November 2022
Volume 63, Issue 12
Open Access
Eye Movements, Strabismus, Amblyopia and Neuro-ophthalmology  |   November 2022
Temporal Eye–Hand Coordination During Visually Guided Reaching in 7- to 12-Year-Old Children With Strabismus
Author Affiliations & Notes
  • Krista R. Kelly
    Retina Foundation of the Southwest, Dallas, TX, United States
    Department of Ophthalmology, UT Southwestern Medical Center, Dallas, TX, United States
  • Dorsa Mir Norouzi
    Retina Foundation of the Southwest, Dallas, TX, United States
  • Mina Nouredanesh
    School of Rehabilitation Science, McMaster University, Hamilton, Ontario, Canada
  • Reed M. Jost
    Retina Foundation of the Southwest, Dallas, TX, United States
  • Christina S. Cheng-Patel
    Retina Foundation of the Southwest, Dallas, TX, United States
  • Cynthia L. Beauchamp
    ABC Eyes Pediatric Ophthalmology, PA, Dallas, TX, United States
  • Lori M. Dao
    ABC Eyes Pediatric Ophthalmology, PA, Dallas, TX, United States
  • Becky A. Luu
    Pediatric Ophthalmology & Adult Strabismus, PA, Plano, TX, United States
  • David R. Stager, Jr
    Pediatric Ophthalmology & Adult Strabismus, PA, Plano, TX, United States
  • James Y. Tung
    Department of Mechanical and Mechatronics Engineering, University of Waterloo, Waterloo, Ontario, Canada
  • Ewa Niechwiej-Szwedo
    Department of Kinesiology, University of Waterloo, Waterloo, Ontario, Canada
  • Correspondence: Krista R. Kelly, Vision and Neurodevelopment Laboratory, Retina Foundation of the Southwest, 9600 N Central Expressway, Suite 200, Dallas, TX 75231, USA; kkelly@rfsw.org
Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science November 2022, Vol.63, 10. doi:https://doi.org/10.1167/iovs.63.12.10
  • Views
  • PDF
  • Share
  • Tools
    • Alerts
      ×
      This feature is available to authenticated users only.
      Sign In or Create an Account ×
    • Get Citation

      Krista R. Kelly, Dorsa Mir Norouzi, Mina Nouredanesh, Reed M. Jost, Christina S. Cheng-Patel, Cynthia L. Beauchamp, Lori M. Dao, Becky A. Luu, David R. Stager, James Y. Tung, Ewa Niechwiej-Szwedo; Temporal Eye–Hand Coordination During Visually Guided Reaching in 7- to 12-Year-Old Children With Strabismus. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 2022;63(12):10. https://doi.org/10.1167/iovs.63.12.10.

      Download citation file:


      © ARVO (1962-2015); The Authors (2016-present)

      ×
  • Supplements
Abstract

Purpose: We recently found slow visually guided reaching in strabismic children, especially in the final approach. Here, we expand on those data by reporting saccade kinematics and temporal eye–hand coordination during visually guided reaching in children treated for strabismus compared with controls.

Methods: Thirty children diagnosed with esotropia, a form of strabismus, 7 to 12 years of age and 32 age-similar control children were enrolled. Eye movements and index finger movements were recorded. While viewing binocularly, children reached out and touched a small dot that appeared randomly in one of four locations along the horizontal meridian (±5° or ±10°). Saccade kinematic measures (latency, accuracy and precision, peak velocity, and frequency of corrective and reach-related saccades) and temporal eye–hand coordination measures (saccade-to-reach planning interval, saccade-to-reach peak velocity interval) were compared. Factors associated with impaired performance were also evaluated.

Results: During visually guided reaching, strabismic children had longer primary saccade latency (strabismic, 195 ± 29 ms vs. control; 175 ± 23 ms; P = 0.004), a 25% decrease in primary saccade precision (0.15 ± 0.06 vs. 0.12 ± 0.03; P = 0.007), a 45% decrease in the final saccade precision (0.16 ± 0.06 vs. 0.11 ± 0.03; P < 0.001), and more reach-related saccades (16 ± 13% of trials vs. 8 ± 6% of trials; P = 0.001) compared with a control group. No measurable stereoacuity was related to poor saccade kinematics.

Conclusions: Strabismus impacts saccade kinematics during visually guided reaching in children, with poor binocularity playing a role in performance. Coupled with previous data showing slow reaching in the final approach, the current saccade data suggest that children treated for strabismus have not yet adapted or formed an efficient compensatory strategy during visually guided reaching.

Strabismus affects 2% to 4% of children and results in discordant binocular experience when the visual and ocular motor systems are still developing.1,2 Even after surgical or optical intervention to align the eyes, esotropic strabismus (nasalward eye turn) is associated with visual deficits, including amblyopia, binocular dysfunction, and ocular motor deficits that persist into adulthood.38 Ocular motor deficits typical of strabismus include fixation instability,6,9,10 decreased vergence,7,11 and abnormal saccade initiation and execution.8,12,13 Most ocular motor studies have focused on adults with strabismus and little is known about ocular motor development in children with treated strabismus. Sensory and ocular motor impairments in strabismus may interfere with other developing systems, such as the motor system, and with the communication between the eyes and the hands, namely, visuomotor integration. Yet, no studies have examined ocular motor impairments in strabismic children in relation to eye–hand coordination. 
Eye–hand coordination in three dimensional space is essential for efficient object manipulation, requiring depth perception cues to localize the object, plan the movements, and guide the arm toward the object.14,15 Normal binocular vision during childhood provides important sensory input for optimal development of eye–hand coordination.1618 The use of binocular cues is immature during childhood,19,20 which could thus be disrupted by discordant binocular experience early in life from strabismus. Children with strabismus and amblyopia have impaired fine motor skills that rely on eye–hand coordination, such as placing coins into a box, threading beads, and transferring answers to a multiple choice form.2124 Poor performance was associated with binocular dysfunction (decreased/no measurable stereoacuity, suppression), regardless of whether amblyopia was present, indicating normal stereoacuity and fusion are essential to optimal task performance.21,23,25,26 
We previously reported that children with treated strabismus are slower at reach execution, especially in the final approach, with greater end point error than their peers with normal vision when asked to touch a dot on a screen.27 These findings suggest an inefficient use of visual feedback during online control of reaching in the final approach. During eye–hand coordination tasks, the eyes move first to fixate the target, providing high-resolution information about its physical properties and location, which can facilitate planning and execution of the reach.2830 Given the sensory and ocular motor dysfunction typical of strabismus, information gathered after the saccade regarding a target's physical properties and location may be suboptimal and could impact the control of the reach in the final approach. 
Here, we use a protocol previously established in adults12,31,32 to examine visually guided reaching in children 7 to 12 years of age with a history of strabismus. We previously published reach kinematic data from this study as described elsewhere in this article.27 As a next step in the current study, we analyze the eye movement data and evaluate temporal eye–hand coordination to determine the extent to which strabismus impacts visuomotor integration, and explore clinical and sensory factors associated with deficits. We hypothesize that children with strabismus will have slower saccades with more corrections made during the reach. Further, we predict that poorer control will be associated with impaired sensory binocular dysfunction (i.e., decreased or no measurable stereoacuity, suppression). The current analyses provides further insight into the role of vision and binocular function in the development of visuomotor integration, which may guide interventions to ameliorate or prevent eye–hand coordination impairments in children with strabismus. 
Methods
Participants
Children 7 to 12 years of age diagnosed with esotropic strabismus (herein called strabismus) alone or strabismus and anisometropia were referred to the Retina Foundation of the Southwest by Dallas–Fort Worth pediatric ophthalmologists. Strabismic children were aligned with surgery or spectacle correction to less than 12 prism diopters of orthotropia at near at the time of testing. Age-similar control children with age-normal visual acuity and stereoacuity and no history of vision disorders were also enrolled. Testing was completed with the child's habitual spectacle correction. Diagnosis, current alignment, and prior treatment were extracted from medical records obtained from the child's referring ophthalmologist. All children spoke English as their primary language. Children who were preterm (<37 weeks gestational age) or had coexisting ocular or systemic disease, congenital infections or malformations, or (neuro)developmental delay were excluded from the study. Only children with arm lengths (shoulder to fingertip) of 50 cm or greater were enrolled. 
Ethics
The research protocol observed the tenets of the Declaration of Helsinki, was approved by the Institutional Review Board of the University of Texas Southwestern Medical Center and conformed to the requirements of the United States Health Insurance Portability and Privacy Act. Informed consent was obtained from a parent or legal guardian and assent was obtained from children 10 years or more of age before testing and after explanation of the study. 
Procedure
Vision Assessment
A vision assessment was conducted before visually guided reaching, (1) Crowded monocular best-corrected visual acuity with the Electronic Early Treatment Diabetic Retinopathy Study protocol, scored in logMAR.33 Amblyopia was defined as an interocular difference of 0.2 or more logMAR, with best-corrected visual acuity in the fellow eye of 0.1 logMAR or less (20/25 or better). (2) Stereoacuity with the Randot Preschool Stereoacuity and Stereo Butterfly Tests,34 converted to log arcsec (ranging from 1.3 to 3.3 log arcsec). No measurable stereoacuity was arbitrarily assigned a value of 4 log arcsec. (3) Extent of suppression was quantified with the Worth four-dot fusion test at seven different distances, measured as the farthest distance that four dots are reported, converted to size of suppression scotoma in log degrees.35,36 
Visually Guided Reaching
A detailed description of the setup and testing protocol can be found in our recent article that reported reach kinematics data from this study.27 Briefly, children wore their habitual optical correction with both eyes open, used their self-reported dominant hand, and sat at a table with their head stabilized at a 35-cm viewing distance. The initial hand position required the child to use their index finger and thumb to hold a stick attached to the table at body midline 5 cm from the eyes (Fig. 1). Reach kinematics were recorded with the Leap Motion Controller system (software version 4.0; Leap Motion Inc., San Francisco, CA, USA) placed 10 cm in front of the initial hand position. Eye movements were simultaneously recorded with a 500-Hz high-speed video binocular eye tracker (EyeLink 1000; SR Research, Ontario, Canada) placed behind and above the display monitor 45 cm from the child's eyes. Piloting showed this eye tracker position was best to avoid occlusion of the eye tracker by the hand or display monitor. 
Figure 1.
 
Visually guided reaching experimental set up. Children held on to a stick placed 5 cm in front of them as they fixated a cross displayed on a computer monitor with both eyes open at a viewing distance of 35 cm. The cross then disappeared and a small white dot appeared on the left or right displaced 5° or 10° from fixation. The child was instructed to reach out and touch the dot with their index finger as quickly and accurately as possible, and then return to the stick. The EyeLink 1000 recorded eye movements and the Leap Motion Controller system (LMC) recorded hand movements.
Figure 1.
 
Visually guided reaching experimental set up. Children held on to a stick placed 5 cm in front of them as they fixated a cross displayed on a computer monitor with both eyes open at a viewing distance of 35 cm. The cross then disappeared and a small white dot appeared on the left or right displaced 5° or 10° from fixation. The child was instructed to reach out and touch the dot with their index finger as quickly and accurately as possible, and then return to the stick. The EyeLink 1000 recorded eye movements and the Leap Motion Controller system (LMC) recorded hand movements.
Separate five-point horizontal calibrations were performed for the index finger (touch each dot as accurately as possible) and the eyes (look at each dot for 4 seconds) during binocular viewing using a 0.3° white dot presented sequentially from left to right at −10°, −5°, 0°, +5°, and +10°. In the experimental trials, the child was instructed to fixate a white cross (1.4⁰) with a red dot in the middle centered on the screen with both eyes open. Once the cross disappeared, a 0.3° white dot appeared randomly at one of the four locations along the horizontal meridian (±5° or ±10° from fixation). The child was instructed to reach out and touch the dot with the tip of their index finger as quickly and accurately as possible. A total of 40 trials were completed per child, with the first four counting as practice trials (36 experimental trials). Testing time was approximately 15 minutes. 
Data Processing
Saccade Kinematics
Eye position data per eye were filtered with a low-pass second-order Butterworth filter and a cutoff frequency of 80 Hz. Filtered data was used to obtain eye velocity using a two-point differentiation method. A custom MATLAB script (MathWorks Inc, Natick, MA, USA) identified primary saccades using a velocity threshold of 30 deg/s. Each trial was inspected visually to confirm that saccades were correctly identified by the custom script and to ensure that both eyes moved together. A primary saccade was the first saccade that occurred within 80 to 1000 ms after target onset in the correct direction, with a gain of 30% or more of the expected amplitude. Corrective saccades were those occurring within 50 to 250 ms after the primary saccade. Reach-related saccades were those occurring more than 250 ms after the primary saccade ended and during the reach. This latency distinction between corrective and reach-related saccades is based on research showing that corrective saccades typically occur with a latency of 250 ms.37,38 To minimize the risk of categorizing a microsaccade as a corrective or reach-related saccade, we only included saccades that were 0.4° or more. Trials were excluded if data was missing (i.e., blink, lost tracking of eye) or noisy during the period from 250 ms before target onset to the end of the primary saccade. 
Mean saccade kinematic measures include (1) primary saccade latency: time from target onset to saccade initiation; (2) primary saccade gain: ratio of saccade amplitude to target amplitude, a measure of accuracy; (3) primary saccade precision: variability (i.e., standard deviation) of primary saccade gain; (4) primary saccade peak velocity (PV): maximum eye velocity attained during saccade; (5) final saccade gain: ratio of final saccade amplitude, which is the sum of the primary, corrective, and reach-related saccade amplitude, to target amplitude; (6) final saccade precision: variability of final saccade gain; (7) frequency of corrective saccades: percentage of trials that included a corrective saccade; and (8) frequency of reach-related saccades: percentage of trials that included a reach-related saccade. 
Temporal Eye–Hand Coordination
Details on reach kinematics data processing can be found in our previously published article.27 Using reach kinematics measures in combination with primary saccade latency, we calculated two temporal eye–hand coordination measures: (1) saccade-to-reach planning interval: interval between end of primary saccade and reach initiation, which reflects time available for planning the reaching response after the primary saccade was complete; and (2) saccade-to-reach PV interval: interval between end of the primary saccade and when PV was attained, which reflects amount of time after the eyes were in close vicinity of the target to the end of the initial stage of reach execution. 
Statistical Analyses
Primary Analyses
Independent t tests were used to compare strabismic children to control children for all saccade kinematics and temporal eye–hand coordination measures. Effect size was calculated using Cohen's d
Secondary Analyses
Kruskal-Wallis one-way ANOVAs were conducted to determine clinical and sensory factors related to performance: prior surgery (yes, no); amblyopia present (yes, no); stereoacuity measurable (present, not present); extent of suppression scotoma (bifoveal–macular fusion, −0.15 to 0.45 log deg; peripheral–no fusion, 0.60 to 1.2 log deg). Significant ANOVAs were followed with Mann–Whitney U post hoc tests. All tests were corrected for multiple comparisons and P values were adjusted using Holm's sequential Bonferroni procedure, which corrects for type I error as effectively as the traditional Bonferroni method while retaining more statistical power.39 Children with fewer than 14 useable saccade trials (at least 7 useable trials per side, left/right) were excluded from further analysis. 
Results
Reach kinematic data from 36 strabismic children and 35 control children for this task have been published.27 Of the children tested, eye movement data were available from 30 strabismic children (female = 20; mean age, 9.7 ± 1.8 years) and 32 control children (female = 18; 9.6 ± 1.8 years). The remaining 6 strabismic children and 3 control children were not included due to having fewer than 14 useable saccade trials because of artefacts, blinks, or poor calibration. Children with strabismus did not differ from controls in age (P = 0.79) or arm length (P = 0.32). (See Table 1 for group characteristics.) 
Table 1.
 
Group Characteristics
Table 1.
 
Group Characteristics
Saccade Kinematics
No interocular differences (strabismus, nonpreferred vs. preferred eyes; control, right vs. left eyes) were found for either group. Therefore, only the preferred eye (left eye for controls) was included in the analysis. See Figure 2 for group comparisons of saccade kinematic measures, and Figure 3 for example eye traces from a typical child with strabismus and a control. 
Figure 2.
 
Violin plots displaying the distribution of saccade kinematic measures for strabismic children compared with controls. For each violin plot, the embedded boxplot represents the interquartile range, the black cross represents the mean, and black horizontal lines represent the median. Strabismic children were similar to controls for primary saccade PV (B), primary saccade gain (C), and final saccade gain (E), but had significantly longer primary saccade latency (A), and decreased primary saccade gain (D) and final saccade gain (F).
Figure 2.
 
Violin plots displaying the distribution of saccade kinematic measures for strabismic children compared with controls. For each violin plot, the embedded boxplot represents the interquartile range, the black cross represents the mean, and black horizontal lines represent the median. Strabismic children were similar to controls for primary saccade PV (B), primary saccade gain (C), and final saccade gain (E), but had significantly longer primary saccade latency (A), and decreased primary saccade gain (D) and final saccade gain (F).
Figure 3.
 
Example eye traces showing increased saccade variability (i.e., decreased precision) in a child with strabismus (top) compared with a control child (bottom) for each target position.
Figure 3.
 
Example eye traces showing increased saccade variability (i.e., decreased precision) in a child with strabismus (top) compared with a control child (bottom) for each target position.
Latency
Strabismic children had longer saccade latency than controls (strabismus, 195 ± 29 ms vs. control; 175 ± 23 ms; t60 = 3.0; P = 0.004; d = 0.8). 
Accuracy
No difference between groups was found for primary saccade gain (strabismus, 0.94 ± 0.11 vs. control, 0.95 ± 0.07; t60 = 0.44; P = 0.67; d = 0.1) or for final gain (strabismus, 1.00 ± 0.06 vs. control, 1.00 ± 0.05; t60 = 0.34; P = 0.71; d = 0.1). 
Precision
Strabismic children had a 25% decrease in primary saccade precision (strabismus, 0.15 ± 0.06 vs. control, 0.12 ± 0.02; t60 = 3.23; P = 0.002; d = 0.8) and a 45% decrease in final saccade precision (strabismus, 0.16 ± 0.06 vs. control, 0.11 ± 0.03; t60 = 3.9; P < 0.001; d = 1.0) compared with controls 
PV
No difference between groups was found for PV (strabismus, 324 ± 60 deg/sec vs. control, 330 ± 44 deg/sec; t60 = 0.41; P = 0.69; d = 0.1). 
Corrective and Reach-Related Saccades
No group difference was found for frequency of corrective saccades (strabismus, 37 ± 16% vs. control, 38 ± 18%; t60 = 0.32; P = 0.75; d = 0.08). However, strabismic children had more reach-related saccades than controls (strabismus, 16 ± 13 % vs. control, 8 ± 6 %; t60 = 3.33; P = 0.001; d = 0.85). (See Fig. 4.) 
Figure 4.
 
Violin plots displaying the distribution of the percentage of corrective saccades and reach-related saccades for strabismic children compared with controls. For each violin plot, the embedded boxplot represents the interquartile range, the black cross represents the mean, and black horizontal lines represent the median. Strabismic children had a similar frequency of corrective saccades as controls (A), but more reach-related saccades than controls (B).
Figure 4.
 
Violin plots displaying the distribution of the percentage of corrective saccades and reach-related saccades for strabismic children compared with controls. For each violin plot, the embedded boxplot represents the interquartile range, the black cross represents the mean, and black horizontal lines represent the median. Strabismic children had a similar frequency of corrective saccades as controls (A), but more reach-related saccades than controls (B).
Temporal Eye–Hand Coordination
No group difference was found for the saccade-to-reach planning interval (strabismus, 110 ± 78 ms vs. control, 136 ± 64 ms; t60 = 1.42; P = 0.16; d = 0.36) or the saccade-to-reach PV interval (strabismus, 311 ± 91 ms vs. control, 330 ± 75 ms; t60 = 0.88; P = 0.38; d = 0.22). (See Fig. 5 and Fig. 6.) 
Figure 5.
 
Examples of a typical visually guided reaching trial for a child with strabismus (top) and a control child (bottom). The dotted line indicates primary saccade latency (SL). Included in both examples are the saccade-to-reach planning interval (S-R) and the saccade-to-reach-PV interval (S-PV), and a reach-related saccade (RRS) in the strabismus example only. An asterisk (*) in the strabismus example indicates group mean is significantly different than controls.
Figure 5.
 
Examples of a typical visually guided reaching trial for a child with strabismus (top) and a control child (bottom). The dotted line indicates primary saccade latency (SL). Included in both examples are the saccade-to-reach planning interval (S-R) and the saccade-to-reach-PV interval (S-PV), and a reach-related saccade (RRS) in the strabismus example only. An asterisk (*) in the strabismus example indicates group mean is significantly different than controls.
Figure 6.
 
Violin plots displaying the distribution of the saccade-to-reach planning interval (A) and the saccade-to-reach PV interval (B) for strabismic children compared with controls. For each violin plot, the embedded boxplot represents the interquartile range, the black cross represents the mean, and black horizontal lines represent the median. No group differences were found.
Figure 6.
 
Violin plots displaying the distribution of the saccade-to-reach planning interval (A) and the saccade-to-reach PV interval (B) for strabismic children compared with controls. For each violin plot, the embedded boxplot represents the interquartile range, the black cross represents the mean, and black horizontal lines represent the median. No group differences were found.
Factors Associated With Saccade Kinematics
We further probed why strabismic children had longer saccade latency, decreased primary and final saccade precision, and more reach-related saccades by considering clinical and sensory factors. Compared with controls, the following factors were related to poor performance for (1) primary saccade latency: nonamblyopic (U = 91; P < 0.001), stereoacuity not present (U = 163; P = 0.002), bifoveal–macular fusion (U = 159; P = 0.001), (2) primary saccade precision: nonamblyopic (U = 107; P = 0.003), stereoacuity not present (U = 109; P < 0.001), peripheral-no fusion (U = 56; P = 0.005), (3) final saccade precision: no surgery (U = 107; P = 0.001), stereoacuity not present (U = 134; P < 0.001), and (4) reach-related saccades: stereoacuity not present (U = 119; P < 0.001), peripheral-no fusion (U = 58; P = 0.006). The only factor showing a group difference within the strabismic group was stereoacuity; strabismic children with stereoacuity not present had decreased primary saccade precision and more reach-related saccades than strabismic children with stereoacuity present. (See Table 2.) 
Table 2.
 
Factors Affecting Saccade Kinematics in Strabismic Children Compared With Controls
Table 2.
 
Factors Affecting Saccade Kinematics in Strabismic Children Compared With Controls
Between the ages of 7 and 12 years, a transition occurs from beginning to use information derived from visual feedback to the acquisition of more integrated feedforward-feedback control.40,41 For those measures that were impaired in strabismic children compared with controls (saccade latency, saccade precision, reach-related saccades), we compared 7- to 9-year-old children with 10- to 12-year-old children within the strabismic group to determine whether any improvement occurs with age. The only measure that improved with age was primary saccade latency (7−9 years, 205 ± 31 ms vs. 10−12 years, 180 ± 16 ms; P = 0.015; d = 1.0). In contrast, final saccade precision was worse in the older age group (7−9 years, 0.14 ± 0.05 ms vs. 10−12 years, 0.18 ± 16 ms; P = 0.015; d = 1.0). 
Discussion
Children treated for strabismus have prolonged saccade onset latency during visually guided reaching while viewing binocularly, consistent with previous studies in strabismic adults.8,12 Longer latencies may point to an immaturity of controlling visual fixation (i.e., disengaging fixation) that occurs before saccade onset.42 Fixation instability, which is a hallmark of strabismus, may impact the timing of saccade initiation.6,9,10 Because saccade latency reflects the time it takes to program the saccade before initiation, prolonged saccade latency could also reflect a delay in sensorimotor transformation. In other words, there may be a delay in processing the visual information about the location and distance of the target, converting that information into a planned motor command (i.e., the saccade), and then executing that motor command. Spatial distortions and positional uncertainty are present in strabismus43,44 and could impact this sensorimotor transformation during visually guided reaching. 
Strabismic children initiated reach-related saccades twice as frequently as controls (16% vs. 8% of trials), consistent with strabismic adults (11%–15% of trials).32 Inconsistent with strabismic adults who have more corrective saccades,12 no difference in frequency of corrective saccades (i.e., occurred before reach initiation) was found. For strabismic children, the majority of corrective saccades (92%) occurred before or during the acceleration (initial) phase of the reach. Corrective saccades are common in normal vision and may be prepared at the same time as the primary saccade.38 Strabismic children may be overshooting or undershooting the target, with reach-related saccades being generated to correct the positional error that remained after the primary saccade. This is supported by our finding of a 25% to 45% decrease in saccade precision, despite a mean saccade gain comparable with controls. The majority of reach-related saccades (82%) in the strabismic group occurring during the deceleration (final) phase of the reach. However, the initial variability in saccades was not rectified by these reach-related saccades, evidenced by the lack of difference between primary (0.15) and final (0.16) saccade precision, which may be impacting the lower touch accuracy found in this group.27 Again, spatial distortions and positional uncertainty43,44 in encoding the visual information could impact the precision of saccades. 
Coupled with slower reaching in the final approach,27 an increase in the incidence of reach-related saccades, especially in the final approach to the target, suggests a reliance on visual feedback that is less efficient during the reach. It is also possible that the increased incidence of reach-related saccades is due to fixation instability,6,9,10 which would increase the variability of the primary saccade. 
No group differences in temporal eye–hand coordination measures were found (saccade-to-reach, saccade-to-reach PV intervals). These data are in contrast with those from strabismic adults, who show a longer saccade-to-reach PV interval compared with controls,32 particularly if their binocular vision was deficient, pointing to a deficit in the planning and initial execution of the reach. Because the saccade-to-reach planning interval and saccade-to-reach PV interval both exclude the final approach of the reach where strabismic children are slowest, no impairment on our temporal eye–hand coordination measures is not surprising and points to inefficient use of visual feedback as the culprit for slower reaching. It may then appear that the delay in saccade initiation (but normal saccade velocity and accuracy), and the slow reach in the final but not initial approach27 point to a problem with saccades and reaches individually rather than there being a problem with visuomotor integration. Alternatively, despite normal saccade velocity and accuracy, saccade precision was decreased and the incidence of reach-related saccades was increased in strabismic children, suggesting that visuomotor integration is indeed impacted. A reach-related saccade is an extra step that needs to be planned and executed, suggesting that additional information is required after the primary saccade to reach the target properly, and thus changing the coordination pattern. 
Coupled with our previously reported reach kinematic data,27 the current findings suggest that children aged 7 to 12 years treated for strabismus have not yet adapted or formed an efficient compensatory strategy for visually guided reaching while binocularly viewing. Strabismic children take longer to initiate a saccade to a target before reaching, take longer to reach to the target owing to more time spent in the final approach,27 and produce more reach-related saccades. Strabismic adults also take longer to saccade to the target and produce more reach-related saccades, but, unlike strabismic children, they spend more time in the initial approach, and produce more corrective saccades before the reach.12,31,32 Therefore, the strategy for visually guided reaching in strabismic individuals changes from relying more on visual feedback for online control during childhood to relying more on the visuomotor plan in adulthood. At 7 years of age, children are just learning to use visual feedback for online corrections40,41; the use of this feedback is not yet mastered and may be less efficient in strabismic children. This is evidenced by our finding that final saccade precision was worse by 29% in older strabismic children compared with younger strabismic children, despite a quicker saccade latency. This finding may reflect a speed–accuracy tradeoff. Even with quicker saccades, strabismic children age 10 to 12 years were still slower than controls aged 10 to 12 years (161 ± 15 ms; P = 0.002). Our findings point to a change in compensatory strategies that develop with age. It is unknown at what age this switch occurs because there are no data in teenagers with strabismus. 
Nonamblyopic children had prolonged saccade latency and decreased primary and final saccade precision, whereas those with amblyopia only exhibited a decreased final saccade precision. Nonamblyopic strabismic adults also show prolonged saccade latency (191 ± 29 ms), whereas amblyopic strabismic adults do not (177 ± 39 ms).12 This difference may reflect the fact that infantile esotropia (before 12 months of age) is accompanied by poorer binocularity status but typically does not result in amblyopia.45 In this study, seven of the strabismic children had an early onset of strabismus. However, variance of the saccade amplitude precision in the amblyopic group was large, despite having a similar mean as the nonamblyopic group, and may account for the lack of significance. This may also hold true for extent of suppression; both categories yielded similar mean saccade latencies and precision, but only one category was significantly different from controls (see Table 2). The disconjugacy of saccades in strabismus significantly decreases, but saccade accuracy remains the same after strabismus surgery.46 In our study, all strabismic children were aligned within 12 prism diopters; thus, the disconjugacy of saccades would have been minimal. Certainly, we found no interocular difference in saccade kinematics, suggesting that saccade disconjugacies are not the cause of the increased latency or the decreased precision. 
A longer saccade latency, decreased saccade precision, and more reach-related saccades were related to having no measurable stereoacuity and peripheral–no fusion. Poor binocular status is associated with poor ocular motor function in strabismus, including decreased vergence7,11 and abnormal saccade initiation and execution.8,12,13 During visuomotor tasks, binocular cues provide vital information about an object's distance, location, and three-dimensional properties.14 Good binocularity is important for eye–hand coordination,16,21,25,26,4749 and the use of binocular cues may be disrupted by strabismus early in life. This finding is supported not only by better performance in strabismic children with better binocularity in our study, but also by end point inaccuracies during reaching and grasping in the strabismic children in our study and children with binocular dysfunctions in a previous study.16 Previous studies also show better motor performance in those with recovered binocularity,16,50 suggesting that binocularity contributes to optimum planning and execution of visually guided reaching. 
Our study had potential limitations. It is possible microsaccades were incorrectly categorized as corrective or reach-related saccades. To minimize this risk, we used an inclusion criterion of 0.4° or more amplitude in both eyes. It is challenging to tease apart the individual contributions of clinical and sensory factors because they often coexist,4 especially with small sample sizes in two of the sensory categories (peripheral–no fusion, n = 9; stereoacuity present, n = 9). However, our data, along with previous studies on eye–hand coordination in strabismus, point to the role that binocular dysfunction plays in impaired visuomotor ability. Although we were unable to control for experience with eye–hand coordination, our task was a simple reaching task with which all children will have had experience, regardless of enrollment in physical recreational activities. 
Conclusions
Strabismus impacts saccade kinematics during visually guided reaching in children, with poor binocularity playing a role in performance. There seems to be a reorganization of motor control with age; rather, a switch from a disruption in execution in childhood to a change in planning in adulthood that impacts visually guided reaching in individuals with strabismus, suggestive of a compensatory adaptation of reaching. Understanding this switch and the processes underlying impairments in eye–hand coordination may lead to interventions targeted at preventing or ameliorating slow reaching or slow eye movement latency in strabismic children. 
Acknowledgments
Supported by the National Eye Institute (EY028224). 
Presented at the 2022 annual meeting of AAPOS. 
Disclosure: K.R. Kelly, None; D.M. Norouzi, None; M. Nouredanesh, None; R.M. Jost, None; C.S. Cheng-Patel, None; C.L. Beauchamp, None; L.M. Dao, None; B.A. Luu, None; D.R. Stager, Jr, None; J.Y. Tung, None; E. Niechwiej-Szwedo, None 
References
Donnelly UM, Stewart NM, Hollinger M. Prevalence and outcomes of childhood visual disorders. Ophthalmic Epidemiol. 2005; 12(4): 243–250. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Friedman DS, Repka MX, Katz J, et al. Prevalence of amblyopia and strabismus in white and African American children aged 6 through 71 months the Baltimore Pediatric Eye Disease Study. Ophthalmology. 2009; 116(11): 2128–2134.e1-2. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Birch EE. Marshall Parks lecture. Binocular sensory outcomes in accommodative ET. J AAPOS. 2003; 7(6): 369–373. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Birch EE. Amblyopia and binocular vision. Prog Retin Eye Res. 2013; 33: 67–84. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Birch EE, Morale SE, Jost RM, et al. Assessing suppression in amblyopic children with a dichoptic eye chart. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2016; 57(13): 5649–5654. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Kelly KR, Cheng-Patel CS, Jost RM, Wang Y-ZZ, Birch EE. Fixation instability during binocular viewing in anisometropic and strabismic children. Exp Eye Res. 2018; 183: 29–37. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Kelly KR, Felius J, Ramachandran S, et al. Congenitally impaired disparity vergence in children with infantile esotropia. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2016; 57(6): 2545–2550. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Perdziak M, Witkowska DK, Gryncewicz W, Ober JK. Not only amblyopic but also dominant eye in subjects with strabismus show increased saccadic latency. J Vis. 2016; 16(10): 1–11. [CrossRef]
Ciuffreda KJ, Kenyon RV, Stark L. Saccadic intrusions in strabismus. Arch Ophthalmol. 1979; 97(9): 1673–1679. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Ghasia FF, Otero-Millan J, Shaikh AG. Abnormal fixational eye movements in strabismus. Br J Ophthalmol. 2018; 102: 253–259. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Morad Y, Lee H, Westall C, et al. Dynamic fusional vergence eye movements in congenital esotropia. Open Ophthalmol J. 2008; 2: 9–14. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Niechwiej-Szwedo E, Chandrakumar M, Goltz HC, Wong AMF. Effects of strabismic amblyopia and strabismus without amblyopia on visuomotor behavior, I: saccadic eye movements. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2012; 53(12): 7458–7468. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Bucci MP, Kapoula Z, Yang Q, Brémond-Gignac D. Latency of saccades, vergence, and combined movements in children with early onset convergent or divergent strabismus. Vision Res. 2006; 46(8-9): 1384–1392. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Melmoth DR, Grant S. Advantages of binocular vision for the control of reaching and grasping. Exp Eye Res. 2006; 171(3): 371–388. [CrossRef]
Melmoth DR, Storoni M, Todd G, Finlay AL, Grant S. Dissociation between vergence and binocular disparity cues in the control of prehension. Exp Eye Res. 2007; 183(3): 283–298. [CrossRef]
Grant S, Suttle C, Melmoth DR, Conway ML, Sloper JJ. Age- and stereovision-dependent eye–hand coordination deficits in children with amblyopia and abnormal binocularity. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2014; 55(9): 5687–5701. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Niechwiej-Szwedo E, Meier K, Christian L, et al. Concurrent maturation of visuomotor skills and motion perception in typically-developing children and adolescents. Dev Psychobiol. 2019(August): 1–15.
Niechwiej-Szwedo E, Thai G, Christian L. Contribution of stereopsis, vergence, and accommodative function to the performance of a precision grasping and placement task in typically developing children age 8-14 years. Hum Mov Sci. 2020; 72: 102652. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Giaschi D, Narasimhan S, Solski A, Harrison E, Wilcox LM. On the typical development of stereopsis: fine and coarse processing. Vision Res. 2013; 89: 65–71. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Birch E, Petrig B. FPL and VEP measures of fusion, stereopsis and stereoacuity in normal infants. Vision Res. 1996; 36(9): 1321–1327. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
O'Connor AR, Birch EE, Anderson S, Draper H. Relationship between binocular vision, visual acuity, and fine motor skills. Optom Vis Sci. 2010; 87(12): 942–947. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Webber AL, Wood JM, Gole GA, Brown B. The effect of amblyopia on fine motor skills in children. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2008; 49(2): 594–603. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Kelly KR, Morale SE, Beauchamp CL, et al. Factors associated with impaired motor skills in strabismic and anisometropic children. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2020; 61(10): 43. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Kelly KR, Jost RM, De La, Cruz A, Birch EE. Multiple-choice answer form completion time in children with amblyopia and strabismus. JAMA Ophthalmol. 2018; 136(8): 938–941. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Grant S, Moseley MJ. Amblyopia and real-world visuomotor tasks. Strabismus. 2011; 19(3): 119–128. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
O'Connor AR, Birch EE, Anderson S, Draper H. The functional significance of stereopsis. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2010; 51(4): 2019–2023. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Kelly KR, Jr., Hunter J, Norouzi DM, et al. Reach kinematics during binocular viewing in 7- to 12-year-old children with strabismus. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2021; 62(15): 21. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Desmurget M, Turner RS, Prablanc C, et al. Updating target location at the end of an orienting saccade affects the characteristics of simple point-to-point movements. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 2005; 31(6): 1510–1536. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Fisk JD, Goodale MA. The organization of eye and limb movements during unrestricted reaching to targets in contralateral and ipsilateral visual space. Exp Brain Res. 1985; 60(1): 159–178. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
de Brouwer AJ, Flanagan JR, Spering M. Functional use of eye movements for an acting system. Trends Cogn Sci. 2021; 25(3): 252–263. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Niechwiej-Szwedo E, Goltz HC, Chandrakumar M, Wong AMF. Effects of strabismic amblyopia on visuomotor behaviour: Part II. Visually-guided reaching. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2014; 55(12): 7831–7838. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Niechwiej-Szwedo E, Goltz HC, Chandrakumar M, Wong AMF. Effects of strabismic amblyopia and strabismus without amblyopia on visuomotor behavior: III. Temporal eye-hand coordination during reaching. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2014; 55(12): 7831–7838. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Moke PS, Turpin AH, Beck RW, et al. Computerized method of visual acuity testing: adaptation of the amblyopia treatment study visual acuity testing protocol. Am J Ophthalmol. 2001; 132(6): 903–909. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Birch E, Williams C, Drover J, et al. Randot Preschool Stereoacuity Test: normative data and validity. J AAPOS. 2008; 12(1): 23–26. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Rosenbaum A, Santiago A. Clinical Strabismus Management. Philadelphia: WB Saunders; 1999.
Webber AL, Wood JM, Thompson B, Birch EE. From suppression to stereoacuity: a composite binocular function score for clinical research. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt. 2019; 39(1): 53–62. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Cohen ME, Ross LE. Latency and accuracy characteristics of saccades and corrective saccades in children and adults J Exp Child Psychol. 1978; 26: 517–527. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Tian J, Ying HS, Zee DS. Revisiting corrective saccades: role of visual feedback. Vision Res. 2013; 89: 54–64. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Eichstaedt KE, Kovatch K, Maroof DA. A less conservative method to adjust for familywise error rate in neuropsychological research: the Holm's sequential Bonferroni procedure. NeuroRehabilitation. 2013; 32(3): 693–696. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Smyth MM, Peacock KA, Katamba J. The role of sight of the hand in the development of prehension in childhood. Q J Exp Psychol A. 2004; 57(2): 269–296. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Hay L. Spatial-temporal analysis of movements in children. J Mot Beh. 1979; 11(3): 189–200. [CrossRef]
Munoz DP, Broughton JR, Goldring JE, Armstrong IE. Age-related performance of human subjects on saccadic eye movement tasks. Exp Brain Res. 1998; 121: 391–400. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Lagreze WD, Sireteanu R. Two-dimensional spatial distortions in human strabismic amblyopia. Vision Res. 1991; 31(7-8): 1271–1288. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Sireteanu R, Lagreze WD, Constantinescu DH. Distortions in two-dimensional visual space perception in strabismic observers. Vision Res. 1993; 33(5-6): 677–690. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Birch EE, Stager DR. Long-term motor and sensory outcomes after early surgery for infantile esotropia. J AAPOS. 2006; 10(5): 409–413. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Bucci MP, Kapoula Z, Yang Q, Roussat B, Bremond-Gignac D. Binocular coordination of saccades in children with strabismus before and after surgery. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2002; 43(4): 1040–1047. [PubMed]
Grant S, Melmoth DR, Morgan MJ, Finlay AL. Prehension deficits in amblyopia. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2007; 48(3): 1139–1148. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Niechwiej-Szwedo E, Kennedy SA, Colpa L, et al. Effects of induced monocular blur versus anisometropic amblyopia on saccades, reaching, and eye-hand coordination. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2012; 53(8): 4354–4362. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Grant S, Conway ML. Some binocular advantages for planning reach, but not grasp, components of prehension. Exp Eye Res. 2019; 237(5): 1239–1255. [CrossRef]
Grant S, Conway ML. Reach-to-precision grasp deficits in amblyopia: Effects of object contrast and low visibility. Vision Res. 2015; 114: 100–110. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1.
 
Visually guided reaching experimental set up. Children held on to a stick placed 5 cm in front of them as they fixated a cross displayed on a computer monitor with both eyes open at a viewing distance of 35 cm. The cross then disappeared and a small white dot appeared on the left or right displaced 5° or 10° from fixation. The child was instructed to reach out and touch the dot with their index finger as quickly and accurately as possible, and then return to the stick. The EyeLink 1000 recorded eye movements and the Leap Motion Controller system (LMC) recorded hand movements.
Figure 1.
 
Visually guided reaching experimental set up. Children held on to a stick placed 5 cm in front of them as they fixated a cross displayed on a computer monitor with both eyes open at a viewing distance of 35 cm. The cross then disappeared and a small white dot appeared on the left or right displaced 5° or 10° from fixation. The child was instructed to reach out and touch the dot with their index finger as quickly and accurately as possible, and then return to the stick. The EyeLink 1000 recorded eye movements and the Leap Motion Controller system (LMC) recorded hand movements.
Figure 2.
 
Violin plots displaying the distribution of saccade kinematic measures for strabismic children compared with controls. For each violin plot, the embedded boxplot represents the interquartile range, the black cross represents the mean, and black horizontal lines represent the median. Strabismic children were similar to controls for primary saccade PV (B), primary saccade gain (C), and final saccade gain (E), but had significantly longer primary saccade latency (A), and decreased primary saccade gain (D) and final saccade gain (F).
Figure 2.
 
Violin plots displaying the distribution of saccade kinematic measures for strabismic children compared with controls. For each violin plot, the embedded boxplot represents the interquartile range, the black cross represents the mean, and black horizontal lines represent the median. Strabismic children were similar to controls for primary saccade PV (B), primary saccade gain (C), and final saccade gain (E), but had significantly longer primary saccade latency (A), and decreased primary saccade gain (D) and final saccade gain (F).
Figure 3.
 
Example eye traces showing increased saccade variability (i.e., decreased precision) in a child with strabismus (top) compared with a control child (bottom) for each target position.
Figure 3.
 
Example eye traces showing increased saccade variability (i.e., decreased precision) in a child with strabismus (top) compared with a control child (bottom) for each target position.
Figure 4.
 
Violin plots displaying the distribution of the percentage of corrective saccades and reach-related saccades for strabismic children compared with controls. For each violin plot, the embedded boxplot represents the interquartile range, the black cross represents the mean, and black horizontal lines represent the median. Strabismic children had a similar frequency of corrective saccades as controls (A), but more reach-related saccades than controls (B).
Figure 4.
 
Violin plots displaying the distribution of the percentage of corrective saccades and reach-related saccades for strabismic children compared with controls. For each violin plot, the embedded boxplot represents the interquartile range, the black cross represents the mean, and black horizontal lines represent the median. Strabismic children had a similar frequency of corrective saccades as controls (A), but more reach-related saccades than controls (B).
Figure 5.
 
Examples of a typical visually guided reaching trial for a child with strabismus (top) and a control child (bottom). The dotted line indicates primary saccade latency (SL). Included in both examples are the saccade-to-reach planning interval (S-R) and the saccade-to-reach-PV interval (S-PV), and a reach-related saccade (RRS) in the strabismus example only. An asterisk (*) in the strabismus example indicates group mean is significantly different than controls.
Figure 5.
 
Examples of a typical visually guided reaching trial for a child with strabismus (top) and a control child (bottom). The dotted line indicates primary saccade latency (SL). Included in both examples are the saccade-to-reach planning interval (S-R) and the saccade-to-reach-PV interval (S-PV), and a reach-related saccade (RRS) in the strabismus example only. An asterisk (*) in the strabismus example indicates group mean is significantly different than controls.
Figure 6.
 
Violin plots displaying the distribution of the saccade-to-reach planning interval (A) and the saccade-to-reach PV interval (B) for strabismic children compared with controls. For each violin plot, the embedded boxplot represents the interquartile range, the black cross represents the mean, and black horizontal lines represent the median. No group differences were found.
Figure 6.
 
Violin plots displaying the distribution of the saccade-to-reach planning interval (A) and the saccade-to-reach PV interval (B) for strabismic children compared with controls. For each violin plot, the embedded boxplot represents the interquartile range, the black cross represents the mean, and black horizontal lines represent the median. No group differences were found.
Table 1.
 
Group Characteristics
Table 1.
 
Group Characteristics
Table 2.
 
Factors Affecting Saccade Kinematics in Strabismic Children Compared With Controls
Table 2.
 
Factors Affecting Saccade Kinematics in Strabismic Children Compared With Controls
×
×

This PDF is available to Subscribers Only

Sign in or purchase a subscription to access this content. ×

You must be signed into an individual account to use this feature.

×