nerve fiber layers and total retinal thicknesses. Representative SD-OCT images from Wistar controls (A) and GK rats (B) at 6 weeks of age. Total retinal thickness was measured from the bottom of the RPE to the top of the RNFL. (C, D) Mean thicknesses for RNFL (C) and total thickness (D) for GK and Wistar rats for all four quadrants (Sup, Superior; Temp, Temporal; Inf, Inferior; Nas, Nasal) of the peripheral retina. **P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001. Results expressed as means ± SEM. RNFL, retinal nerve fiber layer; IPL, inner plexiform layer; INL, inner nuclear layer; OPL, outer plexiform layer; ONL, outer nuclear layer; ELM, external limiting membrane; IS/OS, inner segment/outer segment layer.
Figure 5
 
GK rats had thinner retinal nerve fiber layers and total retinal thicknesses. Representative SD-OCT images from Wistar controls (A) and GK rats (B) at 6 weeks of age. Total retinal thickness was measured from the bottom of the RPE to the top of the RNFL. (C, D) Mean thicknesses for RNFL (C) and total thickness (D) for GK and Wistar rats for all four quadrants (Sup, Superior; Temp, Temporal; Inf, Inferior; Nas, Nasal) of the peripheral retina. **P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001. Results expressed as means ± SEM. RNFL, retinal nerve fiber layer; IPL, inner plexiform layer; INL, inner nuclear layer; OPL, outer plexiform layer; ONL, outer nuclear layer; ELM, external limiting membrane; IS/OS, inner segment/outer segment layer.
Figure 6
 
Cognitive and exploratory behavior, but not motor, deficits in GK rats. (A) Cognitive function at 4 to 8 weeks of age as measured by spontaneous alternation on Y-maze. (B) Exploratory behavior at 4 to 8 weeks of age as measured by number of entries on Y-maze. (C) Motor coordination as measured by rotarod latency. *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001. Results expressed as means ± SEM.
Figure 6
 
Cognitive and exploratory behavior, but not motor, deficits in GK rats. (A) Cognitive function at 4 to 8 weeks of age as measured by spontaneous alternation on Y-maze. (B) Exploratory behavior at 4 to 8 weeks of age as measured by number of entries on Y-maze. (C) Motor coordination as measured by rotarod latency. *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001. Results expressed as means ± SEM.
Figure 7
 
Sex differences in GK rats. A comparison of performance on behavioral and functional tests for female and male GK rats versus Wistar controls (sexes combined). GK values were normalized to control values for each test. Flicker, flicker implicit time; Exp, exploratory. *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001. Results expressed as means ± SEM.
Figure 7
 
Sex differences in GK rats. A comparison of performance on behavioral and functional tests for female and male GK rats versus Wistar controls (sexes combined). GK values were normalized to control values for each test. Flicker, flicker implicit time; Exp, exploratory. *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001. Results expressed as means ± SEM.
Figure 8
 
A comparison of type II GK rats versus type I STZ-induced rats. GK rats and Wistar controls when compared with STZ-treated rats and nondiabetic Long Evans controls for (A) ERG b-wave amplitude, (B) oscillatory potential (OP3) implicit times at flash intensity −1.9 log cd s/m2, (C) spontaneous alternation on Y-maze, and (D) exploratory behavior on Y-maze. STZ model results are repurposed from data presented in Allen et al.40 *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001. Results expressed as means ± SEM. For (A), black asterisks indicate comparisons between GK and all other groups; gray asterisks indicate comparisons between diabetic and both control groups.
Figure 8
 
A comparison of type II GK rats versus type I STZ-induced rats. GK rats and Wistar controls when compared with STZ-treated rats and nondiabetic Long Evans controls for (A) ERG b-wave amplitude, (B) oscillatory potential (OP3) implicit times at flash intensity −1.9 log cd s/m2, (C) spontaneous alternation on Y-maze, and (D) exploratory behavior on Y-maze. STZ model results are repurposed from data presented in Allen et al.40 *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001. Results expressed as means ± SEM. For (A), black asterisks indicate comparisons between GK and all other groups; gray asterisks indicate comparisons between diabetic and both control groups.
×
×

This PDF is available to Subscribers Only

Sign in or purchase a subscription to access this content. ×

You must be signed into an individual account to use this feature.

×