Open Access
Eye Movements, Strabismus, Amblyopia and Neuro-ophthalmology  |   February 2022
Effect of Viewing Conditions on Fixation Eye Movements and Eye Alignment in Amblyopia
Author Affiliations & Notes
  • Jordan Murray
    Visual Neurosciences and Ocular Motility Laboratory, Cole Eye Institute, Cleveland Clinic, Cleveland, Ohio, United States
  • Palak Gupta
    Department of Biomedical Engineering, Case Western Reserve University, Cleveland, Ohio, United States
    Daroff-Dell'Osso Ocular Motility Laboratory, Louis Stokes Cleveland VA Medical Center, Cleveland, Ohio, United States
  • Cody Dulaney
    Visual Neurosciences and Ocular Motility Laboratory, Cole Eye Institute, Cleveland Clinic, Cleveland, Ohio, United States
  • Kiran Garg
    Case Western Reserve University, Cleveland, Ohio, United States
  • Aasef G. Shaikh
    Department of Biomedical Engineering, Case Western Reserve University, Cleveland, Ohio, United States
    Daroff-Dell'Osso Ocular Motility Laboratory, Louis Stokes Cleveland VA Medical Center, Cleveland, Ohio, United States
    Department of Neurology, Neurological Institute, University Hospitals, Cleveland, Ohio, United States
  • Fatema F. Ghasia
    Visual Neurosciences and Ocular Motility Laboratory, Cole Eye Institute, Cleveland Clinic, Cleveland, Ohio, United States
    Daroff-Dell'Osso Ocular Motility Laboratory, Louis Stokes Cleveland VA Medical Center, Cleveland, Ohio, United States
  • Correspondence: Fatema Ghasia, 2022 E 105th Street, Cleveland, OH 44106, USA; [email protected]
Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science February 2022, Vol.63, 33. doi:https://doi.org/10.1167/iovs.63.2.33
  • Views
  • PDF
  • Share
  • Tools
    • Alerts
      ×
      This feature is available to authenticated users only.
      Sign In or Create an Account ×
    • Get Citation

      Jordan Murray, Palak Gupta, Cody Dulaney, Kiran Garg, Aasef G. Shaikh, Fatema F. Ghasia; Effect of Viewing Conditions on Fixation Eye Movements and Eye Alignment in Amblyopia. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 2022;63(2):33. https://doi.org/10.1167/iovs.63.2.33.

      Download citation file:


      © ARVO (1962-2015); The Authors (2016-present)

      ×
  • Supplements
Abstract

Purpose: Patients with amblyopia are known to have fixation instability, which arises from alteration of physiologic fixation eye movements (FEMs) and nystagmus. We assessed the effects of monocular, binocular, and dichoptic viewing on FEMs and eye alignment in patients with and without fusion maldevelopment nystagmus (FMN).

Methods: Thirty-four patients with amblyopia and seven healthy controls were recruited for this study. Eye movements were recorded using infrared video-oculography during (1) fellow eye viewing (FEV), (2) amblyopic eye viewing (AEV), (3) both eye viewing (BEV), and (4) dichoptic viewing (DcV) at varying fellow eye (FE) contrasts. The patients were classified per the clinical type of amblyopia and FEM waveforms into those without nystagmus, those with nystagmus with and without FMN. Fixational saccades and intersaccadic drifts, quick and slow phases of nystagmus, and bivariate contour ellipse area were analyzed in the FE and amblyopic eye (AE).

Results: We found that FEMs are differentially affected with increased amplitude of quick phases of FMN observed during AEV than BEV and during DcV at lower FE contrasts. Increased fixation instability was seen in anisometropic patients at lower FE contrasts. Incomitance of eye misalignment was seen with the greatest increase during FEV. Strabismic/mixed amblyopia patients without FMN were more likely to demonstrate a fixation switch where the AE attends to the target during DcV than patients with FMN.

Conclusions: Our findings suggest that FEM abnormalities modulate with different viewing conditions as used in various amblyopia therapies. Increased FEM abnormalities could affect the visual function deficits and may have treatment implications.

Amblyopia is a neurodevelopmental disorder that arises from binocularly discordant visual experience.1 Current amblyopia therapies comprise of part-time patching that forces the amblyopic eye (AE) to fixate at near and far distances under monocular viewing, whereas atropine penalization forces the AE to fixate at a near distance while the fellow eye (FE) vision is blurred under binocular viewing. Recent research suggests that interocular suppression is the key factor resulting in diminished binocularity and visual acuity in amblyopia.2,3 Newer therapies that aim to reverse the suppression by rebalancing the contrast presented to each eye (low contrast to the FE and high contrast to the AE under dichoptic viewing conditions) are being extensively investigated.47 Thus, amblyopia therapies aid recovery of vision through distinct mechanisms and require different viewing conditions. 
Fixation instability of the AE and, to some extent, the FE has been reported during monocular and binocular viewing.812 Normally, during attempted visual fixation, our eyes are not entirely still but instead show small involuntary physiologic “fixational eye movements” (FEMs). Physiologic FEMs produce motion that causes variability in eye position confined mainly within the foveola, thereby achieving the highest visual acuity, which is referred to as stable fixation.13 The FEMs comprise of microsaccades (fixational saccades), which are binocular conjugate movements with amplitude (<1 degree) that occur at 1 to 2 hertz (Hz) separated by intersaccadic drifts and tremors. Microsaccades, the fastest of all FEMs, have shown (1) to improve visual acuity by precisely relocating the fovea14,15; (2) to aid in contrast sensitivity estimates,16 and (3) are utilized to scan small and informative visual regions.17,18 Microsaccades and saccades share the same properties and are generated by the same neural circuitry.1924 Non-human primate (NHP) studies have shown that the microsaccades are generated within the rostral superior colliculus, whereas the larger visually guided saccades are generated within the caudal superior colliculus.2224 NHP studies have also shown that the micro-saccade production is associated with an increase in neural activity in cortical area V1.25 It helps explain the role of microsaccades in the maintenance of perception during steady visual fixation by thwarting neural adaptation and visual fading.2528 
Animal models of amblyopia have shown that the earliest functional and anatomic changes occur in area V1.2935 There is a shift in neuronal “acuity” to lower spatial frequencies, which results in impaired acuity and spatial resolution3638 with loss of V1 horizontal axonal connections between ocular dominance columns of the opposite eye, which are key components of fusion development. The effects of discordant input from the 2 eyes on the V1 neurons are greatest if they occur in the first 3 to 5 months of postnatal life at the time of emergence of stereopsis.3941 It causes an unbalanced middle temporal/medial superior temporal drive, where one hemisphere becomes more active,4244 resulting in persistence of nasal-ward bias that is the basis for developing fusion maldevelopment nystagmus (FMN).33,39,40,4548 Beyond infancy, discordant binocular input alters the architecture of V1 cortex with involvement of downstream subcortical eye movement sensitive areas resulting in unstable fixation.41,4951 We have characterized the fixation instability in patients with amblyopia and have found that these patients can have nystagmus with and without FMN.52 Patients with amblyopia without nystagmus have alterations of physiologic FEMs.53,54 These include increased amplitude of fast FEMs with a corresponding decrease in physiologic microsaccades and increased eye velocities of slow FEMs in AE than the FE, which correlate with the severity of visual acuity and stereo-acuity deficits.5254 The FEM abnormalities have shown to limit visual acuity in amblyopia.55 A prior study from our laboratory has shown reduced microsaccade and saccade frequencies with greater difficulties performing a visual search task, which correlated with increasing amblyopia severity.56 
Evidence suggests that FEM abnormalities can vary per the viewing conditions (e.g. the intensity of latent nystagmus now referred to as FMN) increases under monocular viewing.57,58 Forty percent of patients treated with part-time patching and atropine penalization of the FE have recurrent/residual amblyopia.59,60 The results from large randomized-control dichoptic treatment studies in children have been mixed.6163 There is a paucity of data on how the monocular, binocular, and dichoptic viewing as used in various amblyopia therapies affect FEMs and eye alignment. These metrics can have implications for treatment response6468 and can affect the ability to attend to the presented stimuli during therapies effectively.26,28,69 In the current paper, we examined the fixation stability, fast and slow FEM characteristics, and eye alignment under monocular (i.e. amblyopic eye viewing [AEV] and fellow eye viewing [FEV]), both eye viewing (BEV) and dichoptic viewing (DcV) with varying FE contrasts. We hypothesize that fixation stability, fast and slow FEMs of the FE and AE will be differentially modulated per the viewing conditions in patients with amblyopia with and without nystagmus. We also hypothesize that the eye alignment will be differentially affected in patients with and without nystagmus under different viewing conditions. 
Methods
The experiment protocols complied with the tenets of the Declaration of Helsinki, and were approved by the Cleveland Clinic Institutional Review Board. Informed consent was obtained from the study participants/parents or legal guardians on behalf of the minors/children. We recruited 34 subjects with amblyopia and 7 healthy controls. All the subjects had a comprehensive eye examination. 
Visual and Stereo-Acuity and Eye Movement Measurements
Psykinematix (KyberVision) software was used to generate test stimuli, which were displayed on a monitor with a resolution of 1280 × 800 at 60 Hz with a white luminance of 111 cd/m2 at a distance of 3.1 m in a dark room. Monocular distant visual acuity was assessed while the non-viewing eye was occluded. Subjects viewed one randomly selected Early Treatment Diabetic Retinopathy Study (ETDRS) optotype with crowding bars – the size was adjusted per a 2-down-1-up staircase with 6 reversals.70 The thresholds were taken to be the arithmetic mean of the reversals and converted into logMAR. The Titmus Stereoacuity Test was used to measure stereo-acuity in log arcsec. Patients with no stereo-acuity were assigned a value of 3.85 log arcsec. 
A high-resolution video-based eye tracker (EyeLink 1000) was used as described previously to measure binocular horizontal and vertical eye positions.52,56 Each subject's head was supported on a chin-rest, 84 cm away from the LCD screen. An infrared permissive filter was used to block visible light while allowing the non-viewing eye to be tracked. Monocular calibration and validation of each eye was done using a 5-point constellation with best-corrected vision in a dark room. 
The subjects fixated their gaze on a white circular target (0.5 degrees visual angle) projected against a black background on the LCD 30 inch monitor with a resolution of 2560 × 1600 at 60 Hz with brightness of 350 cd/m2. The recordings were obtained under BEV, FEV, and AEV. Recordings were also obtained under DcV, where the dot is presented independently but coincidently to each eye. To deliver different images to the right and left eyes, we used interleaved polarization: every even line was visible only to one eye, and every odd line was visible only to the other eye owing to opposite polarization. During the development of the program, calibrations were performed to ensure that the displayed images (to the right and left eyes) were identical at the pixel level. Subjects viewed a target on a 3D LCD 32 inch monitor with a resolution of 1920 × 1080 at 120 Hz with a brightness of 111 cd/m2. Each session began with both horizontal and vertical alignment of the dichoptic nonius lines. Subjects were shown a PowerPoint presentation on how the cross should appear for each eye and both eyes together, and they were asked to draw the image as observed. The image to one eye was the bottom and left side of the cross, whereas the image to the other eye was the top and right side of the cross. For subjects who were not able to view the entire cross, the contrast of the image of the nonius cross that was presented to the FE were reduced. With proper alignment, the image was a cross with a square cut out of the center, surrounded by four additional squares and a high contrast border that was visible in both eyes. The above stimulus was presented to all subjects. We found that the perception of the portion of the nonius cross that was presented to the AE was frequently transient despite lowering the contrast of the image of the FE, particularly in strabismic subjects. Subjects with strabismic amblyopia (including older participants) frequently verbalized the transient perception and fleeting location of the portion of the nonius cross as seen by the AE as they attempted the alignment procedure. The primary goal of the current study was to evaluate FEMs of the FE and AE eye as the FE contrast was varied. Thus, due to the transient visibility with the variable location of the nonius cross as perceived by several strabismic subjects, we did not shift the target dot location per the nonius cross measurements for consistency of the visual stimulus in the current experiment. Thus, a total of four trials were done under DcV. The target dot (0.5 degrees visual angle) was presented at the center of the screen – the contrast of the target presented to the AE was kept at 100% contrast for all trials, whereas the FE contrast varied from 100% (trial 1), 50% (trial 2), 25% (trial 3), and 10% (trial 4). Each trial lasted for 45 seconds. We used longer fixation times compared to most studies evaluating fixation instability in amblyopia in the literature.12,71 Our choice of longer fixation times allowed us to optimize micro-saccade detection and provide an adequate sample size for drift and microsaccade analysis. Our and other groups studying FEMS have utilized a similar trial duration to allow ample data collection for FEM analysis.53,54,7274 It also balances the amount of experiment time, so it does not appear to be too long, especially for our younger participants, whereas providing enough data for analysis of both eye alignment and FEMs. To maintain the subject's attention, we used a fixation window of 4 × 4 degrees and an auditory alert if the subject's gaze left the area of the fixation window for >500 msec. 
Data Analysis
The eye positions were analyzed using MatLab (MathWorks). Fixational saccades and quick phases of nystagmus were identified using the Engbert and Kleigl algorithm.7577 We pooled together the quick phases and fixational saccades as they share the same dynamic properties. We computed the frequency (Hertz) of fast FEMs (fixational saccade in controls and patients without nystagmus or quick phase in patients with nystagmus), as described in our previous work.52,56 Drifts and slow phases were defined as epochs between fixational saccades and quick phases in patients without and with nystagmus, respectively, as described previously.52,56 The composite amplitude (degrees) and median velocity(degrees/s) of fast and slow FEMs for each eye was calculated as Composite = [Horizontal2 + Vertical2]1/2
We computed the 25th, 50th, 75th, and 90th percentiles of the composite amplitude and velocity of the FE and AE for each subject obtained during each trial. 
The fixation stability was quantified by calculating the bivariate contour ellipse area (BCEA) encompassing 95% of fixation points12,71,78 and a log10 transformation was applied. Vergence BCEA values were also calculated for all subjects (left XY position-right XY position) on data obtained under monocular, binocular, and dichoptic viewing.79 
The eye position data were filtered with a moving average filter (window size 1.5 seconds) to remove fast eye movements. The filtered data were then used to compute the composite eye position for each eye. The difference in eye positions between the two eyes were used to calculate the eye alignment (degrees). A histogram of eye alignment for each trial/subject was plotted and the 95% lower and upper bounds were computed. 
The eye position traces obtained during BEV, FEV, and AEV were evaluated to categorize patients per their FEM waveforms (Fig. 1).52 Like controls, patients without nystagmus exhibited inter-saccadic drifts between fixational saccades. Patients with nystagmus were evaluated for the presence of FMN – a marker of disruption of binocularity in early infancy, defined as having a nasally directed slow phase under monocular viewing with the reversal in the direction of the quick phase toward the uncovered eye.80,81 Patients with jerk nystagmus/nystagmus-like movements with dynamic overshoots who did not exhibit this direction reversal were characterized as nystagmus without FMN. The slow phase velocities were decreasing or constant, in contrast to the increasing velocity seen in infantile nystagmus. In addition, patients with nystagmus without FMN did not have the dissociated vertical deviation frequently seen in FMN. 
Figure 1.
 
Examples of fixation eye movements during a 3 second epoch under conditions of (A) both eye viewing (BEV), (B) fellow eye viewing (FEV), and (C) amblyopic eye viewing (AEV) from a subject without nystagmus (top row), subject with fusion maldevelopment nystagmus (FMN) (middle row), and subject with nystagmus without FMN (bottom row). The x-axis represents time and the y-axis represents horizontal (solid line, black: fellow eye, and grey: amblyopic eye) and vertical (dotted line, black: fellow eye, and grey: amblyopic eye) positions. The black arrow represents the fast fixation eye movements (FEMs), whereas the grey arrows represent slow FEMs. Rightward and upward movements correspond to positive vertical axis.
Figure 1.
 
Examples of fixation eye movements during a 3 second epoch under conditions of (A) both eye viewing (BEV), (B) fellow eye viewing (FEV), and (C) amblyopic eye viewing (AEV) from a subject without nystagmus (top row), subject with fusion maldevelopment nystagmus (FMN) (middle row), and subject with nystagmus without FMN (bottom row). The x-axis represents time and the y-axis represents horizontal (solid line, black: fellow eye, and grey: amblyopic eye) and vertical (dotted line, black: fellow eye, and grey: amblyopic eye) positions. The black arrow represents the fast fixation eye movements (FEMs), whereas the grey arrows represent slow FEMs. Rightward and upward movements correspond to positive vertical axis.
We categorized the subjects per the clinical amblyopia type based on Pediatric Eye Disease Investigator Group (PEDIG) studies63 and FEM waveforms (Table 1). There were 13 anisometropic (10 = no nystagmus and 3 = nystagmus no FMN), 9 mixed (4 = no nystagmus, 4 = nystagmus no FMN, and 1 = FMN), and 12 strabismic (4 = nystagmus no FMN and 8 = FMN) patients with amblyopia. We wanted to examine the effects of different viewing conditions on changes in FEMs and eye alignment in patients with and without FMN. Thus, for analysis, the subjects were divided into four groups: controls (group 0), anisometropic (group 1), mixed/strabismic without FMN (group 2), and mixed/strabismic with FMN (group 3). Thirty percent of the amblyopic subjects had absent stereopsis (group A), 38% had intermediate level with some stereopsis defined as 3500 arc sec to worse than 140 sec arc (group B), and 32% had good stereopsis defined as 140 sec arc or better (group C). None of the subjects in group 1 had absent stereopsis whereas none of the subjects in group 3 had good stereopsis. 
Table 1.
 
Clinical and Demographic Characteristics of Participants
Table 1.
 
Clinical and Demographic Characteristics of Participants
The statistical analysis was performed using SPSS. Normality of data was evaluated using the Kolmogorov–Smirnov test. For all the tests, the Levene's test and the Mauchly's test of sphericity indicated that the assumption of homogeneity of variance and sphericity were met. FEM characteristics (log 95% BCEA, vergence BCEA, percentile data of composite amplitude and velocity, and frequency of fast FEMs) and eye alignment were analyzed separately using 2-way repeated-measures ANOVA (one within viewing conditions and one between subject factors – groups 0–3). We also analyzed the fast and slow FEMs and vergence fixation instability in amblyopic subjects per their stereo-acuity deficits using 2-way repeated-measures ANOVA (one within viewing conditions and one between subject factors) and have included the results in supplementary tables. Three-way repeated-measures ANOVA was used to examine the effects of varied contrasts during DcV on FE and AE fixation stability. Visual acuity and stereopsis across the four groups were compared using 1-way ANOVA. Post hoc analyses were conducted on significant main effects and interactions using Bonferroni correction. Mann-Whitney U test was used to compare the age between controls versus amblyopic subjects. All statistical tests had a critical alpha value of 0.05. 
Results
We examined the effects of monocular, binocular, and dichoptic viewing on fast and slow FEMs, fixation stability, and eye alignment across the four groups. There were no differences in age (years) between controls (age range = 6–44 years) versus amblyopes (controls =14.6 ± 13.3, amblyopes = 16.4 ± 13.3, P = 0.7). There was no difference in the AE visual acuity (logMAR) across the amblyopes (group 1 = 0.41 ± 0.31, group 2 = 0.39 ± 0.33, and group 3 = 0.27 ± 0.21, F(2,33) = 0.5, P = 0.5). The stereopsis (log arcsec) was significantly worse in group 3 (group 1 = 2.0 ± 0.2, group 2 = 3.0 ± 0.8, and group 3 = 3.5 ± 0.5, F(2,33) = 18.7, P < 0.0001, Bonferroni correction group 1 vs. 2 P = 0.001, and group 1 vs. 3 P < 0.0001). 
Monocular and Binocular Viewing
Fast and Slow FEMs
Table 2 depicts the frequencies of fast FEMs obtained during BEV, FEV, and AEV and were greatest in group 3. There was no effect of viewing conditions on frequencies F(2,70) = 0.96, P = 0.38, ηp2 = 0.02 with no interaction between viewing conditions and groups, F(6,70) = 0.29, P = 0.93, ηp2 = 0.02. Figure 2 summarizes the normalized cumulative sum histogram of the composite amplitude of fast FEMs of the FE (top) and AE (bottom) during monocular and binocular viewing in groups 0 and 1 (Figs. 2A, 2C) and in groups 0, 2, and 3 (Figs. 2B, 2D). There is a rightward shift of the distribution in the amplitude of AE during AEV than BEV particularly for patients in group 3, which was significant for the 25th and 50th percentiles (Table 3). We analyzed the percentile data as a function of stereopsis deficits obtained under FEV, AEV, and BEV. We found that for the 25th percentile the amplitude of the AE was greater during AEV than during BEV (Supplementary Table S1). 
Table 2.
 
Frequency of Fast FEMs During Monocular and Binocular Viewing Across the Four Groups
Table 2.
 
Frequency of Fast FEMs During Monocular and Binocular Viewing Across the Four Groups
Figure 2.
 
Cumulative sum histogram of composite amplitude (degrees) of fast fixation eye movements of fellow eye obtained during fellow eye viewing and both eye viewing in groups 0 vs. 1 (A) and groups 0, 1, and 2 (B) and amblyopic eye obtained during amblyopic eye viewing and both eye viewing in groups 0 vs. 1 (C) and groups 0, 1, and 2 (D).
Figure 2.
 
Cumulative sum histogram of composite amplitude (degrees) of fast fixation eye movements of fellow eye obtained during fellow eye viewing and both eye viewing in groups 0 vs. 1 (A) and groups 0, 1, and 2 (B) and amblyopic eye obtained during amblyopic eye viewing and both eye viewing in groups 0 vs. 1 (C) and groups 0, 1, and 2 (D).
Table 3.
 
Percentile of Composite Amplitude and Velocity of Fast and Slow Fixation Eye Movements of Fellow Eye and Amblyopic Eye During Monocular and Binocular Viewing Across the Four Groups
Table 3.
 
Percentile of Composite Amplitude and Velocity of Fast and Slow Fixation Eye Movements of Fellow Eye and Amblyopic Eye During Monocular and Binocular Viewing Across the Four Groups
Figure 3 summarizes the normalized cumulative sum histogram of the composite velocity of slow FEMs of the FE (top) and AE (bottom) during monocular and binocular viewing in groups 0 and 1 (Figs. 3A, 3C) and groups 0, 2, and 3 (Figs. 3B, 3D). There is a rightward shift of the distribution of the velocity of the AE during AEV than BEV in patients in group 3. We analyzed the percentiles and found that the velocity of the AE is greater during AEV than BEV in group 3 with a significant interaction for the 90th percentile (see Table 3). We analyzed the percentile data of the composite velocity as a function of stereopsis deficits and did not see any statistical significance across groups and viewing conditions (see Supplementary Table S1). 
Figure 3.
 
Cumulative sum histogram of composite eye velocity (degrees/sec) of slow fixation eye movements of fellow eye obtained during fellow eye viewing and both eye viewing in groups 0 vs. 1 (A) and groups 0, 1, and 2 (B) and amblyopic eye obtained during amblyopic eye viewing and both eye viewing in groups 0 vs. 1 (C) and groups 0, 1, and 2 (D).
Figure 3.
 
Cumulative sum histogram of composite eye velocity (degrees/sec) of slow fixation eye movements of fellow eye obtained during fellow eye viewing and both eye viewing in groups 0 vs. 1 (A) and groups 0, 1, and 2 (B) and amblyopic eye obtained during amblyopic eye viewing and both eye viewing in groups 0 vs. 1 (C) and groups 0, 1, and 2 (D).
Fixation Stability
We compared the log BCEA of the FE during FEV (group 0 = 0.38 ± 0.35, group 1 = 0.52 ± 0.35, group 2 = 0.64 ± 0.53, and group 3 = 0.37 ± 0.18) and BEV (group 0 = 0.4 ± 0.41, group 1 = 0.53 ± 0.358, group 2 = 0.50 ± 0.35, and group 3 = 0.36 ± 0.26). There was no effect of viewing conditions on log BCEA of FE, F(1,36) = 0.04, P = 0.83, ηp2 = 0.001 with no interaction between viewing conditions and groups, F(3,36) = 1.5, P = 0.2, ηp2 = 0.11. Similarly, we found no difference in log BCEA of the AE during AEV (group 0 = 0.39 ± 0.44, group 1 = 0.73 ± 0.41, group 2 = 0.6 ± 0.36, and group 3 = 0.61 ± 0.35) and BEV (group 0 = 0.4 ± 0.45, group 1 = 0.72 ± 0.49, group 2 = 0.79 ± 0.42, and group 3 = 0.69 ± 0.43) F(1,36) = 1.67, P = 0.21, ηp2 = 0.04 with no interaction between viewing conditions and groups F(3,36) = 0.50, P = 0.68, ηp2 = 0.04. The log BCEA of the AE is higher than FE and controls.12,53,55 
We analyzed the log vergence BCEA and found that the values were greater during AEV (group 0 = 0.73 ± 0.48, group 1 = 0.91 ± 0.54, group 2 = 0.94 ± 0.45, and group 3 = 1.06 ± 0.27) and FEV (group 0 = 0.65 ± 0.48, group 1 = 0.73 ± 0.42, group 2 = 0.91 ± 0.29, and group 3 = 0.99 ± 0.22) than during BEV (group 0 = 0.47 ± 0.36, group 1 = 0.74 ± 0.48, group 2 = 0.63 ± 0.37, and group 3 = 0.76 ± 0.35) for all groups, F(2,72) = 8.0, P = 0.001, ηp2 = 0.18 with no interaction between viewing conditions and groups, F(6,72) = 0.56, P = 0.75, ηp2 = 0.04. This is in agreement with prior studies which have shown higher vergence BCEA in patients with amblyopia than controls.52 
We also analyzed log vergence BCEA as a function of stereoacuity deficits and found a similar result where the values were greater during AEV (group A = 0.97 ± 0.30, group B = 1.19 ± 0.47, group C = 0.65 ± 0.35, and group D = 0.73 ± 0.48) and FEV (group A = 0.95 ± 0.27, group B = 0.97 ± 0.30, group C = 0.65 ± 0.37, and group D = 0.65 ± 0.48) than during BEV (group A = 0.77 ± 0.31, group B = 0.82 ± 0.39, group C = 0.48 ± 0.45, and group D = 0.47 ± 0.36) for all groups, F(2,72) = 7.5, P = 0.001, ηp2 = 0.17 with no interaction between viewing conditions and groups, F(6,72) = 0.43, P = 0.85, ηp2 = 0.03. In agreement with previous studies, we found that amblyopic subjects with good stereopsis had lower vergence BCEA suggestive of less vergence instability than those with some or absent stereopsis.52 
Eye Alignment
Figure 4 depicts the scatter plot of eye positions in a subject from group 3 during BEV, FEV, and AEV. The bottom panel depicts the histogram of composite eye position differences of the right and left eyes from the same subject. The 95% upper and lower bounds of the histogram are greatest during FEV. We calculated the 95% upper and lower bounds of the histogram of eye alignment obtained from each subject/trial and pooled the values across the 4 groups (Table 4). We found that the eye misalignment was greatest during FEV for all amblyopic subjects with most increases in group 3 (lower bound: main effect F(2,70) = 3.9, P = 0.02, ηp2 = 0.1), pairwise comparison BEV versus FEV: 0.009, interaction (F(6,70) = 2.2, P = 0.049, ηp2 = 0.16), pairwise comparison group 0 vs. 3: P = 0.007; upper bound: main effect (F(2,70) = 3.4, P = 0.03, ηp2 = 0.09), pairwise comparison BEV versus FEV: 0.005, interaction (F(6,70) = 2.1, P = 0.06, ηp2 = 0.15), pairwise comparison group 0 vs. 3: P = 0.006). 
Figure 4.
 
Top: Horizontal and vertical eye positions of fellow eye (black) and amblyopic eye (grey) during both eye viewing (BEV), fellow eye viewing (FEV), and amblyopic eye viewing (AEV). Rightward and upward movements correspond to positive vertical axis. Notice the right hypotropia and exotropia during BEV and FEV condition and left exotropia and left hypertropia during AEV. Bottom: Histogram showing range of composite eye position difference during BEV (grey), FEV (black), and AEV (white).
Figure 4.
 
Top: Horizontal and vertical eye positions of fellow eye (black) and amblyopic eye (grey) during both eye viewing (BEV), fellow eye viewing (FEV), and amblyopic eye viewing (AEV). Rightward and upward movements correspond to positive vertical axis. Notice the right hypotropia and exotropia during BEV and FEV condition and left exotropia and left hypertropia during AEV. Bottom: Histogram showing range of composite eye position difference during BEV (grey), FEV (black), and AEV (white).
Table 4.
 
Eye Alignment (Degrees) During Monocular and Binocular Viewing Across the Four Groups
Table 4.
 
Eye Alignment (Degrees) During Monocular and Binocular Viewing Across the Four Groups
Dichoptic Viewing at Varied FE Contrasts
Fast and Slow FEMs
Table 5 depicts the frequencies of fast FEMs obtained during DcV and were greatest in group 3. There was no effect of DcV at varied FE contrasts on frequencies of FEMs F(3,9) = 0.25, P = 0.85, ηp2 = 0.008 with no interaction between FE contrasts and groups, F(9,90) = 1.5, P = 0.16, ηp2 = 0.13. Figure 5 summarizes the normalized cumulative sum histogram of the composite amplitude of fast FEMs of the FE (top) and AE (bottom) during DcV at varied FE contrasts in groups 0 and 1 (see Figs. 5A, 5C) and in groups 0, 2, and 3 (see Figs. 5B, 5D). There is a rightward shift of the distribution of amplitude of FE and AE of all amblyopes than controls which was most pronounced in group 3. For all percentiles, the amplitude of the FE and AE is greater when FE contrast is 10% vs. 100% and for the 50th and 75th percentiles when FE contrast is 25% vs. 100%. There was no statistically significant interaction across groups as a function of various FE contrasts (Table 6). We also analyzed the percentile data of the FE and AE as a function of stereopsis deficits during DcV. We found that for 25th, 50th, and 75th percentiles, the amplitude of the FE and AE were greater when FE contrast is 10% vs. 100% and for the 50th percentiles when FE contrast is 25% vs. 100%. There was no statistically significant interaction across groups as a function of various FE contrasts (Supplementary Table S2). 
Table 5.
 
Frequency of Fast Fixation Eye Movements (Hertz) During Dichoptic Viewing Across the Four Groups
Table 5.
 
Frequency of Fast Fixation Eye Movements (Hertz) During Dichoptic Viewing Across the Four Groups
Figure 5.
 
Cumulative sum histogram of composite amplitude (degrees) of fast fixation eye movements of fellow eye and amblyopic eye obtained during dichoptic viewing across fellow eye contrasts of 100% (black), 50% (darkest grey), 25% (lighter grey), and 10% (lightest grey) in groups 0 vs. 1 (A, C) and groups 0, 1, and 2 (B, D).
Figure 5.
 
Cumulative sum histogram of composite amplitude (degrees) of fast fixation eye movements of fellow eye and amblyopic eye obtained during dichoptic viewing across fellow eye contrasts of 100% (black), 50% (darkest grey), 25% (lighter grey), and 10% (lightest grey) in groups 0 vs. 1 (A, C) and groups 0, 1, and 2 (B, D).
Table 6.
 
Percentile Composite Amplitude (Degrees) of Fast Fixation Eye Movements of Fellow Eye and Amblyopic Eye during Dichoptic Viewing Across the Four Groups
Table 6.
 
Percentile Composite Amplitude (Degrees) of Fast Fixation Eye Movements of Fellow Eye and Amblyopic Eye during Dichoptic Viewing Across the Four Groups
Figure 6 summarizes the normalized cumulative sum histogram of the composite velocity of slow FEMs of the FE (top) and AE (bottom) during DcV in groups 0 and 1 (see Figs. 6A, 6C) and groups 0, 2, and 3 (see Figs. 6B, 6D). There is a rightward shift of the distribution of velocity of FE and AE of all amblyopes than controls which was most pronounced in group 3. We computed the percentiles of the composite velocity of the FE and AE for each subject obtained at various FE contrasts and found no significant differences across the four groups (Table 7). We also computed the percentiles of the composite velocity of the FE and AE for each subject obtained at various FE contrasts under DcV per the stereopsis deficits and found no significant differences across the groups (Supplementary Table S3). 
Figure 6.
 
Cumulative sum histogram of composite velocity (degrees/sec) of slow fixation eye movements of fellow eye and amblyopic eye obtained during dichoptic viewing across fellow eye contrasts of 100% (black), 50% (darkest grey), 25% (lighter grey), and 10% (lightest grey) in groups 0 vs. 1 (A, C) and groups 0, 1, and 2 (B, D).
Figure 6.
 
Cumulative sum histogram of composite velocity (degrees/sec) of slow fixation eye movements of fellow eye and amblyopic eye obtained during dichoptic viewing across fellow eye contrasts of 100% (black), 50% (darkest grey), 25% (lighter grey), and 10% (lightest grey) in groups 0 vs. 1 (A, C) and groups 0, 1, and 2 (B, D).
Table 7.
 
Composite Velocity of Slow Fixation Eye Movements of Fellow Eye and Amblyopic Eye During Dichoptic Viewing Across the Four Groups
Table 7.
 
Composite Velocity of Slow Fixation Eye Movements of Fellow Eye and Amblyopic Eye During Dichoptic Viewing Across the Four Groups
Fixation Stability
Table 8 depicts fixation stability of the right eye (100% contrast) and the left eye (varied contrasts) in group 0 and FE (varied contrasts) and AE (100% contrast) in group 1. We conducted a 3-way ANOVA evaluating the effects of FE contrasts on FE and AE fixation stability. We found a statistically significant three-way interaction between FE contrasts, eye in groups 0 vs. 1, F(3,48) = 2.8, P = 0.04, ηp2 = 0.15. There was a significant simple two-way interaction between FE contrast and eye for group 0, F(3,18) = 6.61, P = 0.015, ηp2 = 0.52 but not for group 1, F(3,30) = 0.32, P = 0.8, ηp2 = 0.03. The simple main effect of varying contrast for group 0 in the left eye was statistically significant, F(318) = 4.5, P = 0.015, ηp2 = 0.43 but not in the right eye, F(3,18) = 0.33, P = 0.8, ηp2 = 0.05. The results suggest the differential effects on fixation stability of the two eyes in group 0, where the fixation instability of the left eye whose contrast was varied increased without any change in the fixation instability of the right eye. The fixation instability of both the FE and AE was greater in group 1 and increased further with reduced FE contrast particularly at 25% and 10%. 
Table 8.
 
Bivariate Contour Ellipse Area of FE and AE During Dichoptic Viewing in Controls and Patients With Anisometropic Amblyopia
Table 8.
 
Bivariate Contour Ellipse Area of FE and AE During Dichoptic Viewing in Controls and Patients With Anisometropic Amblyopia
The log vergence BCEA was higher in patients with amblyopia compared to controls during DcV (Table 9). There was an effect of various FE contrasts under DcV on the vergence BCEA, F(3,90) = 3.0, P = 0.03, ηp2 = 0.09 with no interaction between viewing conditions and groups, F(9,90) = 0.58, P = 0.80, ηp2 = 0.05. The log vergence BCEA was higher in amblyopic patients particularly those with absent and some stereopsis during DcV (Supplementary Table S4). There was an effect of various FE contrasts under DcV on the vergence BCEA, F(3,90) = 3.3, P = 0.03, ηp2 = 0.09 with no interaction between viewing conditions and groups, F(9,90) = 1.3, P = 0.24, ηp2 = 0.11. 
Table 9.
 
Vergence Bivariate Contour Ellipse Area During Dichoptic Viewing Across the Four Groups
Table 9.
 
Vergence Bivariate Contour Ellipse Area During Dichoptic Viewing Across the Four Groups
Eye Alignment
Figure 7 plots eye positions of the FE and AE from 2 subjects (left- subject 1 = group 2 and right-subject 2 = group 3). In subject 1, the AE picked up fixation during DcV at 25% and 10% FE contrast (fixation switch) (i.e. the AE attends to presented target). In subject 2, the FE fixates with an increase in eye misalignment at lower FE contrasts (arrows) with no fixation switch (i.e. the AE did not attend to the target). We found that 5 of 12 patients in group 2 vs. 1 of 9 patients in group 3 exhibited a fixation switch during DcV at lower FE contrasts. 
Figure 7.
 
Horizontal and vertical eye position traces (vertical axis) and time (horizontal axis) in 2 subjects obtained during dichoptic viewing at fellow eye contrasts of (A) 100%, (B) 50%, (C) 25%, and (D) 10% contrast. To the left is a subject with mixed/strabismic amblyopia without fusion maldevelopment nystagmus (FMN) that demonstrates a fixation switch indicated by the black arrow when the fellow eye is fixing at FE contrast of 25% and the grey arrow that represents the amblyopic eye picks up with fixation. The horizontal arrows on the far left represent the primary position. To the right is a subject with mixed/strabismic amblyopia with fusion maldevelopment nystagmus (FMN). Notice that through all the trials A to D the fellow eye fixates in primary position with increase in eye misalignment at lower fellow eye contrasts indicated by the vertical black arrows on the far right.
Figure 7.
 
Horizontal and vertical eye position traces (vertical axis) and time (horizontal axis) in 2 subjects obtained during dichoptic viewing at fellow eye contrasts of (A) 100%, (B) 50%, (C) 25%, and (D) 10% contrast. To the left is a subject with mixed/strabismic amblyopia without fusion maldevelopment nystagmus (FMN) that demonstrates a fixation switch indicated by the black arrow when the fellow eye is fixing at FE contrast of 25% and the grey arrow that represents the amblyopic eye picks up with fixation. The horizontal arrows on the far left represent the primary position. To the right is a subject with mixed/strabismic amblyopia with fusion maldevelopment nystagmus (FMN). Notice that through all the trials A to D the fellow eye fixates in primary position with increase in eye misalignment at lower fellow eye contrasts indicated by the vertical black arrows on the far right.
Figure 8 depicts the scatter plot of eye positions (right panel) in the same subject as used in Figure 4 during DcV. The left panel depicts the histogram of composite eye position differences of the right and left eyes for each trial. We calculated the 95% upper and lower bounds of the histogram of eye alignment obtained from each subject at various FE contrasts and pooled the values across the 4 groups (Table 10). There was no statistically significant difference across the four groups (lower bound: main effect F(3,81) = 1.5, P = 0.19, ηp2 = 0.05), interaction (F(9,81) = 0.44, P = 0.9, ηp2 = 0.04), and (upper bound: main effect (F(3,81) = 2.3, P = 0.09, ηp2 = 0.08), and interaction (F(9,81) = 0.96, P = 0.47, ηp2 = 0.09). 
Figure 8.
 
Right: Horizontal and vertical eye positions of fellow eye (black) and amblyopic eye (grey) obtained from the same subject shown in Figure 4 during dichoptic viewing across fellow eye contrasts of 100% (A), 50% (B), 25% (C), and 10% (D). Rightward and upward movements correspond to positive vertical axis. Notice the right hypotropia and exotropia during all dichoptic viewing conditions. Left: Histogram showing range of composite eye position difference during dichoptic viewing across the fellow eye contrasts of 100% (A), 50% (B), 25% (C), and 10% (D).
Figure 8.
 
Right: Horizontal and vertical eye positions of fellow eye (black) and amblyopic eye (grey) obtained from the same subject shown in Figure 4 during dichoptic viewing across fellow eye contrasts of 100% (A), 50% (B), 25% (C), and 10% (D). Rightward and upward movements correspond to positive vertical axis. Notice the right hypotropia and exotropia during all dichoptic viewing conditions. Left: Histogram showing range of composite eye position difference during dichoptic viewing across the fellow eye contrasts of 100% (A), 50% (B), 25% (C), and 10% (D).
Table 10.
 
Eye Alignment (Degrees) During Dichoptic Viewing Across The Four Groups
Table 10.
 
Eye Alignment (Degrees) During Dichoptic Viewing Across The Four Groups
Discussion
We categorized patients with amblyopia per the clinical type and the FEM waveforms and examined FEMs and eye alignment under different viewing conditions. The main findings were (1) FEMs are differentially affected with increased amplitude of quick phases of FMN observed during AEV than BEV and incomitance of eye misalignment with increased strabismus angles observed during FEV than BEV, (2) reduction of contrasts as used during DcV results in increase fixation and vergence instability, (3) there was a significant increase in the amplitude of quick phases with reducing the contrast of the FE during DcV in patients with FMN, and (4) strabismic/mixed amblyopia patients without FMN demonstrate a fixation switch where the AE attends to the target presented during DcV at lower FE contrasts, whereas patients with FMN were less likely to demonstrate the fixation switch. 
Vision is used to optimize eye movements. Thus, eye movements elicited during a target's fixation reflect the visual sensory and motor functions. They constitute an excellent quantitative end point of a final common pathway. Abnormalities of fast (increased saccadic intrusions) and slow (increased inter-saccadic drifts) FEMs are seen in patients with low vision, such as those with age-related macular degeneration.8285 Studies from our and other laboratories have found that fixation instability in amblyopia limits the visual acuity.56,5355,86 We have described features per the FEM waveforms that reflect the severity of visual acuity and stereo acuity deficits.52 We have also found that patients with amblyopia with nystagmus have greater impairment of visual functions with increased amplitude of quick phase, eye position variance, and velocities of the slow phases compared to patients without nystagmus.52,65,87 These collectively suggest that increased fixation instability with increased amplitude and velocities of fast and slow FEMs in amblyopia are associated with worse visual function deficits. 
In the current study, we found that the FEM abnormalities are differentially affected per the viewing condition, with greater amplitude of the fast FEMs of the AE during AEV than BEV for all patients with amblyopia, which was most pronounced for patients with FMN. The slow phase velocity of AE of patients with FMN increased substantially under AEV than BEV. Thus, we found better fixation stability under binocular viewing than monocular viewing in agreement with previous studies.12,52,71 Incomitance of strabismus angle during right eye versus left eye viewing has been reported in patients with intermittent exotropia without paralytic strabismus.8 We also found that eye misalignment (esotropia/exotropia) is greater during FEV than BEV. Engagement of vergence and fusion mechanisms with binocular summation could result in better fixation stability and eye alignment during BEV.50,88 Thus, collectively, the results suggest that visual input is a key factor affecting fixation stability and eye alignment control. 
Reducing the contrast of stimuli presented to the FE is thought to overcome the suppression of AE and aid the visual recovery and is the basis of dichoptic treatments.2,89,90 Thus, we would expect the fixation instability of the AE to improve while lowering the FE contrast during DcV as it reduces suppression. We found that the reduction of contrast is associated with increased BCEA values suggestive of greater fixation instability of that eye in controls and FE of patients with anisometropic amblyopia. Patients with anisometropic amblyopia also exhibited an increase in the amplitude of AE at lower FE contrasts reflected by an increase in the instability of the AE. Thus, contrary to our expected results of reduced FEM abnormalities in the AE, we saw an increase in FEM abnormalities as the FE contrasts are reduced. We have previously shown that FEM abnormalities of the non-viewing eye under monocular viewing are driven primarily by the viewing eye (i.e. the FEM abnormalities of the FE are increased when the AE is viewing and the FEM abnormalities of the AE are reduced when the FE is viewing).53 Thus, the worsening of FE fixation stability at lower FE contrasts likely drives the increased AE fixation instability under DcV. This is in agreement with a prior report examining fixation stability in adult patients with anisometropic amblyopia and controls during DcV.91 
In strabismic amblyopia, reduced AE fixation instability appears to be associated with a loss of foveation and suppression, both of which degrade fusion and vergence.91,92 Thus, we expect the AE fixation instability to improve when rebalancing the inter-ocular contrast overcomes and reduces the suppression. Our study found that several patients with strabismic/mixed amblyopia were unable to overcome suppression with contrast rebalancing between the two eyes.92 Subjects with strabismus who were able to perceive the nonius cross frequently verbalized that the perception was transient and the location of the nonius cross would vary as they attempted the alignment procedure. This is in agreement with previous studies that have shown that in patients with strabismic/mixed amblyopia where the balance point can be determined, there is a transient improvement in AE fixation stability with subsequent drifting of the eye away from foveal alignment.92 In our cohort, we found an overall increase in amplitude of FE and AE in patients with mixed/strabismic amblyopia with and without FMN, particularly at lower FE contrasts. We also found a similar change in amplitude of FE and AE in all patients with amblyopia at lower FE contrasts irrespective of their stereo-acuity deficits. An increase in the amplitude of the AE may be driven by the changes in the FE fast FEMs amplitude at lower FE contrasts. The collective increase in the FE and AE instability would also affect the vergence instability, which was seen in all subjects particularly in subjects with greater stereo-acuity deficits. 
The DcV recordings were performed without offsetting the target per the subjective nonius cross alignment due to the nonius cross's transient perception and fleeting location in the patients with strabismic/mixed amblyopia. We found a small and variable change in eye alignment under DcV at varied FE contrasts. Interestingly, we also found that patients with strabismic/mixed amblyopia without FMN were more likely to demonstrate a fixation switch where the AE fixates on the presented target during DcV than those with FMN. Thus, the current experiments provide novel evidence that the AE can overcome the suppression at lower FE contrasts and attend to the presented target, as demonstrated by the fixation switch on eye movement recordings. NHP studies have shown that loss of horizontal cells in area V1 due to disruption of binocularity is the necessary and sufficient cause for FMN development and contributes to inter-ocular suppression and stereopsis deficits.40,47,93 We have shown that patients with FMN had worse stereopsis deficits than patients without FMN.70 In the current study, most of the patients with FMN had absent stereopsis. We examined the inter-ocular suppression using dichoptic motion coherence, and preliminary analysis demonstrates that patients with FMN had greater inter-ocular suppression for a given level of visual acuity deficits. Thus, we speculate that the poor binocularity and greater inter-ocular suppression in patients with FMN could be a contributing factor to lack of fixation switch seen during DcV. 
We have previously examined characteristics of FEMs during binocular viewing in school-aged children versus adults and did not find differences in frequency of fixational saccades or inter-saccadic drifts. The amplitude of fixational saccades was slightly higher in children than adults [median fixation saccade amplitude in adults was 0.4 degrees; 25 and 75 percentiles = 0.26–0.6 degrees, whereas in children it was 0.55 degrees (25 and 75 percentiles = 0.36–0.84 degrees)].74 The current study is the largest cohort of patients with amblyopic with FEM measurements obtained under DcV at various contrasts, monocular and binocular viewing. The majority of patients with amblyopia (27/34) were <18 years of age, thus limiting our ability to analyze differences in changes in FEM abnormalities as a function of age. Future studies systematically investigating the interactions of age and viewing conditions on FEM abnormalities in patients with amblyopia with and without nystagmus are warranted. 
The central research emphasis in the field is to create new amblyopia treatments, however; both conventional and emerging treatments have produced mixed results.46,59,60 Most amblyopia therapies take effect through changing viewing conditions. We have found that fixation instability and FEM abnormalities are sensitive to viewing conditions (i.e. the high-speed eye trackers can capture the modulation of FEMs in response to transient changes in the viewing conditions). Increased fixation instability or greater FEM abnormalities during a given viewing condition can affect visual functions and could potentially render the amblyopia treatment less effective.94,95 Similarly, changes in strabismus angle across various viewing conditions should be considered while planning strabismus repair and determining the likelihood of developing diplopia with amblyopia treatments.6,96 Development of treatment protocols designed per the optimal conditions when FEM abnormalities are minimal could improve outcomes. Thus, future studies quantifying FEMs before and during treatment are critical. It may serve as a missing link to understand the mechanisms responsible for varying therapeutic efficacy seen with current treatment regimens. 
Acknowledgments
Funding information: NEI T32: 5 T32 EY 24236-4 (J.M.), Case Western Reserve University Biomedical Research Fellowship - Hartwell Foundation (C.D.), Summer Student Research Fellowship Case Western Reserve University (K.G.), American Academy of Neurology Career Development Award (A.S.), American Parkinson's Disease Association Cotzias fellowship (A.S.), Dystonia Medical Research Foundation research grant (A.S.), Department of Veterans Affairs Merit Review (A.S.), Blind Children's Foundation grant (F.G.), and Research to Prevent Blindness Disney Amblyopia Award (F.G.), CWRT CTSC Pilot Grant Program (F.G.), Cleveland Clinic RPC Grant (F.G.), Lerner Research Institute Artificial Intelligence in Medicine (F.G.), Departmental Grants from Research to Prevent Blindness, Unrestricted Block Grant CCLCM, NIH-NEI P30 Core Grant Award and Cleveland Eye Bank. 
Disclosure: J. Murray, None; P. Gupta, None; C. Dulaney, None; K. Garg, None; A.G. Shaikh, None; F.F. Ghasia, None 
References
Hatt SR, Leske DA, Castaneda YS, et al. Understanding the Impact of Residual Amblyopia on Functional Vision and Eye-related Quality of Life Using the PedEyeQ. Am J Ophthalmol. 2020; 218: 173–181. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Li J, Thompson B, Lam CS, et al. The role of suppression in amblyopia. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2011; 52: 4169–4176. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Sengpiel F, Blakemore C. The neural basis of suppression and amblyopia in strabismus. Eye (Lond). 1996; 10: 250–258. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Gao TY, Guo CX, Babu RJ, et al. Effectiveness of a Binocular Video Game vs Placebo Video Game for Improving Visual Functions in Older Children, Teenagers, and Adults With Amblyopia: A Randomized Clinical Trial. JAMA Ophthalmol. 2018; 136: 172–181. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Hunter DG. Treatment of amblyopia in older children. Arch Ophthalmol. 2005; 123: 557–558. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Piano MEF, Simmers AJ. ‘It's too late’. Is it really? Considerations for amblyopia treatment in older children. Ther Adv Ophthalmol. 2019; 11: 2515841419857379. [PubMed]
Pediatric Eye Disease Investigator Group, Holmes JM, Manny RE, et al. A Randomized Trial of Binocular Dig Rush Game Treatment for Amblyopia in Children Aged 7 to 12 Years. Ophthalmology. 2019; 126: 456–466. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Economides JR, Adams DL, Horton JC. Variability of Ocular Deviation in Strabismus. JAMA Ophthalmol. 2016; 134: 63–69. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Ghasia FF, Otero-Millan J, Shaikh AG. Abnormal fixational eye movements in strabismus. Br J Ophthalmol. 2018; 102: 253–259. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Pirdankar OH, Das VE. Influence of Target Parameters on Fixation Stability in Normal and Strabismic Monkeys. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2016; 57: 1087–1095. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Richards M, Wong A, Foeller P, Bradley D, Tychsen L. Duration of binocular decorrelation predicts the severity of latent (fusion maldevelopment) nystagmus in strabismic macaque monkeys. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2008; 49: 1872–1878. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Subramanian V, Jost RM, Birch EE. A quantitative study of fixation stability in amblyopia. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2013; 54: 1998–2003. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Putnam NM, Hofer HJ, Doble N, Chen L, Carroll J, Williams DR. The locus of fixation and the foveal cone mosaic. J Vision. 2005; 5: 632–639. [CrossRef]
Ko HK, Poletti M, Rucci M. Microsaccades precisely relocate gaze in a high visual acuity task. Nat Neurosci. 2010; 13: 1549–1553. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Poletti MLC, Rucci M. Microscopic eye movements compensate for nonhomogeneous vision within the fovea. Curr Biol. 2013; 23(17): 1691–1695. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Denniss J, Scholes C, McGraw PV, Nam SH, Roach NW. Estimation of Contrast Sensitivity From Fixational Eye Movements. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2018; 59: 5408–5416. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Otero-Millan J, Troncoso XG, Macknik SL, Serrano-Pedraza I, Martinez-Conde S. Saccades and microsaccades during visual fixation, exploration, and search: foundations for a common saccadic generator. J Vis. 2008; 8: 21–28. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Ghasia FF, Shaikh AG. Uncorrected Myopic Refractive Error Increases Microsaccade Amplitude. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2015; 56: 2531–2535. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Otero-Millan J, Macknik SL, Serra A, Leigh RJ, Martinez-Conde S. Triggering mechanisms in microsaccade and saccade generation: a novel proposal. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2011; 1233: 107–116. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Hafed ZM, Goffart L, Krauzlis RJ. A neural mechanism for microsaccade generation in the primate superior colliculus. Science. 2009;323.
Zuber BL, Stark L, Cook G. Microsaccades and the velocity-amplitude relationship for saccadic eye movements. Science. 1965; 150: 1459–1460. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Schiller PH, Stryker M. Single-unit recording and stimulation in superior colliculus of the alert rhesus monkey. J Neurophysiol. 1972; 35: 915–924. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Robinson DA . Eye movements evoked by collicular stimulation in the alert monkey. Vis Res. 1972; 12: 1795–1808. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Rolfs M, Kliegl R, Engbert R. Toward a model of microsaccade generation: the case of microsaccadic inhibition. J Vis. 2008; 8:5 1–23. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Martinez-Conde S, Macknik SL, Hubel DH. Microsaccadic eye movements and firing of single cells in the striate cortex of macaque monkeys. Nat Neurosci. 2000; 3: 251–258. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Costela FM, McCamy MB, Macknik SL, Otero-Millan J, Martinez-Conde S. Microsaccades restore the visibility of minute foveal targets. PeerJ. 2013; 1: e119. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Martinez-Conde S, Otero-Millan J, Macknik SL. The impact of microsaccades on vision: towards a unified theory of saccadic function. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2013; 14: 83–96. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
McCamy MB, Otero-Millan J, Macknik SL, et al. Microsaccadic efficacy and contribution to foveal and peripheral vision. J Neurosci. 2012; 32: 9194–9204. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Kiorpes L, Kiper DC, O'Keefe LP, Cavanaugh JR, Movshon JA. Neuronal correlates of amblyopia in the visual cortex of macaque monkeys with experimental strabismus and anisometropia. J Neurosci. 1998; 18: 6411–6424. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Kiorpes L, McKee SP. Neural mechanisms underlying amblyopia. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 1999; 9: 480–486. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Movshon JA, Eggers HM, Gizzi MS, Hendrickson AE, Kiorpes L, Boothe RG. Effects of early unilateral blur on the macaque's visual system. III. Physiological observations. J Neurosci. 1987; 7: 1340–1351. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Crewther DP, Crewther SG. Neural site of strabismic amblyopia in cats: spatial frequency deficit in primary cortical neurons. Exp Brain Res. 1990; 79: 615–622. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Tychsen L, Burkhalter A. Nasotemporal asymmetries in V1: ocular dominance columns of infant, adult, and strabismic macaque monkeys. J Comp Neurol. 1997; 388: 32–46. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Horton JC, Hocking DR, Kiorpes L. Pattern of ocular dominance columns and cytochrome oxidase activity in a macaque monkey with naturally occurring anisometropic amblyopia. Vis Neurosci. 1997; 14: 681–689. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Wiesel TN. Postnatal development of the visual cortex and the influence of environment. Nature. 1982; 299: 583–591. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Hendrickson AE, Movshon JA, Eggers HM, Gizzi MS, Boothe RG, Kiorpes L. Effects of early unilateral blur on the macaque's visual system. II. Anatomical observations. J Neurosci. 1987; 7: 1327–1339. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Kiorpes L, Boothe RG. Naturally occurring strabismus in monkeys (Macaca nemestrina). Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1981; 20: 257–263. [PubMed]
Kiorpes L, Boothe RG, Hendrickson AE, Movshon JA, Eggers HM, Gizzi MS. Effects of early unilateral blur on the macaque's visual system. I. Behavioral observations. J Neurosci. 1987; 7: 1318–1326. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Hasany A, Wong A, Foeller P, Bradley D, Tychsen L. Duration of binocular decorrelation in infancy predicts the severity of nasotemporal pursuit asymmetries in strabismic macaque monkeys. Neuroscience. 2008; 156: 403–411. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Tychsen L, Wong AM, Burkhalter A. Paucity of horizontal connections for binocular vision in V1 of naturally strabismic macaques: Cytochrome oxidase compartment specificity. J Comp Neurol. 2004; 474: 261–275. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Kiorpes L. Visual processing in amblyopia: animal studies. Strabismus. 2006; 14: 3–10. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Maunsell JH, van Essen DC. The connections of the middle temporal visual area (MT) and their relationship to a cortical hierarchy in the macaque monkey. J Neurosci. 1983; 3(12): 2563–2586. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Bedell HE, Yap YL, Flom MC. Fixational drift and nasal-temporal pursuit asymmetries in strabismic amblyopes. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1990; 31: 968–976. [PubMed]
Martinez-Conde S. Fixational eye movements in normal and pathological vision. Prog Brain Res. 2006; 154: 151–176. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Tychsen L, Leibole M, Drake D. Comparison of latent nystagmus and nasotemporal asymmetries of optokinetic nystagmus in adult humans and macaque monkeys who have infantile strabismus. Strabismus. 1996; 4: 171–177. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Tychsen L, Richards M, Wong A, Foeller P, Bradley D, Burkhalter A. The neural mechanism for Latent (fusion maldevelopment) nystagmus. J Neuroophthalmol. 2010; 30: 276–283. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Wong AM, Burkhalter A, Tychsen L. Suppression of metabolic activity caused by infantile strabismus and strabismic amblyopia in striate visual cortex of macaque monkeys. J AAPOS. 2005; 9: 37–47. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Tusa RJ, Mustari MJ, Das VE, Boothe RG. Animal models for visual deprivation-induced strabismus and nystagmus. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2002; 956: 346–360. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Kiorpes L, Daw N. Cortical correlates of amblyopia. Vis Neurosci. 2018; 35: E016. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Otero-Millan J, Macknik SL, Martinez-Conde S. Fixational eye movements and binocular vision. Front Integr Neurosci. 2014; 8: 52. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Alexander RG, Macknik SL, Martinez-Conde S. Microsaccade Characteristics in Neurological and Ophthalmic Disease. Front Neurol. 2018; 9: 144. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Kang SL, Beylergil SB, Otero-Millan J, Shaikh AG, Ghasia FF. Fixational Eye Movement Waveforms in Amblyopia : Characteristics of Fast and Slow Eye Movements. J Eye Mov Res. 2019; 12: 10.
Shaikh AG, Otero-Millan J, Kumar P, Ghasia FF. Abnormal Fixational Eye Movements in Amblyopia. PLoS One. 2016; 11: e0149953. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Shi XF, Xu LM, Li Y, Wang T, Zhao KX, Sabel BA. Fixational saccadic eye movements are altered in anisometropic amblyopia. Restor Neurol Neurosci. 2012; 30: 445–462. [PubMed]
Chung ST, Kumar G, Li RW, Levi DM. Characteristics of fixational eye movements in amblyopia: Limitations on fixation stability and acuity? Vis Res. 2015; 114: 87–99. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Chen D, Otero-Millan J, Kumar P, Shaikh AG, Ghasia FF. Visual Search in Amblyopia: Abnormal Fixational Eye Movements and Suboptimal Sampling Strategies. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2018; 59: 4506–4517. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Abadi RV, Scallan CJ. Waveform characteristics of manifest latent nystagmus. Investig Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2000;3805–3817.
Hertle RW . A next step in naming and classification of eye movement disorders and strabismus. J AAPOS. 2002; 6(4): 201–202. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Bhola R, Keech RV, Kutschke P, Pfeifer W, Scott WE. Recurrence of amblyopia after occlusion therapy. Ophthalmology. 2006; 113: 2097–2100. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Repka MX, Kraker RT, Holmes JM, et al. Atropine vs patching for treatment of moderate amblyopia: follow-up at 15 years of age of a randomized clinical trial. JAMA Ophthalmol. 2014; 132: 799–805. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Birch EE, Jost RM, De La, Cruz A, et al. Binocular amblyopia treatment with contrast-rebalanced movies. J AAPOS. 2019; 23: 160.e1–160.e5. [CrossRef]
Li SL, Jost RM, Morale SE, et al. Binocular iPad treatment of amblyopia for lasting improvement of visual acuity. JAMA Ophthalmol. 2015; 133: 479–480. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Manh VM, Holmes JM, Lazar EL, et al. A Randomized Trial of a Binocular iPad Game Versus Part-Time Patching in Children Aged 13 to 16 Years With Amblyopia. Am J Ophthalmol. 2018; 186: 104–115. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Scaramuzzi M, Murray J, Otero-Millan J, Nucci P, Shaikh AG, Ghasia FF. Fixation instability in amblyopia: Oculomotor disease biomarkers predictive of treatment effectiveness. Prog Brain Res. 2019; 249: 235–248. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Scaramuzzi M, Murray J, Otero-Millan J, Nucci P, Shaikh AG, Ghasia FF. Part time patching treatment outcomes in children with amblyopia with and without fusion maldevelopment nystagmus: An eye movement study. PLoS One. 2020; 15: e0237346. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Scaramuzzi M, Murray J, Nucci P, Shaikh AG, Ghasia FF. Fixational eye movements abnormalities and rate of visual acuity and stereoacuity improvement with part time patching. Sci Rep. 2021; 11: 1217. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Simonsz HJ. The effect of prolonged monocular occlusion on latent nystagmus in the treatment of amblyopia. Doc Ophthalmol. 1989; 72: 375–384. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
von Noorden GK, Avilla C, Sidikaro Y, LaRoche R. Latent nystagmus and strabismic amblyopia. Am J Ophthalmol. 1987; 103: 87–89. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Troncoso XG, Macknik SL, Martinez-Conde S. Microsaccades counteract perceptual filling-in. J Vis. 2008; 8: 15.1–15.9. [CrossRef]
Murray J, Garg K, Ghasia F. Monocular and Binocular Visual Function Deficits in Amblyopic Patients with and without Fusion Maldevelopment Nystagmus. Eye Brain. 2021; 13: 99–109. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Gonzalez EG, Wong AM, Niechwiej-Szwedo E, Tarita-Nistor L, Steinbach MJ. Eye position stability in amblyopia and in normal binocular vision. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2012; 53: 5386–5394. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
McCamy MB, Otero-Millan J, Leigh RJ, et al . Simultaneous recordings of human microsaccades and drifts with a contemporary video eye tracker and the search coil technique. PLoS One. 2015; 10(6): e0128428. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Ghasia FF, Otero-Millan J, Shaikh AG. Abnormal fixational eye movements in strabismus. Br J Ophthalmol. 2018; 102: 253–259. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Shaikh AG, Ghasia FF. Fixational saccades are more disconjugate in adults than in children. PLoS One. 2017; 12: e0175295. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Engbert R, Kliegl R. Microsaccades uncover the orientation of covert attention. Vis Res. 2003; 43: 1035–1045. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Engbert R, Kliegl R. Microsaccades keep the eyes’ balance during fixation. Psychol Sci. 2004; 15: 431–436. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Laubrock J, Engbert R, Kliegl R. Microsaccade dynamics during covert attention. Vis Res. 2005; 45: 721–730. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Steinman RM, Cushman WB, Martins AJ. The precision of gaze. A review. Hum Neurobiol. 1982; 1: 97–109. [PubMed]
Upadhyaya S, Pullela M, Ramachandran S, Adade S, Joshi AC, Das VE. Fixational Saccades and Their Relation to Fixation Instability in Strabismic Monkeys. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2017; 58: 5743–5753. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Dell'Osso LF, Schmidt D, Daroff RB. Latent, manifest latent, and congenital nystagmus. Arch Ophthalmol. 1979; 97: 1877–1885. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Tychsen L, Richards M, Wong A, Foeller P, Bradley D, Burkhalter A. The neural mechanism for Latent (fusion maldevelopment) nystagmus. J Neuroophthalmol. 2010; 30(3): 276–283. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Bellmann C, Feely M, Crossland MD, Kabanarou SA, Rubin GS. Fixation stability using central and pericentral fixation targets in patients with age-related macular degeneration. Ophthalmology. 2004; 111: 2265–2270. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Grenga PL, Fragiotta S, Meduri A, Lupo S, Marenco M, Vingolo EM. Fixation stability measurements in patients with neovascular age-related macular degeneration treated with ranibizumab. Can J Ophthalmol. 2013; 48: 394–399. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Schneider RM, Thurtell MJ, Eisele S, Lincoff N, Bala E, Leigh RJ. Neurological basis for eye movements of the blind. PLoS One. 2013; 8: e56556. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Seiple W, Rosen RB, Garcia PM. Abnormal fixation in individuals with age-related macular degeneration when viewing an image of a face. Optom Vis Sci. 2013; 90: 45–56. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Birch ES, Vidhya , Weakley D. Fixation instability in anisometropic children with reduced stereopsis. J AAPOS. 2013; 17: 287–290. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Murray J, Garg K, Ghasia F. Monocular and Binocular Visual Function Deficits in Amblyopic Patients with and without Fusion Maldevelopment Nystagmus. Eye Brain. 2021; 13: 99–109. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Schor CM. The relationship between fusional vergence eye movements and fixation disparity. Vis Res. 1979; 19: 1359–1367. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Baker DH, Meese TS, Hess RF. Contrast masking in strabismic amblyopia: attenuation, noise, interocular suppression and binocular summation. Vis Res. 2008; 48: 1625–1640. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Mansouri B, Thompson B, Hess RF. Measurement of suprathreshold binocular interactions in amblyopia. Vis Res. 2008; 48: 2775–2784. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Raveendran RN, Bobier WR, Thompson B. Binocular vision and fixational eye movements. J Vis. 2019; 19: 9. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Raveendran RN, Babu RJ, Hess RF, Bobier WR. Transient improvements in fixational stability in strabismic amblyopes following bifoveal fixation and reduced interocular suppression. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt. 2014; 34: 214–225. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Wong AM, Lueder GT, Burkhalter A, Tychsen L. Anomalous retinal correspondence: neuroanatomic mechanism in strabismic monkeys and clinical findings in strabismic children. J AAPOS. 2000; 4: 168–174. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Zubcov AA, Stärk N, Weber A, Wizov SS, Reinecke RD. Improvement of visual acuity after surgery for nystagmus. Ophthalmology. 1993; 100: 1488–1497. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Dell'Osso LF, Flynn JT. Congenital nystagmus surgery. A quantitative evaluation of the effects. Arch Ophthalmol. 1979; 97: 462–469. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Newsham D, O'Connor AR. Assessment of the Density of Suppression to Identify Risk of Intractable Diplopia in the United Kingdom. Strabismus. 2016; 24: 45–50. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1.
 
Examples of fixation eye movements during a 3 second epoch under conditions of (A) both eye viewing (BEV), (B) fellow eye viewing (FEV), and (C) amblyopic eye viewing (AEV) from a subject without nystagmus (top row), subject with fusion maldevelopment nystagmus (FMN) (middle row), and subject with nystagmus without FMN (bottom row). The x-axis represents time and the y-axis represents horizontal (solid line, black: fellow eye, and grey: amblyopic eye) and vertical (dotted line, black: fellow eye, and grey: amblyopic eye) positions. The black arrow represents the fast fixation eye movements (FEMs), whereas the grey arrows represent slow FEMs. Rightward and upward movements correspond to positive vertical axis.
Figure 1.
 
Examples of fixation eye movements during a 3 second epoch under conditions of (A) both eye viewing (BEV), (B) fellow eye viewing (FEV), and (C) amblyopic eye viewing (AEV) from a subject without nystagmus (top row), subject with fusion maldevelopment nystagmus (FMN) (middle row), and subject with nystagmus without FMN (bottom row). The x-axis represents time and the y-axis represents horizontal (solid line, black: fellow eye, and grey: amblyopic eye) and vertical (dotted line, black: fellow eye, and grey: amblyopic eye) positions. The black arrow represents the fast fixation eye movements (FEMs), whereas the grey arrows represent slow FEMs. Rightward and upward movements correspond to positive vertical axis.
Figure 2.
 
Cumulative sum histogram of composite amplitude (degrees) of fast fixation eye movements of fellow eye obtained during fellow eye viewing and both eye viewing in groups 0 vs. 1 (A) and groups 0, 1, and 2 (B) and amblyopic eye obtained during amblyopic eye viewing and both eye viewing in groups 0 vs. 1 (C) and groups 0, 1, and 2 (D).
Figure 2.
 
Cumulative sum histogram of composite amplitude (degrees) of fast fixation eye movements of fellow eye obtained during fellow eye viewing and both eye viewing in groups 0 vs. 1 (A) and groups 0, 1, and 2 (B) and amblyopic eye obtained during amblyopic eye viewing and both eye viewing in groups 0 vs. 1 (C) and groups 0, 1, and 2 (D).
Figure 3.
 
Cumulative sum histogram of composite eye velocity (degrees/sec) of slow fixation eye movements of fellow eye obtained during fellow eye viewing and both eye viewing in groups 0 vs. 1 (A) and groups 0, 1, and 2 (B) and amblyopic eye obtained during amblyopic eye viewing and both eye viewing in groups 0 vs. 1 (C) and groups 0, 1, and 2 (D).
Figure 3.
 
Cumulative sum histogram of composite eye velocity (degrees/sec) of slow fixation eye movements of fellow eye obtained during fellow eye viewing and both eye viewing in groups 0 vs. 1 (A) and groups 0, 1, and 2 (B) and amblyopic eye obtained during amblyopic eye viewing and both eye viewing in groups 0 vs. 1 (C) and groups 0, 1, and 2 (D).
Figure 4.
 
Top: Horizontal and vertical eye positions of fellow eye (black) and amblyopic eye (grey) during both eye viewing (BEV), fellow eye viewing (FEV), and amblyopic eye viewing (AEV). Rightward and upward movements correspond to positive vertical axis. Notice the right hypotropia and exotropia during BEV and FEV condition and left exotropia and left hypertropia during AEV. Bottom: Histogram showing range of composite eye position difference during BEV (grey), FEV (black), and AEV (white).
Figure 4.
 
Top: Horizontal and vertical eye positions of fellow eye (black) and amblyopic eye (grey) during both eye viewing (BEV), fellow eye viewing (FEV), and amblyopic eye viewing (AEV). Rightward and upward movements correspond to positive vertical axis. Notice the right hypotropia and exotropia during BEV and FEV condition and left exotropia and left hypertropia during AEV. Bottom: Histogram showing range of composite eye position difference during BEV (grey), FEV (black), and AEV (white).
Figure 5.
 
Cumulative sum histogram of composite amplitude (degrees) of fast fixation eye movements of fellow eye and amblyopic eye obtained during dichoptic viewing across fellow eye contrasts of 100% (black), 50% (darkest grey), 25% (lighter grey), and 10% (lightest grey) in groups 0 vs. 1 (A, C) and groups 0, 1, and 2 (B, D).
Figure 5.
 
Cumulative sum histogram of composite amplitude (degrees) of fast fixation eye movements of fellow eye and amblyopic eye obtained during dichoptic viewing across fellow eye contrasts of 100% (black), 50% (darkest grey), 25% (lighter grey), and 10% (lightest grey) in groups 0 vs. 1 (A, C) and groups 0, 1, and 2 (B, D).
Figure 6.
 
Cumulative sum histogram of composite velocity (degrees/sec) of slow fixation eye movements of fellow eye and amblyopic eye obtained during dichoptic viewing across fellow eye contrasts of 100% (black), 50% (darkest grey), 25% (lighter grey), and 10% (lightest grey) in groups 0 vs. 1 (A, C) and groups 0, 1, and 2 (B, D).
Figure 6.
 
Cumulative sum histogram of composite velocity (degrees/sec) of slow fixation eye movements of fellow eye and amblyopic eye obtained during dichoptic viewing across fellow eye contrasts of 100% (black), 50% (darkest grey), 25% (lighter grey), and 10% (lightest grey) in groups 0 vs. 1 (A, C) and groups 0, 1, and 2 (B, D).
Figure 7.
 
Horizontal and vertical eye position traces (vertical axis) and time (horizontal axis) in 2 subjects obtained during dichoptic viewing at fellow eye contrasts of (A) 100%, (B) 50%, (C) 25%, and (D) 10% contrast. To the left is a subject with mixed/strabismic amblyopia without fusion maldevelopment nystagmus (FMN) that demonstrates a fixation switch indicated by the black arrow when the fellow eye is fixing at FE contrast of 25% and the grey arrow that represents the amblyopic eye picks up with fixation. The horizontal arrows on the far left represent the primary position. To the right is a subject with mixed/strabismic amblyopia with fusion maldevelopment nystagmus (FMN). Notice that through all the trials A to D the fellow eye fixates in primary position with increase in eye misalignment at lower fellow eye contrasts indicated by the vertical black arrows on the far right.
Figure 7.
 
Horizontal and vertical eye position traces (vertical axis) and time (horizontal axis) in 2 subjects obtained during dichoptic viewing at fellow eye contrasts of (A) 100%, (B) 50%, (C) 25%, and (D) 10% contrast. To the left is a subject with mixed/strabismic amblyopia without fusion maldevelopment nystagmus (FMN) that demonstrates a fixation switch indicated by the black arrow when the fellow eye is fixing at FE contrast of 25% and the grey arrow that represents the amblyopic eye picks up with fixation. The horizontal arrows on the far left represent the primary position. To the right is a subject with mixed/strabismic amblyopia with fusion maldevelopment nystagmus (FMN). Notice that through all the trials A to D the fellow eye fixates in primary position with increase in eye misalignment at lower fellow eye contrasts indicated by the vertical black arrows on the far right.
Figure 8.
 
Right: Horizontal and vertical eye positions of fellow eye (black) and amblyopic eye (grey) obtained from the same subject shown in Figure 4 during dichoptic viewing across fellow eye contrasts of 100% (A), 50% (B), 25% (C), and 10% (D). Rightward and upward movements correspond to positive vertical axis. Notice the right hypotropia and exotropia during all dichoptic viewing conditions. Left: Histogram showing range of composite eye position difference during dichoptic viewing across the fellow eye contrasts of 100% (A), 50% (B), 25% (C), and 10% (D).
Figure 8.
 
Right: Horizontal and vertical eye positions of fellow eye (black) and amblyopic eye (grey) obtained from the same subject shown in Figure 4 during dichoptic viewing across fellow eye contrasts of 100% (A), 50% (B), 25% (C), and 10% (D). Rightward and upward movements correspond to positive vertical axis. Notice the right hypotropia and exotropia during all dichoptic viewing conditions. Left: Histogram showing range of composite eye position difference during dichoptic viewing across the fellow eye contrasts of 100% (A), 50% (B), 25% (C), and 10% (D).
Table 1.
 
Clinical and Demographic Characteristics of Participants
Table 1.
 
Clinical and Demographic Characteristics of Participants
Table 2.
 
Frequency of Fast FEMs During Monocular and Binocular Viewing Across the Four Groups
Table 2.
 
Frequency of Fast FEMs During Monocular and Binocular Viewing Across the Four Groups
Table 3.
 
Percentile of Composite Amplitude and Velocity of Fast and Slow Fixation Eye Movements of Fellow Eye and Amblyopic Eye During Monocular and Binocular Viewing Across the Four Groups
Table 3.
 
Percentile of Composite Amplitude and Velocity of Fast and Slow Fixation Eye Movements of Fellow Eye and Amblyopic Eye During Monocular and Binocular Viewing Across the Four Groups
Table 4.
 
Eye Alignment (Degrees) During Monocular and Binocular Viewing Across the Four Groups
Table 4.
 
Eye Alignment (Degrees) During Monocular and Binocular Viewing Across the Four Groups
Table 5.
 
Frequency of Fast Fixation Eye Movements (Hertz) During Dichoptic Viewing Across the Four Groups
Table 5.
 
Frequency of Fast Fixation Eye Movements (Hertz) During Dichoptic Viewing Across the Four Groups
Table 6.
 
Percentile Composite Amplitude (Degrees) of Fast Fixation Eye Movements of Fellow Eye and Amblyopic Eye during Dichoptic Viewing Across the Four Groups
Table 6.
 
Percentile Composite Amplitude (Degrees) of Fast Fixation Eye Movements of Fellow Eye and Amblyopic Eye during Dichoptic Viewing Across the Four Groups
Table 7.
 
Composite Velocity of Slow Fixation Eye Movements of Fellow Eye and Amblyopic Eye During Dichoptic Viewing Across the Four Groups
Table 7.
 
Composite Velocity of Slow Fixation Eye Movements of Fellow Eye and Amblyopic Eye During Dichoptic Viewing Across the Four Groups
Table 8.
 
Bivariate Contour Ellipse Area of FE and AE During Dichoptic Viewing in Controls and Patients With Anisometropic Amblyopia
Table 8.
 
Bivariate Contour Ellipse Area of FE and AE During Dichoptic Viewing in Controls and Patients With Anisometropic Amblyopia
Table 9.
 
Vergence Bivariate Contour Ellipse Area During Dichoptic Viewing Across the Four Groups
Table 9.
 
Vergence Bivariate Contour Ellipse Area During Dichoptic Viewing Across the Four Groups
Table 10.
 
Eye Alignment (Degrees) During Dichoptic Viewing Across The Four Groups
Table 10.
 
Eye Alignment (Degrees) During Dichoptic Viewing Across The Four Groups
×
×

This PDF is available to Subscribers Only

Sign in or purchase a subscription to access this content. ×

You must be signed into an individual account to use this feature.

×