Abstract
Purpose:
The purpose of this study was to locate the visual axis and evaluate its correlation with the Stiles–Crawford effect (SCE) peak.
Methods:
Ten young, healthy individuals (20 eyes) were enrolled. An optical system was developed to locate the visual axis and measure SCE. To locate the visual axis, 2 small laser spots at 450 nm and 680 nm were co-aligned and delivered to the retina. The participants were asked to move a translatable pinhole until these spots were perceived to overlap each other. The same system assessed SCE at 680 nm using a bipartite, 2-channel (reference and test) Maxwellian-view optical system. The peak positions were estimated using a two-dimensional Gaussian fitting function and correlated with the visual axis positions.
Results:
Both the visual axis (x = 0.24 ± 0.35 mm, y = −0.16 ± 0.34 mm) and the SCE peak (x = 0.27 ± 0.35 mm, y = −0.15 ± 0.31 mm) showed intersubject variability among the cohort. The SCE peak positions were highly correlated in both the horizontal and vertical meridians to the visual axes (R2 = 0.98 and 0.96 for the x and y coordinates, respectively). Nine of the 10 participants demonstrated mirror symmetry for the coordinates of the visual axis and the SCE peak between the eyes (R2 = 0.71 for the visual axis and 0.76 for the SCE peak).
Conclusions:
The visual axis and SCE peak locations varied among the participants; however, they were highly correlated with each other for each individual. These findings suggest a potential mechanism underlying the foveal cone photoreceptor alignment.
In cataract/corneal refractive surgery, accurate centration of optical correction is imperative to obtain optimal visual outcomes. Nevertheless, locating the correction center has been a highly controversial topic.
1 One of the commonly used references for centration is the entrance pupil center (EPC), wherein the optical axis of the instrument is along the line of sight (LOS). Alternatively, the coaxially sighted corneal light reflex (CSCLR), which is a practicable centration reference proximate to the corneal intercept of the visual axis, has also been introduced.
2 The CSCLR is the first Purkinje image observed by the examiner along the identical path with a light source, such that it falls on the line from the fixation point to the center of the anterior corneal curvature.
3 Clinically, the CSCLR-centered correction was reported to be superior to the EPC-centered correction regarding lower induction of total higher-order aberrations.
4–6 Chang et al.
7 showed that centration locations deviated from the EPC to the CSLCR about 80% to 100% were reliable for myopic laser in situ keratomileuses.
Based on the results from recent clinical studies, the visual axis may be a logical candidate for the preferred reference axis, especially for centration, because the visual axis serves as the actual path that a person views.
2,8 However, identifying the visual axis is difficult in a clinical setting because nodal points, which are the cardinal points in optics, have no corresponding anatomic landmarks. Ivanoff
9 first introduced the concept of the foveal achromatic axis, and Thibos et al.
10 interpreted Ivanoff’s concept as a functional definition of the visual axis. They proposed that a pinhole should be situated on the pupillary intersection of the visual axis to minimize the effect of transverse chromatic aberration (TCA), and the pinhole is on the achromatizing pupil position as the achromatic axis becomes the visual axis. However, locating the visual axis has been challenging, particularly in a clinical setting.
The human eye has a unique directional sensitivity (i.e. the Stiles–Crawford effect [SCE]).
11 It is a phenomenon that a light beam transmitted through the peripheral region of the pupil is not perceived to be as bright as that through the EPC.
11 Generally, the cone photoreceptors serve as optical fibers that capture light more effectively when illuminated along their axes and tend to point toward the same position on the pupil plane.
12–15 However, a waveguide model of the photoreceptors cannot explain the possibility of inter-receptor crosstalk (i.e. light leakage and re-capture by adjacent photoreceptors). Vohnsen et al.
16 suggested that the volumetric intersection model, based on light absorption by visual pigments in the elongated, layered outer segment of the cone photoreceptor, could explain this gap in understanding.
17,18 However, this model cannot support the phototropism.
19–24 The pupil coordinates of the peak sensitivity may demonstrate the pupillary location at which the cones aim. In a study population, cones showed a tendency to be directed slightly nasal to the EPC,
25 but large intersubject variability and SCE peak sensitivity pupil positions farther than 1 mm from the EPC have also been reported.
19 It is still controversial which mechanism stimulates the cones to aim at a specific pupil position, and the mechanism is probably related to the visual axis in that both the visual axis and SCE peak remarkably deviate from the EPC. This study aimed to evaluate whether the visual axis is correlated with the SCE peak position on the pupil plane compared to the CSCLR and EPC. We developed a custom-built optical system to measure the positions of the visual axis, SCE peak, CSCLR, and EPC.
An optical system was designed to investigate the achromatizing pupil (visual axis) position and SCE peak position with a common optical path.
Figure 1 shows a configuration of the optical system. In the common path, the participant's pupil was optically conjugated with a pinhole (PH1, 600 µm diameter) using a 4-f system relayed with lenses L1 and L2 (both 100 mm focal length, achromatic doublets). Real-time sequential images of the participant's pupil superimposed by the pinhole were recorded using a pupil camera (DCC1545M; Thorlabs). A reticle was located at the pupil plane in front of the M1 and was used to ensure the participant's pupil centration. The pupil camera captured images of the participant's pupil, pinhole position, and reticle at the same time. For pupil alignment, the eye was illuminated with a 4 cm diameter circular array of infrared light-emitting diodes (LEDs; central λ = 870 nm) located 6 cm from the participant's pupil plane. Chin and forehead rests were used to stabilize the participant's head.
In the assessment of the visual axis, the visual stimuli consisted of two monochromatic (red and blue) sources to generate the chromatic visual test: a red super-luminescent diode (SLD; λ = 680 ± 3.5 nm) with a collimating lens (30 mm focal length, achromatic doublet) and a blue laser diode (λ = 450 nm) with the same collimating lens. Two synchronized rotating choppers were used to create two anti-phase flickering stimuli at 2 hertz (Hz). Neutral density filters were applied to optimize the brightness of the two stimuli. A beam splitter (BS4) and a mirror (M2) were used to direct the two chromatic sources coaxially to the optical path toward the eye. The pinhole (600 µm diameter) on the pupil plane was motorized and translatable in the horizontal and vertical directions so participants could easily adjust the pinhole position using two controllers.
In assessing the SCE, the first light source was a digital micromirror device (DMD; DLP Discovery 4100 0.7XGA; Texas Instruments) relayed by lens L4 and a flipping mirror (M3). A bandpass filter with a 10-nm bandwidth centered at a wavelength of 680 nm was applied in front of L4. The DMD provided the participant with a 0.9 degree circular central test stimulus. The other source, the tungsten lamp, was relayed by lens L3, a fixed pinhole (600 µm diameter), and a pellicle beam splitter (BS3). This source provided the participant with a 0.9 degree (inner)/2.2 degree (outer) ring-shaped reference stimulus, defined by the annular aperture. Another identical bandpass filter was applied in front of L3.
We developed an optical system to subjectively determine the position of the visual axis and SCE peak on the pupil plane in healthy participants. Our results showed that the pupillary locations of the visual axis were remarkably well correlated with the SCE peak compared to the CSCLR and EPC. We also found a significant mirror symmetry of the visual axes and SCE peak positions. These findings suggest that the functional orientation of cone photoreceptors is associated with the visual axis along which the transverse chromatic aberration is zero.
Because the fovea lies off the optical axis of the eye and the lens and cornea are slightly tilted and decentered to each other, the human eye is not a rotationally symmetric or centered optical system.
28 These multiple misaligned refractive surfaces cause challenges in the optical characterization of the eye and clinical decisions for centration. Recently, researchers have been using the subject-fixated CSCLR as the clinical reference for centration.
3 It is commonly assumed that the CSCLR is close to the intersection of the visual axis on the pupil plane.
3 However, our study found a significant discrepancy between the CSCLR and the visual axis. Manzanera et al.
29 already reported that 27% of healthy eyes showed a statistically significant distance between the CSCLR and visual axis, and in 5% of cases, the distance was >0.4 mm. Moreover, their simulation study demonstrated that the linear addition of the effects of the crystalline lens decentration, tilt, and thickness could account for this discrepancy. The distance between the CSCLR and the visual axis may be explained by the difference between the center of the anterior corneal curvature (related to the CSCLR) and the anterior nodal point (related to the visual axis).
In addition, another discrepancy exists among the currently available devices for the measurement of angle κ. Undoubtedly, angle κ plays a critical role in laser corneal refractive surgery
1,30 and the implantation of multifocal intraocular lenses.
31,32 The definition of angle κ in the literature is conflicting because the same angle (angle κ) has been used to specify two different angles.
3 Although angle κ has been commonly accepted, angle λ is distinctly defined as the angle between the pupillary axis and LOS.
3,31 Angle κ has been used interchangeably with angle λ basis of the assumption that both angles are similar when the fixation point is far away from the pupil plane.
3 However, clinicians should be careful because the visual axis differs from the LOS and the CSCLR. For example, Pentacam reported the distance from the CSCLR to the EPC only,
33 and not from the visual axis.
In our study, the coordinates of the visual axis on the pupil plane clearly showed intersubject variability. This intersubject variability was consistent with the findings of previous studies.
34,35 Theoretical eye models estimated that the vertical component might be approximately half of the horizontal component in the visual axis–EPC difference.
36 Those estimations were based on the foveal displacement relative to the optical axis. Variability in the visual axis locations from the EPC cannot be explained only by angle α, the angle between the visual axis and optical axis, but may also be interpreted by misalignment of the cornea, crystalline lens, and iris. Individual differences in the refractive indices, especially in crystalline lenses with a gradient index, may also play a role in the diversity of the visual axis position.
The coordinates of the SCE peak also varied widely among participants. Previous studies using psychophysical
11,25,37 and reflectometric measurements
19,37–40 demonstrated large intersubject variability in cone orientation. Phototropism has been proposed as a potential mechanism underlying cone orientation.
19–24 Kono et al.
24 found minor nasal shifts of the SCE peak after 8 days of dark patching, whereas a substantial temporal re-adjustment after 3 day’s recovery, supporting the phototropic mechanism in normal eyes. Applegate and Bonds
22 reported the SCE peak shift toward an artificial pupil after wearing a contact lens with the 2-mm centered artificial pupil in a nasally displaced pupil after trauma. Smallman et al.
23 presented the SCE peak shift toward the pupil center after congenital cataract extraction. However, what drives the cone photoreceptors to point toward the given pupil position remains unclear. Marcos and Burns
19 proposed that optical aberrations and cone directionality may interact, so the pupil’s best optical area corresponds to the area of maximum transmittance. They also reported that cone photoreceptors did not point toward optically degraded pupil positions; maximal cone directionality coincides with the pupillary region of the best optical quality for some eyes only.
19 Our results implied that the SCE peak might lie adjacent to the visual axis. The potential visual advantage of this finding could be the maximization of polychromatic visual quality by minimizing TCA. The cone photoreceptors cannot adjust their orientation quickly as to the dynamics of vision (e.g. eye movement), and the optimal direction of alignment may be different in three types of cones.
41 The best strategy may be achromatizing the pupil position corresponding to the visual axis. However, this hypothesis must be tested.
The mirror symmetry between the two eyes was noticeable on the visual axis and the SCE peak position. First, the mirror symmetry of the visual axis position may indicate anatomical symmetry in terms of the shape and layout of each ocular optical element, which may also explain the mirror symmetry of the SCE tilt angle. Furthermore, the mirror symmetry in the SCE peak may support the potential systematic mechanism of the cone orientation. Assuming the active phototropic capability of cone photoreceptors, they may point toward the second nodal point of the eye, which is close to the plane of the pupil,
42,43 and also toward one part of the visual axis. Based on the definition of the nodal point, this may produce angular linearity for rays transmitted through the pupil and toward the curved retina
42 and may optimize the capture efficiency of the cone photoreceptors for light with minimal TCA. Interestingly, the shape factor ρ did not show direct symmetry, which conflicted with a previous finding.
19 This implies that the factors regulating the distribution widths of the cone directionality may be between the two eyes due to differences in cone spacing or aperture size distribution and the structural features of the cones.
37,44
In our study, 9 participants had 17 myopic eyes and showed a tendency for nasal bias in cone photoreceptor alignment. This finding is somewhat consistent with previous studies,
25,45–48 which reported nasal bias in myopic individuals. The anatomic sections of myopic eyes indicate a slope in the sclera (i.e. the temporal retina is anterior to the nasal retina). Westheimer
48 suggested that the fovea has a sloping shape factor that makes the foveal cone photoreceptors point toward the nasal pupillary area. Furthermore, the tractional force on the retina has been suggested to affect cone photoreceptor alignment.
20 Choi et al.
45 hypothesized a potential scenario associated with an extension of the vitreous chamber depth. Specifically, they speculated that when the eye elongates axially in myopic eyes, the anterior vitreomacular traction might apply a pulling force to the nasal retina between the optic nerve head and fovea. This force originating near the optic disc’s temporal rim may drag the cone photoreceptors nasally.
On the other hand, the visual axis also demonstrated a nasally biased position. A recent study showed that crystalline lenses were tilted outward, and the tilt magnitude tended to be lower in myopic eyes than in non-myopic eyes.
49 If the crystalline lenses are tilted inward in myopic eyes, the first nodal point is shifted nasally so that it may affect the visual axis position on the pupil plane. Further studies are required to test these hypotheses.
A major limitation of this study was the subjective nature of psychophysical measurements, where the retina operated as a detector after a single pass of light through the ocular media. The measurement errors in terms of standard deviations in our repeated measurements might have occurred because of these subjective characteristics. Although SCE has been commonly studied by psychophysical methods, it demands a high level of concentration among the participants and involves lengthy sessions, making it difficult to use in a clinical setting.
25 Instead, objective methods that use the reflectometric technique
38,39 can be applied, and are faster and more patient-friendly than the psychophysical measurements. We could not use the objective reflectometric method for the SCE measurement because of the absence of a current method to measure the visual axis objectively to avoid a mismatch in measurements between the visual axis and SCE. Recent studies have proposed a technique to measure human ocular TCA by adopting an adaptive optics scanning laser ophthalmoscope with a wide object instead of a point source.
50,51 It may be plausible to measure the visual axis objectively using this approach by scanning for TCA on the pupil plane.
The SCE was measured at a single wavelength (680 nm); therefore, the potential influence of wavelength on the SCE could not be ruled out. The shape factor ρ tends to increase with longer wavelengths,
11,52–55 and the SCE peak position shows relatively small but opposite horizontal shifts between 570 nm and 670 nm in 2 participants.
55 Further studies are warranted to determine the cause of these wavelength-dependent changes in the SCE.
Our experimental protocol could not elucidate any visual benefit of choosing the visual axis as a centration reference compared to the CSCLR or EPC. A direct comparative study of the visual performance of each reference would help evaluate the benefit.
56 Moreover, to clarify the potential interaction between retinal image quality and cone alignment, highly aberrated eyes, such as keratoconus, would be a promising model to test this hypothesis. Additionally, whether there is a closed-loop mechanism (feedback between polychromatic retinal image quality and cone alignment) should be clarified to rationalize the cone directionality toward the visual axis. Furthermore, it is important to study the correlation between the visual axis and cone directionality in abnormal conditions such as eccentric fixation or age-related macular degeneration with a preferred retinal locus.
In summary, a significant positional correlation between the visual axis and cone directionality was observed in the present study. The mirror symmetry and refractive error correlation of the visual axis and cone directionality may also support this positional interconnection, indicating the potential role of the visual axis as an optical cue mediating cone orientation. These findings may provide a clearer understanding of the clinical implications of the visual axis and further research topics, including anatomic and/or optical factors for cone photoreceptor alignment.
Part of this research was conducted at the University of Rochester Flaum Eye Institute. The authors thank Chi Huang for assistance in building the optical chopper, and Cuong Pham for assistance with the statistical analyses.
Supported by a research grant from the Research to Prevent Blindness and United States National Eye Institute (grant number: R01 EY014999).
The authors alone are responsible for the content and writing of this manuscript.
Disclosure: S.P. Bang, None; J. Lyu, None; C.J. Ng, None; G. Yoon, None