This study revealed changes linked to high myopic astigmatism, in the patterns of meridional anisotropy, as inherent in spatial resolution at the fovea and more peripheral locations. The astigmatic participants exhibited lower (or worse) resolution acuity for horizontal gratings than vertical gratings in all tested retinal locations, which conformed to the orientational defocus blur caused by their WTR astigmatism (see further discussion below). Their meridional anisotropy differed from the two nonastigmatic groups in the fovea and nasal field, even though they had normal visual acuities (logMAR 0 or better).
It is evident that uncorrected astigmatism can result in meridional deficits in foveal vision.
7 Because myopic WTR astigmatism smears the horizontal component of retinal images projected from distant objects, the reduced resolution acuity for horizontal gratings may be associated with any uncorrected astigmatism or partially corrected residual astigmatism during the critical periods of visual development. While spectacle correction can effectively recover visual acuity in astigmatism-related amblyopia, meridional sensitivity loss usually persists, particularly in those with myopic astigmatism.
22
Previous studies of astigmatism-related visual loss
9 considered only the fovea, so whether astigmatism affects peripheral vision had not been thoroughly investigated. Apart from foveal vision, apparently in the nasal field, the presence of astigmatism affected resolution acuity for the selected orientations and, hence, disturbed meridional anisotropy. In the peripheral fields, the nonastigmatic eye had finer visual resolution for gratings oriented radially: horizontal gratings in the nasal field and vertical gratings in the inferior field. Such radial bias has been thought to help maximize contextual information extraction,
23 compute optic flow,
24 and plan for saccadic eye movement.
24 Notably, in the nasal field, the astigmatic participants exhibited higher resolution acuity for
tangential gratings (i.e., vertical gratings) rather than radial gratings, with meridional anisotropy opposite from the nonastigmatic participants.
In contrast to the nasal field, astigmatism did not affect the radial orientation bias in the inferior field of the astigmatic participants. The resolution acuity for both horizontal and vertical gratings was similar to that of the nonastigmatic participants. Note that visual inputs at peripheral field locations, even in the nonastigmatic eye, are anisotropic. In the inferior field, the retinal images contrast
15 and power spectral density for natural scenes
17,18 are lower for the horizontal (i.e.,
tangential) orientation than for the vertical orientation. While the presence of myopic WTR astigmatism may further blur the horizontal component of retinal images, it does not affect the radially biased visual input. This may explain why the astigmatic participants had the same pattern of meridional anisotropy (i.e., higher resolution acuity for vertical than horizontal gratings) in the inferior field as the nonastigmatic, although the mean difference in grating acuity between horizontal and vertical gratings in the astigmatic participants was slightly higher (MA: 24%; SM: 17%; EM: 18%).
This study intentionally recruited participants with compound myopic astigmatism. It was suspected that meridional anisotropy might be less predictable in hyperopic astigmatism, depending on the ocular accommodation status. Using compound hyperopic WTR astigmatism as an example, the two orthogonal line foci would be formed behind the retina when ocular accommodation is relaxed fully (horizontal line foci closer to the retina than the vertical). As such, vertical gratings are expected to be more blurred than horizontal gratings. However, when the eye accommodates, both of the image foci are brought closer to the retina. If the circle of least confusion falls onto the retinal plane, the retinal image quality along the principal meridians will be degraded equally. Additional ocular accommodation will further shift the image foci forward and reverse the orientation-dependent blur pattern, resulting in blurrier horizontal than vertical gratings. This unstable meridional blur may explain why meridional visual loss is usually less evident in hyperopic astigmats.
7,22 In the MA group with compound myopic astigmatism, the retinal image was far more stable and not much affected by ocular accommodation, providing an effective role model for determining how astigmatism affects meridional anisotropy. However, because this study included only myopic astigmatism, whether the results can be generalizable to hyperopic or mixed astigmatism requires further investigation.
It was not entirely clear whether myopia per se affects meridional anisotropy. It has been shown that radial orientation bias in the visual periphery originates from the radial arrangement of the retinal ganglion cells and their dendritic arborization, possibly due to the retina stretching radially as the eye grows.
25–29 Evidence from both clinical and laboratory studies has demonstrated that myopic eyes result from an overgrowth of the posterior eyeball,
30 which usually expands more for its height than its width.
31 Furthermore, recent studies have also revealed functional connectivity and morphological changes in the brains of high myopes, even though their best-corrected distance visual acuity was normal.
32–34 To remove the potential contribution of myopia to meridional anisotropy at peripheral visual fields, participants with emmetropia were recruited as a control group. However, no significant difference in the meridional anisotropy between the SM and EM groups was observed across all tested retinal locations. Thus, it is unlikely that myopia per se contributed to the difference in meridional anisotropy in the MA group.
Several measures were put in place to rule out interference of optical factors with resolution acuity measurement. First, on- and off-axis refractive errors determined respectively from subjective refraction and autorefraction were corrected by ophthalmic lenses when measuring resolution acuity. Second, high-contrast gratings were employed for the measurement of resolution acuity. While optical correction using conventional ophthalmic lenses could not fully compensate for higher-order aberrations at the peripheral visual field,
35 peripheral vision is highly resistant to optical blur. High-contrast visual acuity is largely unaffected by imposing defocus as high as 6D at 20° eccentricity.
20 The averaged high-contrast resolution acuity reported in this study (
Fig. 2b & c) fell within the range for resolution acuity at or close to 15° eccentricity of peripheral visual field ranges from 2.5 to 12 arcmin reported by previous studies.
20,36,37 The wide variations in measured peripheral resolution acuity obtained by previous studies could be attributable to the differences in psychophysical methods (method of adjustment,
20 2-up 1-down staircase,
33 and 1-up 1-down staircase
34), tested visual field locations, and participants’ refractive errors. Third, this study measured resolution acuity rather than detection acuity (i.e., identifying the presence or absence of gratings), even though the latter is also a common spatial acuity parameter. However, because detection acuity could be significantly affected by the amount of uncorrected optical blur,
20 it would be difficult to determine whether the meridional anisotropy, if any, is attributable to neural or optical factors. Lastly, the spectacle magnification difference was compensated between horizontal and vertical power meridians in the acuity measurement. Thus, it may be postulated that neural, rather than optical, factors modulate the orientation tuning in astigmatic eyes.
In conclusion, our study highlights the importance of astigmatism on meridional resolution acuity. Different meridional anisotropy patterns of the fovea and peripheral visual fields were characterized in patients with emmetropia, myopia, and astigmatism. Meridional resolution acuity can be affected by high astigmatism, even when corrected visual acuity is better than 20/20. It should be noted that only two peripheral visual fields were tested in our study. Caution should be applied before generalizing the findings to other retinal locations.