Abstract
Purpose:
To use a human-based model to study the effects of repeated tear film instability on corneal detection thresholds to cold, mechanical, and chemical stimuli.
Methods:
Twenty-five subjects participated in three study visits. A computer-controlled Belmonte esthesiometer was used to estimate corneal detection thresholds to cold, mechanical, and chemical stimuli before, after, and 30 minutes following 10 consecutive sustained tear exposure (STARE) trials. Subjects turned a pain knob (0–10) to indicate discomfort during STARE trials. The area of tear breakup and thinning in each trial was analyzed. Symptoms were evaluated by the Current Symptom Questionnaire (CSQ).
Results:
There was a significant time effect on CSQ symptoms during both visits (Friedman test, P < 0.001), with immediately after repeated STARE and 30 minutes later significantly differing from before STARE (Wilcoxon, P < 0.017). Tear breakup occurred in every trial, ranging from 25% to 88% of the exposed corneal area and all subjects indicated discomfort during trials. There was a significant time effect on mechanical thresholds between before STARE mechanical thresholds and 30 minutes later (repeated measures analysis of variance [ANOVA] P < 0.001), but not cold (P = 0.057) or chemical (P = 0. 565) thresholds.
Conclusions:
In this study, tear breakup during STARE trials was associated with discomfort, which when repeated, resulted in increased symptoms of ocular discomfort and alterations of mechanical sensory thresholds after 30 minutes. These results suggest that tear film instability, which is thought to occur repeatedly during normal blinking among dry eye patients over the day, can produce neurosensory alterations.
Dry eye disease (DED) is a condition that affects millions in the United States
1–4 and worldwide.
5–9 It adversely affects quality of life
2,10 and poses a considerable economic burden, estimated at $55.4 billion yearly, to the US economy.
11 According to the Dry Eye Workshop (DEWS) II report,
12 DED involves a loss of homeostasis of the tear film and is accompanied by ocular symptoms, with tear film instability, hyperosmolarity, inflammation, and neurosensory abnormalities playing an etiological role. However, as the definition suggests, the connection between the etiological factors and their impact on tear film homeostasis and symptoms remains speculative,
12–14 perhaps due to the potential multifactorial causes of DED.
12
Theoretically, tear film homeostasis is disrupted in a proposed cycle of events leading to DED, sometimes referred to as the “vicious circle of DED.”
12–14 The circle or cycle includes tear film hyperosmolarity, inflammation, neural stimulation, and surface alterations and damage. It can be entered at multiple locations, but an important entry point is tear film instability, which includes both tear breakup and excessive tear thinning.
12–14 It is thought that the sequelae of tear film instability and associated ocular stress could repeatedly stimulate ocular surface sensory neurons and may lead to sensitization, increasing neural damage and functional disturbances that can ultimately lead to neuropathic pain.
15 Bron et al.
16 suggested that the predicted natural progression of dry eye follows this course, beginning with tear film instability and increased tear film osmolarity, which provides noxious stimulation to ocular surface sensory nerve endings and increase sensory drive, followed eventually by increasing neural damage and decreased sensation.
Thus, studying neurosensory abnormalities in DED and their relationship to other potential etiological factors can be challenging due to the potential for ocular surface damage and neurosensory alterations in the condition.
12 An alternative approach is to create a method to study DED etiological factors in healthy subjects with an unaltered ocular surface and normal sensory response to understand potential initiating factors in the condition. In previous studies,
17–19 we used the technique of sustained tear exposure (STARE) to induce tear film instability, a known etiological factor in DED, and study its effects. Using STARE, we have shown that tear film instability, or more specifically tear breakup, is associated with immediate pain, discomfort, burning, and stinging in both healthy and DED subjects, suggesting that tear breakup can directly stimulate underlying corneal neurons. Furthermore, we have recently shown that ratings of intensity to cooling stimuli decreased, whereas irritation ratings to mechanical stimuli increased following repeated STARE trials, which suggested that tear film instability affects neurosensory function.
20 However, the link between symptoms and neurosensory function has yet to be demonstrated, and is likely not to be a direct one. In this study, we use the STARE technique to induce and measure tear breakup
17,18,21,22 in healthy subjects to test the hypothesis that repeated episodes of tear film instability and breakup are associated with discomfort and lead to symptoms of ocular irritation and alterations in corneal thresholds.
This study was reviewed and approved by the Indiana University Institutional Review Board and was conducted at the Indiana University School of Optometry in Bloomington, Indiana. Informed consent was obtained from all subjects before beginning the study, which was conducted in compliance with the tenets of the Declaration of Helsinki. Twenty-five subjects with no self-reported history of dry eye, other ocular diseases, ocular surgeries, systemic diseases, or use of topical or systemic medications that could affect ocular sensation were included in the study. Before admission into the study, subjects were screened to exclude those with evidence of ocular surface disease.
This study involved three visits on three different days. Visit 1 involved examination of the ocular surface to screen for damage or disease, measurement of tear breakup time, and the Schirmer's tear test. Subjects were trained in the procedures to be used in visits 2 and 3, including sustained eye exposure (STARE) and threshold measurements by the Indiana University Belmonte esthesiometer.
23 If subjects were unable to perform the latter two procedures adequately, they were determined ineligible for the study.
Visits 2 and 3 were experimental visits, which occurred within 2 weeks of each other. Subjects filled out three questionnaires. The Demographic Questionnaire asks subjects about their age, sex, race, and ocular and general health. The Dry Eye Questionnaire (DEQ-5) queries habitual symptoms of dry eye over the past month, with a cutoff score of 6 dry eye and 12 for suspected Sjögren syndrome.
24 The Current Symptom Questionnaire (CSQ) asks about the intensity of symptoms of ocular irritation associated with dry eye at the time of testing (total summed score = 50).
25
Sensory thresholds to pneumatic cool stimuli were measured by the ascending method of limits
26 using our custom, computer-controlled, modified Belmonte esthesiometer.
23 Subjects fixated on a target with the untested eye during the experiment. The tip of the esthesiometer was 5 mm from the central corneal surface of the tested eye and was continuously monitored by a calibrated video camera. Before stimulus presentation, a sound signaled the subject to blink and fixate on the target. The stimulus duration was 2 seconds with an interval of 10 seconds between stimuli. Subjects pressed a knob to indicate stimulus detection. Initial starting flow rates were 30 mL/min, with steps of at least 10 mL/min above the initial flow rates. This procedure was repeated three times and the final threshold was determined as the average flow rate. The order of study visits was always cool stimulus in visit 2 and mechanical/chemical in visit 3, because of the length of time required to cool the esthesiometer.
The pneumatic cool stimulus in visit 2 consisted of room temperature air (approximately 20°C) with a flow rate varying from 0 to 200 mL/min. During visit 3, the mechanical stimulus was measured first (flow rates from 0 to 200 mL/min), with the esthesiometer air heated to approximate the corneal temperature at the eye and minimize the possibility of a thermal (cooling) effect.
27 After measurement of the mechanical stimulus threshold, the chemical stimulus was delivered at 50% flow rates beneath the subject's threshold for the mechanical stimulus to minimize the likelihood of mechanical effects contaminating the chemical threshold.
28 The chemical stimulus was produced by adding CO
2 to the stimulus air column
29 (0% to 80% CO
2) to induce a local pH change in the tear film. The initial level of 10% CO
2 was increased in increments of 5% CO
2.
After threshold measurements, subjects were seated behind a slit lamp biomicroscope custom fitted with a Tamron (Saitoma, Japan) (M111FM16) C1.1 16 mm F/1.8 12 MegaPixel Fixed Focal lens (Tamron, USA). Images of the tear film of the tested eye (untested eye held shut) were taken after instilling 5 μL 2% fluorescein dye with a micro-pipette onto the superior conjunctiva. Subjects then performed 10 consecutive STARE trials, during which subjects kept the tested eye open for as long as possible.
18 During each STARE trial, subjects turned a pain knob to indicate the level of ocular discomfort, from 0 (no discomfort) to 10 (pain level that requires blinking), with a 5-second break between each trial. Additional fluorescein dye was instilled when necessary to ensure adequate fluorescence of the tear film.
Immediately after the 10th STARE trial, the CSQ was completed and threshold measurements were repeated, using the same procedures. Subjects then waited for 30 minutes in the laboratory, engaging in activities of their choice (reading or electronic device usage). The 30-minute time period was chosen based on previous studies
18,20 using the STARE technique, during which subjects noted that most ocular symptoms were greatly diminished after 30 minutes (data not published). After the 30-minute break, subjects again filled out the CSQ and threshold measurements were made as before.
Ten subjects from the pool of experimental subjects participated in a control experiment, in which the procedures in visits 2 and 3 were repeated, except without the instillation of fluorescein and STARE trials. As in the experimental visits, control subjects filled out questionnaires at the beginning of each visit and then took initial threshold measures. Afterward, subjects sat in the laboratory and participated in the activity of their choice for 30 minutes (reading or electronic device), which was the average time required for 10 STARE trials. Then thresholds for either cool or mechanical/chemical stimuli were measured and again after 30 minutes, as in experimental visits. Control experiments were designed to control for the effect of extended eye opening and delayed blinking (STARE) versus normal blinking during activities chosen by subjects.
Supported by Grant Number R01EY021794 (CGB) from the National Eye Institute. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Eye Institute or the National Institutes of Health.
Disclosure: D. Awisi-Gyau, None; C.G. Begley, None; P. Situ, None; T.L. Simpson, None