Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science Cover Image for Volume 60, Issue 1
January 2019
Volume 60, Issue 1
Open Access
Glaucoma  |   January 2019
An Electrophysiological Comparison of Contrast Response Functions in Younger and Older Adults, and Those With Glaucoma
Author Affiliations & Notes
  • Jia Jia Lek
    Department of Optometry & Vision Sciences, The University of Melbourne, Parkville, Victoria, Australia
  • Bao N. Nguyen
    Department of Optometry & Vision Sciences, The University of Melbourne, Parkville, Victoria, Australia
  • Allison M. McKendrick
    Department of Optometry & Vision Sciences, The University of Melbourne, Parkville, Victoria, Australia
  • Algis J. Vingrys
    Department of Optometry & Vision Sciences, The University of Melbourne, Parkville, Victoria, Australia
  • Correspondence: Jia Jia Lek, Department of Optometry & Vision Sciences, The University of Melbourne, Level 4 Alice Hoy Building, Monash Road, Parkville, VIC 3010, Australia; [email protected]
Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science January 2019, Vol.60, 442-450. doi:https://doi.org/10.1167/iovs.17-23522
  • Views
  • PDF
  • Share
  • Tools
    • Alerts
      ×
      This feature is available to authenticated users only.
      Sign In or Create an Account ×
    • Get Citation

      Jia Jia Lek, Bao N. Nguyen, Allison M. McKendrick, Algis J. Vingrys; An Electrophysiological Comparison of Contrast Response Functions in Younger and Older Adults, and Those With Glaucoma. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 2019;60(1):442-450. https://doi.org/10.1167/iovs.17-23522.

      Download citation file:


      © ARVO (1962-2015); The Authors (2016-present)

      ×
  • Supplements
Abstract

Purpose: Aging and glaucoma both result in contrast processing deficits. However, it is unclear the extent to which these functional deficits arise from retinal or post-retinal neuronal changes. This study aims to disentangle the effects of healthy human aging and glaucoma on retinal and post-retinal contrast processing using visual electrophysiology.

Methods: Steady-state pattern electroretinograms (PERG) and pattern visual evoked potentials (PVEP) were simultaneously recorded across a range of contrasts (0%, 4%, 9%, 18%, 39%, 73%, 97%; 0.8° diameter checks, 31° diameter checkerboard) in 13 glaucoma patients (67 ± 6 years), 15 older (63 ± 8 years) and 14 younger adults (27 ± 3 years). PERG and PVEP contrast response functions were fit with a linear and saturating hyperbolic model, respectively. PERG and PVEP magnitude, timing (phase), and model fit parameters (slope, semi-saturation constant) were compared between groups.

Results: PERG responses were reduced and delayed in older adults relative to younger adults, and further reduced and delayed in glaucoma patients across all contrasts. PVEP signals were also reduced and delayed in glaucoma patients, relative to age-similar (older) controls. However, despite having reduced PERG magnitudes, older adults did not demonstrate reduced PVEP magnitudes.

Conclusions: Older adults with healthy vision demonstrate reduced magnitude and delayed timing in the PERG that is not reflected in the PVEP. In contrast, glaucoma produces functional deficits in both PERG and PVEP contrast response functions. Our results suggest that glaucomatous effects on contrast processing are not a simple extension of those that arise as part of the aging process.

Visual cortical neurons predominantly respond to contrast information rather than absolute luminance within the prevailing visual environment. In primary visual cortex (V1), single neurons typically—but not all—show a saturating response to increasing contrast.1,2 That is, as stimulus contrast increases, the response produced by a given amount of contrast increases monotonically until it reaches a plateau. Varying response magnitudes to different contrast levels is a feature found in both cortical (V1) and pre-cortical (retinal and lateral geniculate nucleus, LGN) cells of the visual system.36 
Although primarily a retinal ganglion cell disease,7,8 the neurodegenerative changes in glaucoma extend to post-retinal structures including the LGN9,10 and visual cortex.1113 Accordingly, functional visual processing deficits in glaucoma may arise from dysfunction of retinal and/or post-retinal structures. While it is well established that contrast sensitivity is reduced in people with glaucoma (most typically measured across the visual field using perimetry), perceptual measures of contrast gain signatures are also altered by glaucoma.1416 Although these contrast processing deficits have been measured using behavioral methods in patients with glaucoma, such perceptual exploration is very limited in the ability to disentangle whether such deficits arise from retinal and/or post-retinal sources. 
In this study, we used pattern electrophysiology to investigate retinal and post-retinal contrast processing deficits in early glaucoma. Pattern electrophysiology provides an indirect measure of the neuronal response to contrast, with the pattern electroretinogram (PERG) providing a measure of retinal ganglion cell function17 and the pattern visual evoked potential (PVEP) providing a representation of pooled responses from visual cortical neurons, including at the primary visual cortex, V1.18 Previous studies in glaucoma patients report reduced PERG and PVEP responses to high contrast stimuli (>70%).1921 Given that natural vision requires interpretation of environments with a wide range of contrasts, we simultaneously measured PERG and PVEP responses from low to high contrast instead of measuring responses to a single contrast level. Previous electrophysiological research measuring responses elicited by a range of stimulus contrasts reports flatter PVEP contrast response functions in those with glaucoma,22 although concurrently measured PERG data is lacking. 
A secondary aim of our study was to compare PERG and PVEP contrast response functions between older and younger adults with healthy vision. Aging is an established risk factor for glaucoma.23 Studies in older adults have shown that aging can result in neurological alterations at the retina including the loss of retinal ganglion cells,24,25 as well as at the LGN26 and visual cortex,27 the same sites that undergo neurodegeneration in glaucoma. Healthy aging alters a range of perceptual contrast effects, including contrast detection,28,29 contrast discrimination,28 spatial contrast suppression30,31 and contrast gain signatures.32 Such perceptual effects are consistent with changes to both structural and neurochemical changes in the senescent visual system (for review, see McKendrick et al.33) In many ophthalmic conditions, the effects of aging and the effects of disease appear along a continuum. For example, the retinal nerve fiber layer thins somewhat with aging but is pathological with glaucoma; lenticular changes arise in most older adults but are pathological with cataracts. Such observations may lead to the expectation that any observed alterations to contrast response functions with age and those with glaucoma should exist on a continuum of change from those of younger adults. However, given the complexity of cortical visual neural circuitry involving substantive feedforward, feedback, and lateral connectivity (for reviews, see for example34,35) and the fact that aging appears to alter the cortical neurochemical environment that influences visual perception (rather than just structural change),36 it is not necessarily clear that the effects of aging on the contrast response are similar in form to those of glaucoma. Our results demonstrate that the characteristics of altered retinal and post-retinal contrast response functions that arise from aging can be distinguished from those of glaucoma, revealing that glaucomatous post-retinal deficits are not simply an extension of aging changes. 
Methods
Participants
All participants provided written informed consent in accordance with the University of Melbourne Human Research Ethics Committee approved protocol and all procedures complied with the tenets of the Declaration of Helsinki. 
No previous studies have investigated the effects of aging and glaucoma on PERG and PVEP contrast response functions. Consequently, our a priori power analysis was based on previous literature comparing electrophysiological responses in those with glaucoma to age-matched controls. Specifically, we utilized simultaneous PERG and PVEP data from Parisi's study19 at high contrast (80%) in glaucoma patients with mild visual field loss (Humphrey visual field analyzer mean deviation: −1.50 to −6.00 dB) and age-similar controls. Five participants in each group provided a power of 80% for detecting a significant reduction in PERG magnitude, while 9 participants in each group provided the same power for detecting a significant reduction in PVEP magnitude (alpha = 0.05). Hence, for the current study, we recruited at least 12 participants in each group to achieve a minimum power of 90% for establishing a difference at higher contrast levels between groups. 
Participants were recruited via written advertisements placed in community newspapers and electronic newsletters associated with the University of Melbourne. In addition, people with glaucoma were recruited via Glaucoma Australia, Royal Victorian Eye and Ear Hospital, or the Melbourne Optometry Clinic (Australian College of Optometry). Fourteen young adults aged 22 to 31 years (mean ± standard deviation: 27 ± 3 years), 15 older adults aged 49 to 76 years (63 ± 8 years), and 13 people with early glaucoma (11 with primary open-angle glaucoma and 2 with normal-tension glaucoma) aged 58 to 77 (67 ± 6 years) participated. Details of the glaucoma participants are provided in the Table
Table
 
Characteristics of Participants With Glaucoma: ID Number, Age, Diagnosis (POAG: Primary Open Angle Glaucoma, NTG: Normal Tension Glaucoma), Moorfields Regression Analysis (MRA), Glaucoma Probability Score (GPS) Tool of the Heidelberg Retinal Tomograph II, and Average Defect (AD), Pattern Defect (PD) on the Central Test of the Medmont Perimeter
Table
 
Characteristics of Participants With Glaucoma: ID Number, Age, Diagnosis (POAG: Primary Open Angle Glaucoma, NTG: Normal Tension Glaucoma), Moorfields Regression Analysis (MRA), Glaucoma Probability Score (GPS) Tool of the Heidelberg Retinal Tomograph II, and Average Defect (AD), Pattern Defect (PD) on the Central Test of the Medmont Perimeter
All participants underwent a comprehensive eye examination, including slit-lamp biomicroscopy, applanation tonometry, ophthalmoscopy, optic nerve head imaging with the Heidelberg Retinal Tomograph II (HRT; Heidelberg Engineering, Heidelberg, Germany) and visual field testing with the Medmont perimeter (Medmont Pty. Ltd., Camberwell, Vic, Australia). Inclusion criteria were: visual acuity of 6/7.5 (20/25) or better, and distance refractive errors of no more than ±6.00 D sphere and/or −2.00 D cylinder in the tested eye, no systemic or ophthalmological disease (other than glaucoma for glaucoma participants), and no medications known to affect vision. Non-glaucoma participants were required to have (1) optic nerve head imaging parameters within the one-tailed 95% range of the normative database of the Moorfields Regression Analysis (MRA) or Glaucoma Probability Score (GPS) tool, and (2) normal visual field results (Medmont Central test) as indicated by the average defect (AD) and pattern defect (PD) indices (within the one-tailed 95% range of the perimeter's normative database). 
Participants with glaucoma had an ophthalmologic diagnosis consistent with glaucoma and were flagged as failing the visual field test (AD or PD ≤ 5% probability of falling within the normative database) or optic nerve head imaging assessment (MRA or GPS ≤ 5% of falling within the normative database), as shown in the Table. Glaucoma patients were using their current medications as prescribed by their ophthalmologist. Exclusion criteria were: a history of migraine, intra-ocular pressures of >21 mm Hg (for non-glaucoma participants), significant age-related lens changes (>NC 1.5 as classified using the LOCS III37), and unreliable visual field results (reliability indices ≥20%). 
Electrophysiology Recordings
The PERG and PVEP were recorded simultaneously while participants viewed a black-and-white square-wave checkerboard generated on a gamma-corrected 21-inch CRT monitor (frame rate: 100 Hz; resolution: 1024 × 768 pixels; Trinitron G520; Sony Corp., Tokyo, Japan) by an Espion Electrodiagnostics system (Version V5; Diagnosys LLC, Cambridge, UK). Observers fixated centrally on a 0.5° diameter red square, wearing an appropriate refractive correction for the working distance (50 cm). A translucent occluder was used to cover the non-tested eye to minimize the effects of luminance adaptation during testing.38 Observers were allowed to blink freely during the session and take breaks as required. Ocular lubricants were used for comfort when required. 
The checkerboard comprised 0.8° checks, subtending 31° × 31° in total. The checkerboard reversed every 60 msecs (16.7 reversals/sec) with a square wave profile and one complete cycle (change from light to dark) occurring every 120 msecs (8.3 Hz, steady-state). The chosen check size39,40 and reversal rate41,42 are within the optimal range for detecting retinal ganglion cell dysfunction in glaucoma. To minimize adaptation effects, the six contrast levels (Michelson contrast: 4%, 9%, 18%, 39%, 73%, 97%) were presented from low to high contrast, and a spatially homogenous gray stimulus (0% contrast, mean luminance: 52 cd/m2) preceded each change in contrast for approximately 1 minute (4 batches of 25 sweeps), which was used to define noise.43 Each contrast level was sampled over a 480 msecs epoch (4 cycles) with signals collected in 8 batches of 25 sweeps (200 trials in total). 
The PERG was recorded with corneal DTL carbon-fiber electrodes (Roland Consult, Brandenburg, Germany) placed near the lower limbus and referenced to an inactive silver-silver chloride electrode (Viasys Healthcare, Madison, WI, USA) at the ipsilateral canthus. Gold cup electrodes (Grass Technologies, West Warwick, RI, USA) were used for PVEP recordings. Three were placed on the head at Oz (10% of the nasion-inion distance above the inion), and at 5% nasion-inion distance above and below Oz, since previous studies have shown that the location of the calcarine fissure relative to the inion can vary between individuals.44,45 Therefore, to take into account possible inter-individual variability due to anatomical variability, the largest response returned from the three locations was taken as the PVEP signal. The reference electrode was located at Fz (30% nasion-inion distance above the nasion) and the ground at Cz (halfway between the nasion and inion). Electrode impedance was kept below 5kΩ. Signals were amplified (100 times), bandpass-filtered (1.25 to 100 Hz) and digitized (1000 Hz) to 16-bit resolution. Blink artefacts (± 50 μV) were rejected post-hoc. 
Data Analysis
Analysis of the contrast signal for all contrast levels in the second harmonic of the recordings (2F = 16.7 Hz, i.e., twice the stimulation frequency) was achieved offline using Microsoft Excel (Microsoft, Redmond, WA, USA). The time series data (480 ms) was resampled to yield 512 data points prior to a Discrete Fourier Transform.46 The 2F PERG and PVEP magnitude in the Fourier domain was returned. The 2F phase was adjusted with an additive constant of 2π radians and bound within ±1π radians to avoid continuity. A decrease in phase of 0.1π radian corresponds to a 3 msecs signal delay in the time domain. 
Figure 1 shows examples of PERG and PVEP waveforms obtained at 97% contrast for a younger and older healthy observer, and an observer with glaucoma (G12). Noise was returned as the upper 95% confidence interval of the 0% contrast stimulus (blank screen) that preceded signal collection for each group. As PERG magnitudes for low contrast stimuli often fell within the 95% confidence interval of noise, we conservatively excluded PERG data for 4%, 9%, and 18% from statistical analysis to ensure that noise signals were not mistakenly being interpreted as containing meaningful signal. All contrast levels for PVEP were outside the 95% confidence interval of noise. 
Figure 1
 
Representative PERG and PVEP steady-state waveforms in response to a 97% contrast stimulus recorded from (A) a healthy younger observer (31 years old), (B) a healthy older observer (57 years old), and (C) an observer with glaucoma (G12, Table, 61 years old). The magnitude of the 2F (16 Hz) component is shown in the top right hand corner of each Fourier domain panel.
Figure 1
 
Representative PERG and PVEP steady-state waveforms in response to a 97% contrast stimulus recorded from (A) a healthy younger observer (31 years old), (B) a healthy older observer (57 years old), and (C) an observer with glaucoma (G12, Table, 61 years old). The magnitude of the 2F (16 Hz) component is shown in the top right hand corner of each Fourier domain panel.
Statistical analysis was carried out using SPSS (ver. 20; SPSS, Inc., Chicago, IL, USA). Data were tested for normality using the Kolmogrov-Smirnov test (P < 0.05). Mann–Whitney U tests were used as appropriate. When using independent t-tests, Levene's test was applied to check for equality of variances. To investigate the effects of aging, the healthy younger and older groups were compared, whereas the older and glaucoma age-similar observers were compared to evaluate the effect of glaucoma. Groups were compared by mixed repeated-measures ANOVA (mixed RM-ANOVA), where the between-subjects factor was group (Aging: young, older; Glaucoma: older, glaucoma) and the within-subjects factor was contrast (PERG: 39%, 73%, 97%; PVEP: 4%, 9%. 18%, 39%, 73%, 97%). Where data violated the sphericity assumption of repeated measures (Mauchly's test of sphericity), a Greenhouse-Geisser correction was applied. Results were considered statistically significant if P < 0.05. 
Contrast Response Function Modeling
Individual data were modeled in Microsoft Excel to characterize the slope of the contrast response functions using a linear function (Equation 1) for PERG data:47,48  
\(\def\upalpha{\unicode[Times]{x3B1}}\)\(\def\upbeta{\unicode[Times]{x3B2}}\)\(\def\upgamma{\unicode[Times]{x3B3}}\)\(\def\updelta{\unicode[Times]{x3B4}}\)\(\def\upvarepsilon{\unicode[Times]{x3B5}}\)\(\def\upzeta{\unicode[Times]{x3B6}}\)\(\def\upeta{\unicode[Times]{x3B7}}\)\(\def\uptheta{\unicode[Times]{x3B8}}\)\(\def\upiota{\unicode[Times]{x3B9}}\)\(\def\upkappa{\unicode[Times]{x3BA}}\)\(\def\uplambda{\unicode[Times]{x3BB}}\)\(\def\upmu{\unicode[Times]{x3BC}}\)\(\def\upnu{\unicode[Times]{x3BD}}\)\(\def\upxi{\unicode[Times]{x3BE}}\)\(\def\upomicron{\unicode[Times]{x3BF}}\)\(\def\uppi{\unicode[Times]{x3C0}}\)\(\def\uprho{\unicode[Times]{x3C1}}\)\(\def\upsigma{\unicode[Times]{x3C3}}\)\(\def\uptau{\unicode[Times]{x3C4}}\)\(\def\upupsilon{\unicode[Times]{x3C5}}\)\(\def\upphi{\unicode[Times]{x3C6}}\)\(\def\upchi{\unicode[Times]{x3C7}}\)\(\def\uppsy{\unicode[Times]{x3C8}}\)\(\def\upomega{\unicode[Times]{x3C9}}\)\(\def\bialpha{\boldsymbol{\alpha}}\)\(\def\bibeta{\boldsymbol{\beta}}\)\(\def\bigamma{\boldsymbol{\gamma}}\)\(\def\bidelta{\boldsymbol{\delta}}\)\(\def\bivarepsilon{\boldsymbol{\varepsilon}}\)\(\def\bizeta{\boldsymbol{\zeta}}\)\(\def\bieta{\boldsymbol{\eta}}\)\(\def\bitheta{\boldsymbol{\theta}}\)\(\def\biiota{\boldsymbol{\iota}}\)\(\def\bikappa{\boldsymbol{\kappa}}\)\(\def\bilambda{\boldsymbol{\lambda}}\)\(\def\bimu{\boldsymbol{\mu}}\)\(\def\binu{\boldsymbol{\nu}}\)\(\def\bixi{\boldsymbol{\xi}}\)\(\def\biomicron{\boldsymbol{\micron}}\)\(\def\bipi{\boldsymbol{\pi}}\)\(\def\birho{\boldsymbol{\rho}}\)\(\def\bisigma{\boldsymbol{\sigma}}\)\(\def\bitau{\boldsymbol{\tau}}\)\(\def\biupsilon{\boldsymbol{\upsilon}}\)\(\def\biphi{\boldsymbol{\phi}}\)\(\def\bichi{\boldsymbol{\chi}}\)\(\def\bipsy{\boldsymbol{\psy}}\)\(\def\biomega{\boldsymbol{\omega}}\)\(\def\bupalpha{\unicode[Times]{x1D6C2}}\)\(\def\bupbeta{\unicode[Times]{x1D6C3}}\)\(\def\bupgamma{\unicode[Times]{x1D6C4}}\)\(\def\bupdelta{\unicode[Times]{x1D6C5}}\)\(\def\bupepsilon{\unicode[Times]{x1D6C6}}\)\(\def\bupvarepsilon{\unicode[Times]{x1D6DC}}\)\(\def\bupzeta{\unicode[Times]{x1D6C7}}\)\(\def\bupeta{\unicode[Times]{x1D6C8}}\)\(\def\buptheta{\unicode[Times]{x1D6C9}}\)\(\def\bupiota{\unicode[Times]{x1D6CA}}\)\(\def\bupkappa{\unicode[Times]{x1D6CB}}\)\(\def\buplambda{\unicode[Times]{x1D6CC}}\)\(\def\bupmu{\unicode[Times]{x1D6CD}}\)\(\def\bupnu{\unicode[Times]{x1D6CE}}\)\(\def\bupxi{\unicode[Times]{x1D6CF}}\)\(\def\bupomicron{\unicode[Times]{x1D6D0}}\)\(\def\buppi{\unicode[Times]{x1D6D1}}\)\(\def\buprho{\unicode[Times]{x1D6D2}}\)\(\def\bupsigma{\unicode[Times]{x1D6D4}}\)\(\def\buptau{\unicode[Times]{x1D6D5}}\)\(\def\bupupsilon{\unicode[Times]{x1D6D6}}\)\(\def\bupphi{\unicode[Times]{x1D6D7}}\)\(\def\bupchi{\unicode[Times]{x1D6D8}}\)\(\def\buppsy{\unicode[Times]{x1D6D9}}\)\(\def\bupomega{\unicode[Times]{x1D6DA}}\)\(\def\bupvartheta{\unicode[Times]{x1D6DD}}\)\(\def\bGamma{\bf{\Gamma}}\)\(\def\bDelta{\bf{\Delta}}\)\(\def\bTheta{\bf{\Theta}}\)\(\def\bLambda{\bf{\Lambda}}\)\(\def\bXi{\bf{\Xi}}\)\(\def\bPi{\bf{\Pi}}\)\(\def\bSigma{\bf{\Sigma}}\)\(\def\bUpsilon{\bf{\Upsilon}}\)\(\def\bPhi{\bf{\Phi}}\)\(\def\bPsi{\bf{\Psi}}\)\(\def\bOmega{\bf{\Omega}}\)\(\def\iGamma{\unicode[Times]{x1D6E4}}\)\(\def\iDelta{\unicode[Times]{x1D6E5}}\)\(\def\iTheta{\unicode[Times]{x1D6E9}}\)\(\def\iLambda{\unicode[Times]{x1D6EC}}\)\(\def\iXi{\unicode[Times]{x1D6EF}}\)\(\def\iPi{\unicode[Times]{x1D6F1}}\)\(\def\iSigma{\unicode[Times]{x1D6F4}}\)\(\def\iUpsilon{\unicode[Times]{x1D6F6}}\)\(\def\iPhi{\unicode[Times]{x1D6F7}}\)\(\def\iPsi{\unicode[Times]{x1D6F9}}\)\(\def\iOmega{\unicode[Times]{x1D6FA}}\)\(\def\biGamma{\unicode[Times]{x1D71E}}\)\(\def\biDelta{\unicode[Times]{x1D71F}}\)\(\def\biTheta{\unicode[Times]{x1D723}}\)\(\def\biLambda{\unicode[Times]{x1D726}}\)\(\def\biXi{\unicode[Times]{x1D729}}\)\(\def\biPi{\unicode[Times]{x1D72B}}\)\(\def\biSigma{\unicode[Times]{x1D72E}}\)\(\def\biUpsilon{\unicode[Times]{x1D730}}\)\(\def\biPhi{\unicode[Times]{x1D731}}\)\(\def\biPsi{\unicode[Times]{x1D733}}\)\(\def\biOmega{\unicode[Times]{x1D734}}\)\begin{equation}\tag{1}y(c) = mc + a\end{equation}
where y(c) is PERG magnitude at a specific contrast, m represents the steepness of the function (slope), c is the contrast level, and a is the noise value of y when c = 0. Parameters m, and a were allowed to float during the optimization process.  
PVEP data were fit with a monotonic saturating hyperbolic (Equation 249), which best fits the non-linear contrast response functions of neurons in primary visual cortex, V1:1,50  
\begin{equation}\tag{2}R\left( c \right) = {R_{max}} \times {{{C^n}} \over {{C^n} + C_{50}^n}} + {R_0}\end{equation}
where R(c) is the PVEP magnitude at a specific contrast, R0 is the noise, Rmax is the maximum saturated response, C50 is the contrast at which the response reaches halfway between baseline and maximum, n represents the exponent that determines the slope, and C is the contrast of the stimulus. Only n and C50 were allowed to float during the optimization process. R0 was fixed to the noise magnitude at 0% contrast, and Rmax was fixed to the average magnitude of 73% and 97% contrasts.43,51  
Results
PERG and PVEP Contrast Response Functions With Age
Noise levels at 0% contrast did not differ between age groups for both the PERG (t(27) = −0.68, P = 0.50) and PVEP (t(27) = −0.80, P = 0.43). Figure 2A illustrates the group mean PERG data for the six contrast levels tested. Using Equation 1, all participants showed the expected linear increase in PERG magnitude with increasing contrast, with the largest response observed at 97% contrast for both groups. Overall, raw PERG magnitudes were significantly reduced in older adults relative to younger adults (RM-ANOVA main effect of group: P = 0.001). A significant interaction (P = 0.02) between group and contrast was found, indicating that PERG slope differed with age. The slope of the older group was significantly flatter (t(27) = 2.51, P = 0.02) relative to the younger group (Fig. 3A). Figure 2E shows that when the raw PERG data of the older adults were normalized to the average PERG of the young group ([individual magnitude − younger group mean magnitude] / younger group mean magnitude), aging resulted in a significant reduction in PERG magnitudes (mean ± 95% CI = −29.48 ± 13.09%) (RM-ANOVA main effect of group; P < 0.001). 
Figure 2
 
Group average retinal (A), (C) and cortical (B), (D) responses with aging and with glaucoma. Group mean (±95% confidence interval of the mean) PERG and PVEP magnitude (A), (B), phase (C), (D) as a function of contrast in younger (circles), older (squares) and glaucoma (triangles) observers. Shaded regions in (A) and (B) represent the upper 95% limits of noise levels as measured with 0% contrast, and in (C), data that have been excluded from statistical analysis due to lying in the region of noise. Relative change (mean ± 95% confidence interval) in PERG (filled symbols) and PVEP (unfilled symbols) with (E) aging and (F) with glaucoma. Gray symbols represent PERG signals that were in noisy regions. Data have been shifted horizontally for clarity.
Figure 2
 
Group average retinal (A), (C) and cortical (B), (D) responses with aging and with glaucoma. Group mean (±95% confidence interval of the mean) PERG and PVEP magnitude (A), (B), phase (C), (D) as a function of contrast in younger (circles), older (squares) and glaucoma (triangles) observers. Shaded regions in (A) and (B) represent the upper 95% limits of noise levels as measured with 0% contrast, and in (C), data that have been excluded from statistical analysis due to lying in the region of noise. Relative change (mean ± 95% confidence interval) in PERG (filled symbols) and PVEP (unfilled symbols) with (E) aging and (F) with glaucoma. Gray symbols represent PERG signals that were in noisy regions. Data have been shifted horizontally for clarity.
Figure 3
 
Group average for (A) PERG slope, and (B) PVEP slope, and (C) semi saturation constant (C50) of younger (circles), older (squares), and glaucoma (triangles) groups. Error bars are 95% confidence intervals of the mean. Note that in Figure 3B, the error bars are largest in the young group (filled circle) due to a single observer's data returning a slope (n) of 17.2 (median of young group = 0.99). The unfilled symbol next to the young group data shows the mean ± 95% confidence interval of the mean without this observer included in the group.
Figure 3
 
Group average for (A) PERG slope, and (B) PVEP slope, and (C) semi saturation constant (C50) of younger (circles), older (squares), and glaucoma (triangles) groups. Error bars are 95% confidence intervals of the mean. Note that in Figure 3B, the error bars are largest in the young group (filled circle) due to a single observer's data returning a slope (n) of 17.2 (median of young group = 0.99). The unfilled symbol next to the young group data shows the mean ± 95% confidence interval of the mean without this observer included in the group.
Figure 2B shows the PVEP contrast response functions and group mean data, indicating that, on average, PVEP magnitudes showed a monotonic saturating response in both younger and older adults. A significant interaction between group and contrast (F1.79,48.43 = 4.22, P = 0.02) was found, where the younger and older groups had similar magnitudes at the first two contrast levels (4%, 9%) followed by a trend for elevated magnitudes in the older group at higher (18%, 39%, 73%, 97%) contrasts. The individual model fits to Equation 2 (which assumes response saturation at high contrast) did not converge for one younger and one older participant, both showing lower PVEP magnitudes at high contrast levels (39%, 73%, 97%—also called “supersaturation,” a relatively uncommon but normal finding in a small proportion of healthy observers43). Consequently, for these individuals, it was not possible to obtain a sensible estimate of PVEP contrast response slope (n) and PVEP semi-saturation constant (C50). For the remainder of the participants, the model fit well and the slope (n) and semi-saturation constant (C50) of the PVEP contrast response functions were not significantly different between the younger and older groups (slope: Fig. 3B; P = 0.59; semi-saturation constant: Fig. 3C; P = 0.79). 
Although younger and older groups returned a similar PVEP slope, older adults showed a trend for larger PVEP magnitudes (Fig. 2B). This trend did not reach statistical significance (no main effect of group: F1,27 = 2.94, P = 0.10). Figure 2E shows the same data but with the older adult data normalized to the average of the younger group. Aging resulted in minimal relative change at low contrast levels (4%, 9%) but substantial elevation in PVEP magnitudes at higher contrasts (18%, 39%, 73%, 97%). This is confirmed by a statistically significant interaction between group (younger, older) and contrast (P = 0.004). 
In both young and older adults, PERG responses were similar in timing across mid to high contrast levels as shown in Figure 2C, while PVEP responses initially become faster with increasing contrast to reach a constant after approximately 20% contrast (Fig. 2D). Differences in signal phase between the younger and older groups with PERG (Fig. 2C) and PVEP (Fig. 2D) were consistent across contrast levels. This was confirmed by the absence of a significant interaction between group (younger, older) and contrast for both PERG (F1.6,41.9 = 0.30, P = 0.68) and PVEP (F2.34,63.06 = 0.79, P = 0.48) phase. The age related phase shift (F1,27 = 4.76, P = 0.04) translates to a significant delay (2.1 msecs) in the PERG response, and a further delay (10.8 msecs) of PVEP response (F1,27 = 8.56, P < 0.01). 
PERG and PVEP Contrast Response Functions With Glaucoma
Relative to age-similar healthy controls, glaucoma patients did not show a significant difference in noise of the PERG (t(26) = 1.33, P = 0.2) and PVEP (t(26) = 0.47, P = 0.65). Overall, PERG magnitudes were reduced in individuals with glaucoma (Fig. 2A; F1,26 = 23.31, P < 0.001), with differences between groups increasing with contrast as indicated by the significant interaction (Fig. 2F; F1.13,29.47 = 16.05, P < 0.001) between group and contrast. Retinal contrast response function slopes (m) were significantly flatter in patients with glaucoma relative to age-similar controls (Fig. 3A; t(26) = 4.06, P < 0.001). 
PVEP magnitudes of observers with glaucoma were also significantly reduced relative to controls as shown in Figure 2B (F1,26 = 9.24, P = 0.005). The interaction between group and contrast did not reach statistical significance (F1.71,44.40 = 3.22, P = 0.06), indicating that the reduction in PVEP magnitudes with glaucoma was similar across contrast levels. The cortical contrast response function slope (n) was similar between patients with early glaucoma and controls (Fig. 3B; t(25) = 0.30, P = 0.77). Semi-saturation (C50) was not statistically significantly different between patients with glaucoma and older adults as shown in Figure 3C (t(25) = −1.18, P = 0.25). 
Figure 2C shows no statistical difference in PERG phase between the older and glaucoma groups, indicating that glaucoma did not result in a change in timing (F1,26 = 0.21, P = 0.65). Figure 2D shows that although the glaucoma group demonstrated a trend toward slower PVEP responses relative to the older group, this difference was not statistically significant (F1,26 = 1.50, P = 0.23). There was no interaction between group and contrast for either PERG (F1.6,42.9 = 1.30, P = 0.27) or PVEP (F2.65,68.93 = 0.34, P = 0.77) phase responses. 
Figure 2F shows that when the glaucoma group was normalized to age-similar controls ([individual magnitude – older group mean magnitude] / older group mean magnitude), there was a significant relative reduction in PERG (mean ± 95% CI = −51.67 ± 10.27) (P <0.001) and PVEP magnitudes (mean ± 95% CI = −45.54 ± 19.94) (P < 0.01) that was not contrast dependent (no significant interaction with contrast: PERG: P = 0.38; PVEP: P = 0.85). 
Discussion
This study used simultaneously recorded PERG and PVEP for stimuli presented from low to high contrast to explore the nature of the contrast response function in younger and older adults, in addition to those with glaucoma. Specifically, we were interested in exploring whether the effects of glaucoma on the contrast response are similar but exaggerated versions of the impact of simple aging on contrast functions, at both the retinal and cortical levels. A simple prediction is that aging might result in a reduction of amplitude of response for both retinal and cortical recordings and that glaucoma would exacerbate the same. However, relative to younger adults, older adults showed reduced (∼29%) PERG magnitudes but elevated (∼57%) PVEP magnitudes across mid to high contrast levels. Glaucoma resulted in a reduction in both in PERG magnitudes (∼52%) and in PVEP magnitudes (∼43%). Both PERG and PVEP deficits with glaucoma were not contrast dependent. 
A reduced PERG response per unit contrast resulted in a flatter retinal contrast response function in older adults. This finding is consistent with reduced contrast gain signatures previously reported using perceptual methods.32 At high contrasts, the reduction in PERG magnitude (∼29%) with age found in this study is consistent with previous studies using high contrast stimuli.52,53 Age-related reductions in PERG magnitude have been attributed to retinal ganglion cell loss with age,24,54 although it is noted that the PERG can also be affected by optical degradation from senile miosis and age-related lens opacities.55 We measured PERG and PVEP responses according to International Society for Clinical Electrophysiology of Vision (ISCEV)56,57 standards where pupils are undilated. As we did not record the pupil size, we are unable to evaluate the influence of pupil size on these responses. However, we consider it unlikely that optical changes can completely account for the PERG magnitude reductions observed, given that the minimum age of the healthy “older” adults included in this study as age-similar controls for the glaucoma cohort was 49 years (i.e., not elderly). Moreover, all participants had normal visual acuity and no significant lens opacity, as determined during the screening eye examination. 
Despite significantly reduced relative PERG magnitudes, there was no evidence for older adults in this study to demonstrate reduced PVEP magnitudes (Fig. 2B). We acknowledge that previous reports find PVEP responses are reduced with aging (see, for example, a review by Tobimatsu58) and that PVEP responses can be highly variable between individuals, as evidenced in our data. In an attempt to reduce the inter-individual variability in PVEP magnitudes, we used the largest signal captured by one of three electrodes. We also investigated whether any systematic differences could be due to gender59; however, we did not find any differences between males and females (data not shown). Nevertheless, when normalized to the younger adult PVEP, there was a trend for the older adult PVEP magnitudes to be elevated for mid to high contrast levels (Fig. 2E). These results are relevant to the specific stimulus parameters we used (0.8° check size or 0.625 cyc/deg spatial frequency, 8Hz temporal frequency) and may not be not directly comparable to previous literature. 
Another difference worth noting between our study and previous electrophysiology literature is that the majority of previous simultaneous PVEP and PERG studies measure only a single contrast (typically high contrast >90% to evoke a strong retinal response), whereas we have measured responses to sequential patterns of increasing contrast. As noted by Nguyen et al.,43 this method may have produced differential contrast adaptation effects that could influence the recorded PVEP magnitude, despite our attempts to minimize adaptation by presenting stimuli from low to high contrast and introducing a gray (0% contrast) homogenous background in between each contrast level. Nevertheless, to offer a perspective of how our results compare to other accounts of aging effects on PVEP magnitude, at lower temporal frequencies (4–6 Hz), older adults demonstrate lower PVEP magnitudes but a trend for higher PVEP magnitudes with increasing temporal frequency (above 8–10 Hz)52,60 relative to younger adults. This trend for a “cross-over” effect is also seen when different spatial frequencies are tested, where aging produces a relative increase in PVEP magnitude for 1 cyc/deg and 3 cyc/deg spatial frequencies but not at 4 cyc/deg.61,62 Thus, our results add to the general finding that age-dependent effects on steady-state PVEP are highly stimulus dependent.58 
The precise mechanisms underpinning our finding of elevated PVEP magnitudes at mid to high contrast levels in older adults is unclear, and why there is an apparent compensation for some retinal reduction in signal amplitude cannot be ascertained by our electrophysiological methods. In interpreting changes to PVEP signals, it is important to keep in mind that PVEP depends on the integrity of the visual pathway from retina to cortex,57 other visual cortical areas,18 and feedback from extrastriate cortical areas to V1.63 Still, a prevailing theory in the past decade regarding the effects of aging on visual processing is that there is less inhibition in older adult visual cortices. A simple model of reduced inhibition could possibly be proposed to explain the increased magnitude of cortical responses to the large high contrast stimuli used in our experiments. This theory has arisen from neurophysiological studies where some V1 neurons of older primates and cats show less orientation selectivity and direction selectivity relative to those of younger animals.64,65 Furthermore, the orientation and direction selectivity of aged primate V1 neurons can, at least partially, be restored with local administration of inhibitory neurotransmitter agonists.66 However, there is no direct evidence in humans for this proposal. Indeed, a recent study has shown elevated, rather than reduced, levels of the inhibitory neurotransmitter GABA in the visual cortex of older people.36 Previous studies in patients with epilepsy51 and migraine43 have proposed that altered contrast response functions could be a result of an imbalance of excitatory/inhibitory processes, rather than a simple reduction in cortical inhibition. Additionally, as previously noted, care needs to be taken in interpreting PVEP signals as its contributions are not limited to V1.18,57,63 
In this study, glaucoma participants demonstrated flatter retinal contrast response functions, a finding consistent with elevated contrast discrimination thresholds14,28,67 and reduced contrast gain signatures found in perceptual studies.1416 The PERG magnitude loss (∼52%) in our early glaucoma cohort was of a similar extent as compared to previous studies,41,42,68,69 and is likely due to retinal ganglion cell loss7 or dysfunction70,71 in glaucoma. Interestingly, due to elevated PVEP magnitudes with age, PVEP magnitudes in participants with glaucoma were similar to those of younger adults. This finding demonstrates that care needs to be taken not to infer perceptual performance from PVEP amplitude alone and that it is important to consider possible aging effects when interpreting glaucoma data, a primary motivation for our study. 
A novel aspect of this study was the simultaneous recording of PERG and PVEP across a range of contrast conditions. This method enabled us to compare the characteristics of the relative loss to PERG and PVEP signals in glaucoma across these two electrophysiological methods in the same patients to the same stimulus set. We were interested in examining whether there might be evidence for neuroplasticity in post-retinal structures in glaucoma, as suggested by animal models following localized or absolute loss of retinal outputs72,73 and in animal models of glaucoma.74,75 Our data show similar differences in PERG and PVEP relative to age-matched controls (Fig. 2F). Assuming that the visual pathway processes signals in a serial manner, the cortical deficit found in our early glaucoma observers might be a simple reflection of an upstream retinal deficit. As the current study only included observers with early glaucoma, the extent of cortical deficits relative to retinal deficits in patients with moderate to advanced glaucoma is unclear. To further improve our understanding of the disease glaucoma, future studies should consider inclusion of participants with different stages of glaucoma and participants with monocular glaucoma to allow for comparison of responses between eyes. 
Through the use of simultaneous PERG and PVEP recordings, we demonstrate that retinal signals and contrast response function slopes were reduced in older adults and further reduced in those with glaucoma. The effects of aging manifest very differently for cortical signals, with a trend of elevation in response magnitude in the older cohort for the specific spatial and temporal frequency tested (0.8° or 0.625 cyc/deg and 8 Hz). Glaucoma decreased responses relative to age-matched controls, resulting in a net effect of the PVEP magnitude for high contrast stimuli being similar between those with glaucoma and younger healthy adults with normal vision. These findings provide insight into the nature of contrast processing in aging and glaucoma and clearly demonstrate the importance of age-matched norms for the interpretation of visual electrophysiological results in glaucoma. 
Acknowledgments
Supported by Australian Research Council FT 0990930 (AMM), Australian Research Council DP1401100157 (AMM). 
Disclosure: J.J. Lek, None; B.N. Nguyen, None; A.M. McKendrick, None; A.J. Vingrys, None 
References
Albrecht DG, Hamilton DB. Striate cortex of monkey and cat: contrast response function. J Neurophysiol. 1982; 48: 217–237.
Geisler WS, Albrecht DG. Cortical neurons: isolation of contrast gain control. Vision Res. 1992; 32: 1409–1410.
Cheng H, Chino YM, Smith EL III, Hamamoto J, Yoshida K. Transfer characteristics of lateral geniculate nucleus X neurons in the cat: effects of spatial frequency and contrast. J Neurophysiol. 1995; 74: 2548–2557.
Dean AF. The relationship between response amplitude and contrast for cat striate cortical neurones. J Physiol. 1981; 318: 413–427.
Shapley R, Enroth-Cugell C. Visual adaptation and retinal gain controls. In: NN, Osborne Chader GJ, eds. Progress in Retinal Research. Oxford, UK: Pergamon Press Ltd.; 1984: 263–346.
Tolhurst DJ, Movshon JA, Thompson ID. The dependence of response amplitude and variance of cat visual cortical neurones on stimulus contrast. Exp Brain Res. 1981; 41: 414–419.
Quigley HA, Dunkelberger GR, Green WR. Chronic human glaucoma causing selectively greater loss of large optic nerve fibers. Ophthalmology. 1988; 95: 357–363.
Quigley HA, Dunkelberger GR, Green WR. Retinal ganglion cell atrophy correlated with automated perimetry in human eyes with glaucoma. Am J Ophthalmol. 1989; 107: 453–464.
Dai H, Mu KT, Qi JP, et al. Assessment of lateral geniculate nucleus atrophy with 3T MR imaging and correlation with clinical stage of glaucoma. Am J Neuroradiol. 2011; 32: 1347–1353.
Yucel YH, Zhang Q, Weinreb RN, Kaufman PL, Gupta N. Atrophy of relay neurons in magno- and parvocellular layers in the lateral geniculate nucleus in experimental glaucoma. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2001; 42: 3216–3222.
Boucard CC, Hernowo AT, Maguire RP, et al. Changes in cortical grey matter density associated with long-standing retinal visual field defects. Brain. 2009; 132: 1898–1906.
Chen WW, Wang N, Cai S, et al. Structural brain abnormalities in patients with primary open-angle glaucoma: a study with 3T MR imaging. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2013; 54: 545–554.
Yucel YH, Zhang Q, Weinreb RN, Kaufman PL, Gupta N. Effects of retinal ganglion cell loss on magno-, parvo-, koniocellular pathways in the lateral geniculate nucleus and visual cortex in glaucoma. Prog Retin Eye Res. 2003; 22: 465–481.
McKendrick AM, Badcock DR, Morgan WH. Psychophysical measurement of neural adaptation abnormalities in magnocellular and parvocellular pathways in glaucoma. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2004; 45: 1846–1853.
Sampson GP, Badcock DR, Walland MJ, McKendrick AM. Foveal contrast processing of increment and decrement targets is equivalently reduced in glaucoma. Br J Ophthalmol. 2008; 92: 1287–1292.
Sun H, Swanson WH, Arvidson B, Dul MW. Assessment of contrast gain signature in inferred magnocellular and parvocellular pathways in patients with glaucoma. Vision Res. 2008; 48: 2633–2641.
Luo X, Frishman LJ. Retinal pathway origins of the pattern electroretinogram (PERG). Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2011; 52: 8571–8584.
Di Russo F, Pitzalis S, Aprile T, et al. Spatiotemporal analysis of the cortical sources of the steady-state visual evoked potential. Hum Brain Mapp. 2007; 28: 323–334.
Parisi V. Impaired visual function in glaucoma. Clin Neurophysiol. 2001; 112: 351–358.
Parisi V, Manni G, Centofanti M, Gandolfi SA, Olzi D, Bucci MG. Correlation between optical coherence tomography, pattern electroretinogram, and visual evoked potentials in open-angle glaucoma patients. Ophthalmology. 2001; 108: 905–912.
Parisi V, Miglior S, Manni G, Centofanti M, Bucci MG. Clinical ability of pattern electroretinograms and visual evoked potentials in detecting visual dysfunction in ocular hypertension and glaucoma. Ophthalmology. 2006; 113: 216–228.
Howe JW, Mitchell KW. Electrophysiologically determined contrast sensitivity in patients with ocular hypertension and chronic glaucoma. Doc Ophthalmol.. 1992; 80: 31–41.
Coleman AL, Miglior S. Risk factors for glaucoma onset and progression. Surv Ophthalmol. 2008; 53 (suppl 1): S3–10.
Curcio CA, Drucker DN. Retinal ganglion cells in Alzheimer's disease and aging. Ann Neurol. 1993; 33: 248–257.
Patel NB, Lim M, Gajjar A, Evans KB, Harwerth RS. Age-associated changes in the retinal nerve fiber layer and optic nerve head. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2014; 55: 5134–5143.
Li M, He HG, Shi W, et al. Quantification of the human lateral geniculate nucleus in vivo using MR imaging based on morphometry: volume loss with age. Am J Neuroradiol. 2012; 33: 915–921.
Gao F, Edden RA, Li M, et al. Edited magnetic resonance spectroscopy detects an age-related decline in brain GABA levels. NeuroImage. 2013; 78: 75–82.
Lek JJ, Vingrys AJ, McKendrick AM. Rapid contrast adaptation in glaucoma and in aging. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2014; 55: 3171–3178.
Owsley C. Aging and vision. Vision Res. 2011; 51: 1610–1622.
Karas R, McKendrick AM. Aging alters surround modulation of perceived contrast. J Vis. 2009; 9 (5): 11.
Karas R, McKendrick AM. Increased surround modulation of perceived contrast in the elderly. Optom Vis Sci. 2011; 88: 1298–1308.
Elliott SL, Werner JS. Age-related changes in contrast gain related to the M and P pathways. J Vis. 2010; 10 (4): 4.
McKendrick AM, Chan YM, Nguyen BN. Spatial vision in older adults: perceptual changes and neural bases. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt. 2018; 38: 363–375.
Angelucci A, Bijanzadeh M, Nurminen L, Federer F, Merlin S, Bressloff PC. Circuits and mechanisms for surround modulation in visual cortex. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2017; 40: 425–451.
Lamme VA, Super H, Spekreijse H. Feedforward, horizontal, and feedback processing in the visual cortex. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 1998; 8: 529–535.
Pitchaimuthu K, Wu QZ, Carter O, et al. Occipital GABA levels in older adults and their relationship to visual perceptual suppression. Sci Rep. 2017; 7: 14231.
Chylack LT Jr, Wolfe JK, Singer DM, et al. The Lens Opacities Classification System III. The Longitudinal Study of Cataract Study Group. Arch Ophthalmol. 1993; 111: 831–836.
Anderson AJ, McKendrick AM. Quantifying adaptation and fatigue effects in frequency doubling perimetry. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2007; 48: 943–948.
Bach M, Hiss P, Rover J. Check-size specific changes of pattern electroretinogram in patients with early open-angle glaucoma. Doc Ophthalmol. 1988; 69: 315–322.
Trick GL. Retinal potentials in patients with primary open-angle glaucoma: physiological evidence for temporal frequency tuning deficits. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1985; 26: 1750–1758.
Bach M, Speidel-Fiaux A. Pattern electroretinogram in glaucoma and ocular hypertension. Doc Ophthalmol. 1989; 73: 173–181.
Tyler CW. Specific deficits of flicker sensitivity in glaucoma and ocular hypertension. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1981; 20: 204–212.
Nguyen BN, McKendrick AM, Vingrys AJ. Abnormal inhibition-excitation imbalance in migraine. Cephalalgia. 2016; 36: 5–14.
Hood DC, Zhang X. Multifocal ERG and VEP responses and visual fields: comparing disease-related changes. Doc Ophthalmol. 2000; 100: 115–137.
Ishikawa K, Nagai T, Yamada Y, Negi A, Nakamura M. Optimal conditions for multifocal VEP recording for normal Japanese population established by receiver operating characteristic analysis. Doc Ophthalmol. 2011; 122: 29–37.
Bach M, Meigen T. Do's and don'ts in Fourier analysis of steady-state potentials. Doc Ophthalmol. 1999; 99: 69–82.
Hess RF, Baker CL Jr. Human pattern-evoked electroretinogram. J Neurophysiol. 1984; 51: 939–951.
Thompson D, Drasdo N. The effect of stimulus contrast on the latency and amplitude of the pattern electroretinogram. Vision Res. 1989; 29: 309–313.
Naka KI, Rushton WA. S-potentials from luminosity units in the retina of fish (Cyprinidae). J Physiol. 1966; 185: 587–599.
Contreras D, Palmer L. Response to contrast of electrophysiologically defined cell classes in primary visual cortex. J Neurosci. 2003; 23: 6936–6945.
Tsai JJ, Norcia AM, Ales JM, Wade AR. Contrast gain control abnormalities in idiopathic generalized epilepsy. Ann Neurol. 2011; 70: 574–582.
Porciatti V, Burr DC, Morrone MC, Fiorentini A. The effects of aging on the pattern electroretinogram and visual evoked potential in humans. Vision Res. 1992; 32: 1199–1209.
Trick GL, Nesher R, Cooper DG, Shields SM. The human pattern ERG: alteration of response properties with aging. Optom Vis Sci. 1992; 69: 122–128.
Harman A, Abrahams B, Moore S, Hoskins R. Neuronal density in the human retinal ganglion cell layer from 16–77 years. Anat Rec. 2000; 260: 124–131.
Trick GL. Pattern electroretinogram: an electrophysiological technique applicable to primary open-angle glaucoma and ocular hypertension. J Glaucoma. 1992; 1: 271–279.
Bach M, Brigell MG, Hawlina M, et al. ISCEV standard for clinical pattern electroretinography (PERG): 2012 update. Doc Ophthalmol. 2013; 126: 1–7.
Odom JV, Bach M, Brigell M, et al. ISCEV standard for clinical visual evoked potentials: (2016 update). Doc Ophthalmol. 2016; 133: 1–9.
Tobimatsu S. Aging and pattern visual evoked potentials. Optom Vis Sci. 1995; 72: 192–197.
Mitchell KW, Howe JW, Spencer SR. Visual evoked potentials in the older population: age and gender effects. Clin Phys Physiol Meas. 1987; 8: 317–324.
Fiorentini A, Porciatti V, Morrone MC, Burr DC. Visual ageing: unspecific decline of the responses to luminance and colour. Vision Res. 1996; 36: 3557–3566.
Tomoda H, Celesia GG, Brigell MG, Toleikis S. The effects of age on steady-state pattern electroretinograms and visual evoked potentials. Doc Ophthalmol. 1991; 77: 201–211.
Trick GL, Trick LR, Haywood KM. Altered pattern evoked retinal and cortical potentials associated with human senescence. Curr Eye Res. 1986; 5: 717–724.
Salin PA, Bullier J. Corticocortical connections in the visual system: structure and function. Physiol Rev. 1995; 75: 107–154.
Hua T, Li X, He L, Zhou Y, Wang Y, Leventhal AG. Functional degradation of visual cortical cells in old cats. Neurobiol Aging. 2006; 27: 155–162.
Schmolesky MT, Wang Y, Pu M, Leventhal AG. Degradation of stimulus selectivity of visual cortical cells in senescent rhesus monkeys. Nat Neurosci. 2000; 3: 384–390.
Leventhal AG, Wang Y, Pu M, Zhou Y, Ma Y. GABA and its agonists improved visual cortical function in senescent monkeys. Science. 2003; 300: 812–815.
McKendrick AM, Sampson GP, Walland MJ, Badcock DR. Impairments of contrast discrimination and contrast adaptation in glaucoma. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2010; 51: 920–927.
Porciatti V, Falsini B, Brunori S, Colotto A, Moretti G. Pattern electroretinogram as a function of spatial frequency in ocular hypertension and early glaucoma. Doc Ophthalmol. 1987; 65: 349–355.
Ventura LM, Porciatti V. Restoration of retinal ganglion cell function in early glaucoma after intraocular pressure reduction: a pilot study. Ophthalmology. 2005; 112: 20–27.
Weber AJ, Harman CD. Structure-function relations of parasol cells in the normal and glaucomatous primate retina. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2005; 46: 3197–3207.
Weber AJ, Kaufman PL, Hubbard WC. Morphology of single ganglion cells in the glaucomatous primate retina. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1998; 39: 2304–2320.
Baekelandt V, Arckens L, Annaert W, Eysel UT, Orban GA, Vandesande F. Alterations in GAP-43 and synapsin immunoreactivity provide evidence for synaptic reorganization in adult cat dorsal lateral geniculate nucleus following retinal lesions. Eur J Neurosci. 1994; 6: 754–765.
Eysel UT, Schweigart G, Mittmann T, et al. Reorganization in the visual cortex after retinal and cortical damage. Restor Neurol Neurosci. 1999; 15: 153–164.
King WM, Sarup V, Sauve Y, Moreland CM, Carpenter DO, Sharma SC. Expansion of visual receptive fields in experimental glaucoma. Vis Neurosci. 2006; 23: 137–142.
Lam DY, Kaufman PL, Gabelt BT, To EC, Matsubara JA. Neurochemical correlates of cortical plasticity after unilateral elevated intraocular pressure in a primate model of glaucoma. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2003; 44: 2573–2581.
Figure 1
 
Representative PERG and PVEP steady-state waveforms in response to a 97% contrast stimulus recorded from (A) a healthy younger observer (31 years old), (B) a healthy older observer (57 years old), and (C) an observer with glaucoma (G12, Table, 61 years old). The magnitude of the 2F (16 Hz) component is shown in the top right hand corner of each Fourier domain panel.
Figure 1
 
Representative PERG and PVEP steady-state waveforms in response to a 97% contrast stimulus recorded from (A) a healthy younger observer (31 years old), (B) a healthy older observer (57 years old), and (C) an observer with glaucoma (G12, Table, 61 years old). The magnitude of the 2F (16 Hz) component is shown in the top right hand corner of each Fourier domain panel.
Figure 2
 
Group average retinal (A), (C) and cortical (B), (D) responses with aging and with glaucoma. Group mean (±95% confidence interval of the mean) PERG and PVEP magnitude (A), (B), phase (C), (D) as a function of contrast in younger (circles), older (squares) and glaucoma (triangles) observers. Shaded regions in (A) and (B) represent the upper 95% limits of noise levels as measured with 0% contrast, and in (C), data that have been excluded from statistical analysis due to lying in the region of noise. Relative change (mean ± 95% confidence interval) in PERG (filled symbols) and PVEP (unfilled symbols) with (E) aging and (F) with glaucoma. Gray symbols represent PERG signals that were in noisy regions. Data have been shifted horizontally for clarity.
Figure 2
 
Group average retinal (A), (C) and cortical (B), (D) responses with aging and with glaucoma. Group mean (±95% confidence interval of the mean) PERG and PVEP magnitude (A), (B), phase (C), (D) as a function of contrast in younger (circles), older (squares) and glaucoma (triangles) observers. Shaded regions in (A) and (B) represent the upper 95% limits of noise levels as measured with 0% contrast, and in (C), data that have been excluded from statistical analysis due to lying in the region of noise. Relative change (mean ± 95% confidence interval) in PERG (filled symbols) and PVEP (unfilled symbols) with (E) aging and (F) with glaucoma. Gray symbols represent PERG signals that were in noisy regions. Data have been shifted horizontally for clarity.
Figure 3
 
Group average for (A) PERG slope, and (B) PVEP slope, and (C) semi saturation constant (C50) of younger (circles), older (squares), and glaucoma (triangles) groups. Error bars are 95% confidence intervals of the mean. Note that in Figure 3B, the error bars are largest in the young group (filled circle) due to a single observer's data returning a slope (n) of 17.2 (median of young group = 0.99). The unfilled symbol next to the young group data shows the mean ± 95% confidence interval of the mean without this observer included in the group.
Figure 3
 
Group average for (A) PERG slope, and (B) PVEP slope, and (C) semi saturation constant (C50) of younger (circles), older (squares), and glaucoma (triangles) groups. Error bars are 95% confidence intervals of the mean. Note that in Figure 3B, the error bars are largest in the young group (filled circle) due to a single observer's data returning a slope (n) of 17.2 (median of young group = 0.99). The unfilled symbol next to the young group data shows the mean ± 95% confidence interval of the mean without this observer included in the group.
Table
 
Characteristics of Participants With Glaucoma: ID Number, Age, Diagnosis (POAG: Primary Open Angle Glaucoma, NTG: Normal Tension Glaucoma), Moorfields Regression Analysis (MRA), Glaucoma Probability Score (GPS) Tool of the Heidelberg Retinal Tomograph II, and Average Defect (AD), Pattern Defect (PD) on the Central Test of the Medmont Perimeter
Table
 
Characteristics of Participants With Glaucoma: ID Number, Age, Diagnosis (POAG: Primary Open Angle Glaucoma, NTG: Normal Tension Glaucoma), Moorfields Regression Analysis (MRA), Glaucoma Probability Score (GPS) Tool of the Heidelberg Retinal Tomograph II, and Average Defect (AD), Pattern Defect (PD) on the Central Test of the Medmont Perimeter
×
×

This PDF is available to Subscribers Only

Sign in or purchase a subscription to access this content. ×

You must be signed into an individual account to use this feature.

×