Abstract
Purpose:
To investigate the influence of interocular suppression and visual acuity loss on stereoacuity in observers with and without abnormal vision development from strabismus or amblyopia. To determine whether stereoacuity improves in amblyopic observers when suppression is neutralized.
Methods:
Experiment 1: Visual acuity (VA), depth of suppression (contrast ratio [CR]), and stereoacuity (digital random-dot) were tested in adult amblyopic observers (n = 21; age 27 ± 11 years). Experiment 2: VA, stereoacuity, and CR were measured at baseline and through a series of monocular contrast attenuation and Bangerter filter conditions that degrade visual input in participants with normal binocular vision (n = 19; age 31 ± 13 years). Multiple regression models were used to determine relative contribution of VA and CR to stereoacuity in both groups. Experiment 3: stereoacuity was retested in a subsample of amblyopic observers (n = 7) after contrast reduction of the stimulus presented to dominant eye to neutralize suppression.
Results:
In amblyopic observers, stereoacuity significantly correlated with CR (P < 0.001), but not with interocular VA difference (P = 0.863). In participants with normal vision development, stereoacuity, VA, and CR declined with introduction of monocular Bangerter filter (P < 0.001), and stereoacuity reduced with monocular attenuation of stimulus contrast (P < 0.001). Reduction in stereoacuity correlated with both VA decrement and degraded CR. Stereoacuity significantly improved in amblyopic observers when the contrast to the dominant eye was adjusted based on the contrast ratio.
Conclusions:
Suppression rather than visual acuity loss limits stereoacuity in observers with abnormal vision development. Stereopsis can be improved when interocular sensory dominance is neutralized.
Amblyopia is a developmental disorder of the visual system in the brain. In most cases, patients suffering from amblyopia will have reduced visual acuity in one eye. There is a degree to which the brain suppresses (or ignores) the visual signal from that eye. This typically results in reduced or absent stereopsis (binocular depth perception). Stereopsis, the ability to perceive the depth of an object relative to an observer, arises from the binocular sensory fusion of slightly dissimilar retinal images that have horizontal disparity by virtue of the lateral separation of the two eyes.
1,2 Stereoacuity, reported in minutes or seconds of arc, describes the smallest horizontal disparity in binocular images that leads to the perception of depth in the observed stimulus. In observers with equal normal visual acuity and normal vision development, stereoacuity thresholds are typically of 20 to 40 seconds of arc.
2–4 Stereoacuity will be worse when there are significant differences in the retinal images between eyes, that is, if the dichoptic images differ in contrast, size, or clarity.
5–9 Differences in retinal image clarity and size can occur if there is unequal refractive error (anisometropia) or ocular media obstruction from corneal or lens anomalies, with both blur and contrast differences known to degrade stereoacuity.
10 Stereoacuity also requires accurate ocular alignment so that the dichoptic images stimulate corresponding retinal areas for the perception of fused single vision.
2
Nonconcordance of retinal images from ocular media obstruction, high anisometropic refractive error or strabismus early in life will interrupt the neurodevelopment of the visual system and cause amblyopia.
11 Patients with amblyopia may have nonrecoverable reduced visual acuity, may suppress or ignore the visual signal from one eye and are likely to have reduced or absent stereopsis (binocular depth vision).
12–15 In addition to visual acuity and stereoacuity loss, amblyopia results in impaired position discrimination and contrast sensitivity
11 and, furthermore, may be associated with higher order functional and processing impairments that suggest visual neurodevelopmental retardation beyond the primary visual cortex.
16–18
Interocular suppression of visual information from the amblyopic eye is frequently reported in amblyopia,
13,19 presumably to compensate for conflicting visual input.
20 As well as limiting stereopsis by preventing sensory fusion,
21 the role that suppression plays in causing or limiting amblyopia recovery is yet to be fully determined. Novel treatments for amblyopia are aimed at reducing suppression of the amblyopic visual pathway to improve binocularity
22–25; however, the relative contribution of interocular differences in visual acuity and depth of suppression in determining the limit of stereoacuity is unclear. For example, it has been believed for some time that amblyopia (reduced acuity in one eye) is the primary problem and loss of binocularity, the consequence. However, more recently it has been suggested that it is the other way around
13,26,27; loss of binocularity is the primary problem and amblyopia the consequence. According to the former explanation one would expect an inverse relationship between suppression and acuity loss; the more amblyopic the eye, the less it needs to be suppressed to ensure it does not interfere in binocular vision. On the other hand, the latter explanation would assume a direct relationship, whereby the greater the suppression the greater the resultant amblyopia. A number of studies have shown that there is a direct relationship between suppression and acuity loss in amblyopia.
4,15 Quite apart from these interrelationships in amblyopia, it would be of value to know the extent to which sensory eye dominance and stereopsis are correlated in the normal binocular population and the extent to which degraded monocular input affects ocular sensory dominance and stereopsis.
In this study we seek to make a distinction between simulations of acuity-like losses and suppressive-like losses. From what we know about the contrast sensitivity deficit in amblyopia,
28–30 the higher the spatial frequency, the greater the loss. It is like a lowpass filtering, and the resultant loss in acuity would be simulated by an increased slope of the amplitude spectrum. We use the Bangerter filters in normal participants to simulate the amblyopic loss.
31,32 Suppression affects low as well as high spatial frequencies
33 and is best simulated by a vertical translation of the 1/f amplitude spectrum line. It is the standard metric used to quantify suppression in the case of amblyopia.
With the advent of polarized or three-dimensional monitors, new dichoptic tests have been developed that measure stereoacuity to threshold,
3,25,34 and tests can also be used to quantify the relative contrast balance required for binocular perception (a measure of the depth of suppression).
19,35,36 The contrast of stimulus can be modified in the dichoptic displays to attenuate presentations to either eye so the factors influencing stereoacuity threshold can be investigated. With this method contrast is equally reduced across all spatial frequency components.
In this study stereoacuity of adults with abnormal binocular vision development from strabismus and/or amblyopia was measured and interactions between the interocular sensory dominance (depth of suppression), visual acuity decrement (degree of amblyopia) and stereoacuity threshold determined. To mimic monocular visual acuity impairment Bangerter filters were introduced before one eye of observers with normal visual development; the impact of the reduced visual acuity and sensory dominance imbalance on stereoacuity was determined. Additionally, the monocular contrast attenuation feature of the digital stereoacuity test was used to separately measure the effect of interocular contrast difference on stereoacuity thresholds. The prediction was that if the main determinant of stereopsis was monocular acuity loss rather than interocular suppression, then stereoacuity data with the Bangerter filters (our simulation of the monocular acuity loss) should be more correlated with stereoacuity than with monocular contrast attenuation (our simulation of the interocular suppressive imbalance). In the case that stereopsis is more affected by the binocular suppressive imbalance, then the reverse would be the case; stereoacuity would be more impacted by the interocular contrast difference. Finally, stereoacuity was retested in a subset of amblyopic participants with the contrast of the stimulus presented to the dominant eye reduced, proportional to the determined interocular dominance, to counter suppression. For amblyopic participants the prediction was that if stereopsis were limited mainly by suppression, then by neutralizing suppression by altering the contrast, stereoacuity would be expected to improve.
This study had three aims: (1) to determine the relative strength of association between reduced visual acuity, the depth of suppression (contrast balance) and the stereoacuity in observers with abnormal binocular vision development (this directly bears on the etiology), (2) to determine the relative contribution of induced visual acuity or interocular dominance deficits to stereoacuity loss when vision is monocularly impaired in observers with normal vision development, and (3) to determine whether stereoacuity improves in amblyopic observers when interocular dominance balance is altered to neutralize suppression.
Adult participants were prospectively recruited for a study of binocular vision perception. All had a comprehensive vision and intraocular health exam (previous treatment history, visual acuity (electronic Early Treatment of Diabetic Study e-ETDRS and near logMAR), clinical stereoacuity (Randot Preschool Stereoacuity Test (RPST); Stereo Optical Co, Inc., Chicago, IL, USA), ocular alignment (prism alternating cover test) and Worth 4 Dot response at 33 cm to confirm eligibility criteria. Classification criteria for abnormal binocular vision group were a history of strabismus and/or amblyopia (anisometropic, strabismic or combined mechanism). Amblyopia was defined as an interocular difference in best-corrected visual acuity of 0.2 logMAR or worse. Inclusion criteria for the normal binocular vision group were best-corrected visual acuity of 0.0 logMAR or better, and 60 sec of arc stereoacuity on RPST. None of the participants had coexisting general developmental, systemic, or ocular pathology or congenital abnormality.
The study was conducted in accordance with the requirements of the Queensland University of Technology Human Research Ethics Committee. All participants were given a full explanation of the experimental procedures and written informed consent was obtained. All protocols were in accord with the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki.
The authors thank Peter Bex and Miyoung Kwon for the use of their dichoptic letter chart software.
Disclosure: A.L. Webber, None; K.L. Schmid, None; A.S. Baldwin, System and Method for Digital Measurement of Stereo Vision (P); R.F. Hess, System and Method for Digital Measurement of Stereo Vision (P)