Achromatopsia is characterized by color blindness and a lack or reduced number of normally functioning cones; it provides a perfect sample to study the importance of color perception and cone photoreceptors in the process of emmetropization in humans. The present study collected retrospective refractive error data from 28 patients with achromatopsia caused by homozygosity for c.1148delC in CNGB3. The advantage of selecting these patients is that the genetic cause is known, confirming the clinical diagnosis, and there is no genetic variance.
We found that emmetropization was abnormal in this population. The prevalence of refractive error was high, and the distribution of SER and spherical refractive error in adults did not exhibit the characteristic leptokurtic distribution
3,38 but resembled a more Gaussian distribution (
Fig. 1). We found a wider range of refractive error than previously described for achromats.
31,32 Although emmetropization in our population of achromats seemed to follow a different pattern than the background population, we did observe a reduction of median SER and sphere during the first 7 years of life, suggesting that some form of emmetropization takes place.
Comparing the distribution of refractive errors in the age group 4 to 7.5 years to published Danish normative data,
36 we found that our population of achromats was more hyperopic and had a larger with-the-rule astigmatic refractive error. Applying the model for childhood development in ocular variables including SER presented by Mutti et al.,
37 we found that for our population of achromats there was no exponential decrease in SER in the first 2 years of life; however, our data can be fitted to the quadratic phase with a decrease in SER continuing past the first 2 years of life.
The prevalence of refractive error was higher in our adult population than published normative data for Danish adults in the age group 30 to 60 years,
38 for which the prevalence of emmetropia was 42% and the prevalence of myopia was 31%. In our population, 19% were emmetropic, and the remainder were either myopic (38%) or hyperopic (43%). We found a similar median astigmatic refractive error of –1.50 D in the pediatric and adult age groups, contrasting with the age-related increase in astigmatic refractive error observed in idiopathic congenital nystagmus and albinism.
33 In albinism, the increase in astigmatism with age has been ascribed to corneal remodeling as a consequence of nystagmus. The majority of our patients had childhood nystagmus, but the direction and amplitude were unfortunately not described in the medical records; hence, it is unknown how it would affect corneal remolding, especially in cases with multidirectional nystagmus. Others have found that
CNGB3 achromatopsia is predominantly associated with a pendular nystagmus with vertical elements.
39 Future larger studies on different forms of congenital nystagmus with well-documented characterization of nystagmus are necessary to further elucidate the role of nystagmus on astigmatism.
The effect that color perception has on emmetropization is not fully understood. Animal studies on non-primates have shown a myopic shift after prolonged exposure to monochromatic long-wavelength (red) light and a more hyperopic shift in short-wavelength (blue) light due to LCA.
40 Rearing animals under quasimonochromatic conditions does not prevent recovery from deprivation-induced myopia or refractive compensation for optically imposed defocus, implying that chromatic cues are not the only parameter guiding emmetropization. In primates, the role of LCA is less convincing. Smith et al.
11 found that reducing chromatic cues with filters interfered with emmetropization, but the effect was opposite of what was expected from LCA. Liu et al.
13 found that two out of nine monkeys kept under red light conditions developed myopia, but no significant difference in refraction was observed in monkeys kept in blue light, suggesting that long-wavelength light may be a risk factor in myopia development and that LCA can interfere with refraction. Humans with red/green color vision deficiency are less myopic that individuals with normal color vision.
15 In other conditions caused by variants in the cone opsin genes, a high degree of myopic refractive error has been documented.
16,17 For humans to perceive colors, functioning cone photoreceptors and an intact optic nerve are required. But, because an intact optic nerve is not required for emmetropization to occur, the detection of LCA probably relies more on the composition of the cone photoreceptor mosaic. A recent study indicated that the presence of non-functional cones may also impact refractive error.
41 Chromatic properties of light play a role in accommodation in humans,
40 but the role of accommodation on emmetropization is not clear. The eye can still compensate for optical lens-induced defocus in the absence of accommodation as a result of ciliary nerve section, at least in chickens.
8
Our results seem to support the notion that color perception and cone photoreceptors play a role in emmetropization but that it is not essential. Refractive development in our color-blind population was abnormal, but some degree of emmetropization did take place. The inability to detect colors may not be the only reason for retinal blur in our population. The blur caused by nystagmus and astigmatism may also play a role.
Adjusting eye growth to compensate for optically induced defocus and form deprivation does not depend on the fovea in primates
9 and may rely on a local peripheral retinal mechanism,
42 but which part of the retina/sclera drives this is not known. Studies on mice seem to indicate that rod photoreceptors might be important, as rod knockout mice do not develop form-deprivation myopia.
43
The retrospective nature of the study limits the rigor of the data, as the preferred clinical information was not always documented in the medical records (specifically, the use of cycloplegia), especially in records of older date. The lack of consistent use of cycloplegia is a limitation in our study design, as accommodation in determination of the refractive error, especially in the pediatric population, may affect spherical refraction in a less hyperopic direction and challenge comparison. In the patients for whom refractive data before and after cycloplegic drops were available, we did not find an indication of impaired accommodation. Where cycloplegic refraction was not specified, documented refractive data were the prescribed correction based on subjective refraction and, most often, on repeated streak retinoscopy. Streak retinoscopy was performed by examiners experienced in determining the refractive error in visually impaired children. Despite this limitation, we chose this retrospective design to be able to accumulate as large a dataset with as long a follow-up as possible, considering the rarity of achromatopsia. The cohort size is small, however, thus placing limitations on our interpretation of the data, especially in the pediatric group.
A further limitation of our study is that there was no analysis of functional parameters. Achromats could be completely without functional cones or they may have some residual cone function. The selection of a genetically homogeneous population was an attempt to minimize this, but in achromatopsia and other hereditary retinal disorders the relationship between genotype and phenotype is not always predictable. However, methods currently available (e.g., color vision testing, electroretinography, adaptive optics) are not sensitive enough to detect small variance in morphology or function of the cone mosaic, and the rarity of achromatopsia makes it difficult to compare a large number of patients.
In conclusion, the refractive development in our population of c.1148delC homozygous
CNGB3 achromats was not normal, but the refractive development in achromats is more complicated than a complete failure to emmetropize, as there were signs of some degree of emmetropization, which seems to support the notion that cone photoreceptors are important in providing cues to guide emmetropization but they are not essential. Although we did find that the majority of our population (43%) were hyperopic, the spread in refractive errors was large, and the prevalence of myopia contradicts the previously suggested notion that achromats are predominantly hyperopic.
29,30 The astigmatic refractive error was higher than for the normal population, but there was no indication of an increase with age as has been seen in other types of congenital nystagmus. Our knowledge of emmetropization is based mainly on animal studies, but the retinal structure varies greatly among species. Studying emmetropization in selected patient groups may provide future insights into the normal emmetropization process in humans.