We were interested in comparing binocular enhancement between our control and amblyopic groups. The magnitude of binocular enhancement was equated to the difference between the binocular reaction time and the fastest eye reaction time, which in fact corresponds with the latency of the fastest eye. This finding is presented for both groups in each contrast condition (
Fig. 4).
t Tests revealed that the magnitude of binocular enhancement was not meaningfully different from zero for any group or condition (all
P > 0.3,
Supplementary S7). Additionally, no differences between groups were found in the −1.0, 0.2, or 1.0 contrast conditions (all
P > 0.2, two-sample
t test) offering further support for a lack of a contrast effect. Owing to the widespread absence of a contrast effect for both amblyopes and controls, reaction time was averaged across contrast condition in the analyses presented in
Figures 5 and
7. Because there is some variability across participants as to which eye displays a longer latency, an investigation was performed to describe the pattern of latency for each participant. In
Figure 5, the latency of one eye, averaged across the three contrast conditions, is displayed as a function of the average latency of the other eye for all individual subjects. Differences in latencies producing a
P value of <0.05, as determined by bootstrapping and Wilcoxon signed-rank tests, are represented with a star symbol. For the controls, 3 participants displayed a higher latency for their nondominant eye and 10 did not show any difference (
Fig. 5a). A moderate correlation between the latency of the dominant eye and the latency of the nondominant eye was found (
R2 = 0.259;
P = 0.076), meaning that the latency of one eye was somewhat predictive of the latency of the other eye. For the amblyopes, 17 participants displayed a higher latency for the AE and 6 participants showed a higher latency for their FE (
Fig. 5b). Wilcoxon signed rank tests indicated that differences in latency were particularly large (
P < 0.05) for 14 subjects, 13 of which displayed a higher latency in their AE. As such, the latency in the AE was higher in the majority of cases. A two-tailed binomial test was computed for both groups with the null expectation being that exactly 50% of participants will fall above the identity line. This resulted in a
P value of 0.867 (
N = 13,
K = 5) for controls and a
P value of 0.005 (
N = 23,
K = 18) for amblyopes, indicating a greater temporal imbalance between the eyes in amblyopes compared with controls. Additionally, we observed good correlation between the latencies of the two eyes in controls (
R2 = 0.259;
P = 0.076), but not in amblyopes (
R2 = 0.052;
P = 0.298). Results of binomial tests and linear regressions performed on each individual clinical group are displayed in
Supplements S9 and
S10, respectively.