Abstract
Purpose:
Center-surround contrast suppression—typically induced when a center pattern is surrounded by another pattern with similar spatial features—is considered a perceptual analogue of center-surround neurophysiology in the visual system. Surround suppression strength is altered in a range of brain conditions affecting young people (e.g., schizophrenia, depression, migraine) and is modulated by various neurotransmitters. The early teen years are associated with neurotransmitter changes in the human visual cortex, which could impact on excitation–inhibition balance and center-surround antagonistic effects. Hence, we predict that early adolescence is associated with perceptual changes in center-surround suppression.
Methods:
In this cross-sectional study, we tested 196 students at every age from 10 to 17 years and 30 adults (aged 21–34 years) to capture the preteen, adolescent, and adult periods. Contrast discrimination thresholds were measured for a central, circular, vertical sinusoidal grating pattern (0.67° radius, 2 cyc/deg spatial frequency, 2 deg/s drift rate) with and without the surround (4° radius, otherwise same spatial properties as the center). Individual suppression strength was determined by comparing the perceived contrast of the target with and without the surround.
Results:
After excluding unreliable data (7% of total), we found an effect of age on perceptual center-surround contrast suppression strength, F(8,201) = 2.30, P = 0.02, with weaker suppression in the youngest adolescents relative to adults (Bonferroni pairwise comparisons between adults vs 12-year-olds P = 0.01; adults vs 13-year-olds P = 0.002).
Conclusions:
Our data demonstrate different center-surround interactions in the visual system—a key building block for visual perception—in early adolescence relative to adulthood.
The developmental trajectories of fundamental visual functions are generally well-understood. Characteristic features of visual cortical neurons such as orientation, spatial frequency, and direction selectivity are established during infancy, as inferred from clinical visual electrophysiology and behavioral studies.
1 Visual acuity improves gradually during childhood
2 (most studies report adult-like acuity by approximately 6–7 years of age
3–6), whereas contrast sensitivity development is more protracted, continuing into late childhood
6–9 and adolescence.
10,11 However, most standard clinical tests of vision use uniform backgrounds that do not reflect the conditions that commonly occur in natural visual experience. In natural vision, objects are often present in nonuniform or cluttered backgrounds, giving rise to contextual effects. Contextual effects occur because of center-surround receptive field antagonism, where extraclassical receptive field stimulation modulates a neuron's response—a ubiquitous property of the primary visual cortex (V1),
12–15 but also present at precortical
16 and extrastriate levels of the visual system.
17 Center-surround antagonism is a fundamental building block of vision and critical for the processing and efficient coding of natural visual stimuli,
18,19 by reducing highly redundant information and serving an important functional role in daily visual tasks like object boundary identification,
20 figure–ground segmentation,
21 and collinear contour integration.
22
A well-studied visual contextual effect is center-surround suppression of contrast, where the perceived contrast of a target is typically decreased (suppressed) by a surrounding pattern, provided the surround has similar spatial properties to the target stimulus.
23 The psychophysical properties of center-surround suppression, such as the stimulus parameters to elicit robust suppressive effects, are well-established and have a sound basis in convergent neurophysiological work.
24,25 Animal studies have discovered the neural circuitry responsible for center-surround suppression, including intra-V1 horizontal connections, feedforward contributions from the lateral geniculate nucleus, and feedback from extrastriate areas V2,
26 V3,
19 and V5/MT.
19 Human studies point to a range of brain neurotransmitters—not solely inhibitory
27–32—that regulate the fundamental neural computations of gain control and divisive normalization and are likely to underpin center-surround antagonistic effects
There are multiple sites and mechanisms by which brain disorder can result in perceptual changes to center-surround suppression and, as such, growing interest in measuring perceptual center-surround effects in conditions affecting young people like migraine,
33 schizophrenia,
34–38 bipolar disorder,
34 and autism spectrum disorder.
39,40 However, these studies have exclusively tested adults, despite global estimates that 1 in 7 (14%) adolescents experience mental health conditions
41 and almost 10% of adolescents suffer from migraine.
42,43 Adolescence, the transitional stage of life between childhood and adulthood, is a critical period for brain development, marked by changes in neurobehavioral function and neural circuitry.
44,45 Most studies of adolescent brain development focus on the association and prefrontal cortices as the “higher” brain areas involved in more complex cognitive and affective functions.
45–47 However, post mortem analysis of human visual cortex indicates that the early teen years are associated with rapid changes in neurotransmitter systems (e.g., the excitatory neurotransmitter glutamate and inhibitory neurotransmitter gamma aminobutyric acid [GABA])
48,49 that could plausibly impact on excitation–inhibition balance.
Developmental studies of vision typically compare pediatric age groups against adult performance, assuming a monotonic improvement with age. Not all studies systematically test vision during childhood, adolescence, and adulthood to determine the point at which visual function becomes adult-like,
2 and some studies omit testing adolescents altogether.
6,7,9,50–52 Here we measured center-surround contrast suppression in preteens, younger and older adolescents, and adults. We predicted that the strength of perceptual center-surround suppression might differ in early adolescence—when there are cortical neurotransmitter changes that could influence center-surround antagonism in visual cortex
48,49—relative to adulthood. The results of this study may be used to inform future research aimed at better understanding the brain mechanism(s) underlying atypical visual performance in young neurotypical and clinical populations.
Participants completed two tasks: a no surround and surround condition. For each task, two runs were completed. The first task was always the no surround condition (
Fig. 1A), to estimate the matching contrast of the target stimulus without the influence of a surround. We also used the task to confirm that participants could reliably perform contrast judgments and check for systematic bias in button pressing (two alternative forced choice). The visual stimuli were two drifting vertically oriented sinusoidal gratings (0.67° radius, 2 cyc/deg spatial frequency, 2 deg/s drift rate), presented adjacent to one another for 500 ms duration. The application chose 1 of 10 pregenerated image variants to present each time to randomize drift direction and phase between trials. The left reference stimulus was fixed at 40% Michelson contrast, and the right stimulus contrast varied according to two interleaved one-down one-up staircases starting at 60% contrast and terminating after four reversals (step sizes: 8%, 4%, 2%, and 2% contrast). The starting contrast was chosen to be at a suprathreshold contrast level (sufficiently above the veridical contrast of 40%; i.e., the expected approximate contrast match) to ensure that participants could demonstrate the correct contrast discrimination judgment from the beginning of each test run. The final two reversals of each staircase were averaged to determine the matching contrast.
For the second surround task (
Fig. 1B), procedures were identical to the no surround condition except that the left reference stimulus was a center-surround pattern (0.67° center radius, 4° surround radius, 40% center contrast, 95% surround contrast, gratings aligned in phase). For a 40% contrast center and 95% contrast surround pattern, the perceived contrast of the center was expected to be lower than the veridical contrast (40%) if there is perceptual suppression. In that case, at a starting contrast of 40%, the two stimuli will look different and so the first decision would be easy for most participants. We did not choose to start at 60% contrast like the no surround condition to accommodate participants who have a strong surround suppressive effect (i.e., perceived contrast closer to 0%). The final two reversals of each staircase were averaged to estimate the perceived contrast of the center when surrounded. A decrease in perceived contrast for the surround condition, relative to the no surround condition, indicates suppression. Suppression indices were calculated according to
Equation 124,57 where an index of 1 indicates maximal suppression and an index of 0 indicates no suppression.
\begin{equation}
1 - \frac{{Perceived{\rm{\;}}contrast{\rm{\;}}with{\rm{\;}}surround}}{{Perceived{\rm{\;}}contrast{\rm{\;}}with{\rm{\;}}no{\rm{\;}}surround}}\end{equation}
Testing took place in a quiet room within each school or university, away from distractions (e.g., no other participants or adults other than the experimenter present in the room). The experimenters (B.R., B.N.N.) ensured there was no glare on the screen and participants viewed the stimuli binocularly in a comfortable, seated position. Instructions were consistent for all participants, only modified for the local language where necessary to ensure comprehension. For training purposes, images of zebras were shown to demonstrate differences in contrast. The task was presented as a game where participants chose the moving zebra with higher contrast, while looking through a little circular window. Initially, to familiarize the participant with the forced choice task, the participant was only asked to view the stimuli and respond verbally. Once the experimenter was satisfied that the participant understood the task, a practice trial was conducted to familiarize participants with button pressing. There was unlimited time for button pressing, and the next trial did not begin until 500 ms after a button response was registered. Participants were closely monitored to maintain the viewing distance (40 cm), and no substantive head movements or obvious lapses in concentration (eye closure) were observed. Participants generally required no more than 10 minutes to complete both tasks, including training, practice runs, and rest breaks between runs. Tests were not repeated unless there was an initial procedural error (e.g., the participant reported pressing the wrong button to begin with, and asked to start again).
Statistical analyses were performed using IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 27.0 (IBM Corp., Armonk, NY). Data were tested for normality and homogeneity of variances using a Kolmogorov–Smirnov test and Levene's statistic, respectively. For our main comparison of perceived contrast with and without the surround across age groups, we used a repeated measures ANOVA with age as the between-group factor and surround condition as the within-group factor. To compare across age groups for single within-group factor analyses, a one-way ANOVA was conducted for normally distributed data or a Kruskal–Wallis nonparametric test when the assumption of normality was violated. Post hoc two-sided Bonferroni tests were conducted for pairwise comparisons, with adjusted significance (P) values for multiple comparisons. A P value of 0.05 was the criterion for statistical significance. Bland–Altman analyses were conducted to show test–retest variability between the two runs of each test condition.
Supported by an Australian Research Council Discovery Project grant (DP140100157) to A.M.M., a Melbourne School of Health Sciences Strategic International Research Seeding Grant to B.N.N., and community vision screening funds provided by the Elite School of Optometry, Medical Research Foundation, Chennai, India.
Disclosure: B.N. Nguyen, None; B. Ramakrishnan, None; A. Narayanan, None; J.R. Hussaindeen, None; A.M. McKendrick, None