Abstract
Purpose:
To investigate the effect of visual adaptation to orientation-dependent optical blur on meridional contrast sensitivity function in artificially imposed astigmatism.
Methods:
The study adopted a top-up adapt-test paradigm. During the blur adaptation process, the 18 non-astigmatic young adult participants were briefly presented with natural scene images (first trial, 10 minutes; subsequent trials, 6 seconds). Contrast sensitivities for horizontal and vertical gratings at spatial frequencies ranging from 1 to 8 cycles per degree (cpd) were measured immediately before and after adaptation to +3.00 diopters cylinder (DC) with-the-rule or against-the-rule astigmatism. Meridional anisotropy was measured to quantify the contrast sensitivity difference between the two grating orientations.
Results:
Adapting to astigmatic blur enhanced contrast sensitivity at the blurred power meridian but reduced contrast sensitivity at the least affected axis meridian. In with-the-rule conditions, contrast sensitivity for horizontal gratings was significantly increased, whereas that for vertical gratings was significantly decreased. Similarly, in against-the-rule conditions, contrast sensitivity for vertical gratings was significantly increased, whereas that for horizontal gratings was significantly decreased. These two factors together resulted in a substantial systematic reduction, averaging 34%, in meridional anisotropy of contrast sensitivity across the spatial frequency spectrum.
Conclusions:
Astigmatism adaptation occurs in natural scene viewing. Brief exposure to astigmatic blur altered contrast sensitivity in the opposite direction at the two principal meridians, indicating that the mature visual system possesses functional plasticity to recalibrate the response characteristics of orientationally tuned cortical filters and thus promote substantial reductions of meridional anisotropy in astigmatic vision, to some extent counterbalancing the elongated oval shape of astigmatic blur.
Astigmatism is an optical imperfection resulting from the difference in optical power between two perpendicular principal meridians. Approximately 20%
1–3 to 40%
4,5 of the population have at least 1 diopter (D) of refractive astigmatism. One distinct optical characteristic is that, unlike spherical refractive error producing non-directional blur, astigmatic refractive error produces orientation-dependent directional blur.
6 When a beam of light passes through an astigmatic surface, two perpendicular image foci are formed at separate image planes along the optical axis. Unless at the circle of least confusion (i.e., the dioptric midway of the two image foci) at which there is non-directional blur, astigmatism degrades the retinal image quality for one orientation more than others, leading to permanent meridional visual deficits if not corrected early in life.
7 Even in the absence of astigmatism in the central visual field, there is still remarkable off-axis astigmatism in the peripheral visual field of human eyes that increases dramatically with retinal eccentricity (eccentricity 30°: ∼3 D; eccentricity 60°: ∼9 D
8,9). Spectacle lenses, particularly progressive addition lenses for correcting presbyopia, also produce unwanted astigmatism away from the optical center, due to the large amounts of astigmatism in the periphery on both sides of the central progressive zone of the lens.
10
It is well known that optical defocus degrades the luminance contrast of retinal image contents, particularly at mid- and high spatial frequencies.
11 Interestingly, there is evidence showing that adaptation to blur can modify visual functions.
12 It is important that the testing procedures are not too lengthy; otherwise, most of the adaptation effect may have already decayed during the measurement. Surprisingly, later studies, using improved measurement strategies, demonstrated that contrast sensitivity was enhanced at a wide range of spatial frequencies following a brief period of adaptation to spherical defocus.
13,14 Rajeev and Metha,
13 using a top-up adaptation technique
15,16 that consisted of a longer initial adaptation to spherical defocus, reported that in each subsequent testing trial the initially adapted images were briefly presented again for topping up the visual effect of blur adaptation. Although contrast sensitivity was found to be decreased at very low spatial frequency (0.5 cpd), adaptation to blur boosted contrast sensitivity at mid-spatial frequencies (8 and 12 cycles per degree [cpd]). Similar results were reported by Venkataraman et al.,
14 who applied an efficient adaptive quick contrast sensitivity function technique, which allowed the entire contrast sensitivity function to be obtained in 3 minutes,
17 and they demonstrated that blur adaptation enhanced contrast sensitivity at low spatial frequencies (3–4 cpd).
The majority of previous adaptation studies focused on spherical defocus that blurred the retinal image equally across orientations. However, a related study revealed that adaptation to digitally simulated astigmatic blur can alter visual perception and induce a strong orientation bias in the subsequently viewed images,
18 thereby raising an interesting question of how the visual brain reacts when exposed to imposed astigmatism. In individuals with simple myopic astigmatism, with-the-rule (WTR) astigmatism diffuses the light in a vertical direction and blurs the horizontal contours, whereas against-the-rule (ATR) astigmatism diffuses in a horizontal direction instead and blurs the vertical contours. As astigmatism blurs the retinal image unequally across orientations, visual adaptation to directional blur in astigmatic defocus may adapt differently than non-directional blur in spherical defocus. We wondered whether orientation tuning can affect how the visual system responds when adapting to directional blur.
The experiments aimed to determine whether optically induced meridional anisotropy of contrast sensitivity could be, to a certain extent, compensated by neural adaptation to directional blur in astigmatism. A top-up adapt-test paradigm similar to that of previous studies
13,15,16 was employed to characterize the orientation specificity of contrast sensitivity adaptation to astigmatic blur. Specifically, the transient adaptational response of the meridional contrast sensitivity function to artificially imposed astigmatism was measured along the two principal meridians.
A psychophysical contrast sensitivity test was developed based on Psykinematix (KyberVision, Miyagi, Japan). A sinusoidal grating pattern (radius: 1.8°) was displayed in the center of the gamma-corrected liquid-crystal display (LCD) monitor screen (resolution: 1920 × 1080, refresh rate: 120 Hz, 10-bit RGB; Display++; Cambridge Research System, Rochester, UK). The background luminance was 50 cd/m2. To minimize any abrupt contrast cues, the grating edge was smoothed by a half-Gaussian ramp (σ = 0.2°) and the onset and offset of gratings by a temporal Gaussian envelope (σ = 50 ms). In each trial, a horizontal or vertical grating pattern was displayed for 500 ms. The observer’s task was to identify the grating orientation, horizontal or vertical, using a keyboard. No audio feedback to the observer's response was provided.
Supported by the Start-Up Fund (P0034097 to TL) and Undergraduate Research and Innovation Scheme (P0038345 to TL) with the Hong Kong Polytechnic University and InnoHK initiative with the Hong Kong Special Administrative Region Government; by the RPB Walt and Lilly Disney Award for Amblyopia Research, Research to Prevent Blindness (RWL); and by Nova Southeastern University research grants (PFRDG 334909 and HPD 334637 to RWL).
Disclosure: T.-W. Leung, None; R.W. Li, None; C.-S. Kee, None